Headbob displays signal sex, social context and species identity in a Liolaemus lizard

in Amphibia-Reptilia
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Abstract

Animal communication has a key role in animals and identifying the signals’ function is crucial. Most lizards communicate with each other through visual signals with headbob displays, which are up-and-down movements of the head or the anterior part of the body. In the present work, I described and analysed the headbob displays of Liolaemus pacha lizards in their natural habitat. Specifically, the objectives were to describe the form of headbobs, to analyse their structure and to compare between sexes and social contexts. Adult lizards were video-recorded, registering the sex and the social context, classified as broadcast, same-sex and female-male interactions. The form and structure of sequences and headbobs were obtained. To evaluate the effect of sex and social context on the structure of headbob sequences and on headbob bouts, generalized linear mixed models were made. Intersexual differences were found in headbob display frequency and in the structure of headbob sequences. Lizards in same-sex context made sequences with more bouts, shorter intervals, headbob bouts of longer duration and higher amplitude than broadcast and female-male context. Presence of concurring behaviour such as lateral compression, gular expansion, and back arching occurred simultaneously with headbobs in same-sex context. Liolaemus pacha made four different headbob bout forms, and males were characterised by using bouts A and B, whereas females used bouts D more frequent. Sex and social context influenced only the structure of bouts A and B. The results showed that bouts A and B might be multi-component signals and non-redundant.

  • Supplementary data

Amphibia-Reptilia

Publication of the Societas Europaea Herpetologica

Sections

References

Baird, T.A. (2013): Male collared lizards, Crotaphytus collaris (Sauria: Crotaphytidae), signal females by broadcasting visual displays. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 108: 636-646.

Barlow, G.W. (1968): Ethological units of behaviour. In: Central Nervous System and Fish Behaviour, p. 217-232. Ingle, D., Ed., University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

Brandt, Y. (2003): Lizard threat display handicaps endurance. Proc. R. Soc. B 270: 1061-1068.

Briffa, M. (2014): Agonistic signals: integrating analysis of functions and mechanisms. In: Animal Signaling and Function: an Integrative Approach, p. 141-173. Irschick, D.J., Briffa, M., Podos, J., Eds, Wiley, Hoboken, NJ.

Brown, D. (2009): Computer program TRACKER: video analysis and modeling tool. Version 4.81.

Carpenter, C.C. (1961a): Patterns of social behavior in the desert iguana, Dipsosaurus dorsalis. Copeia 1961: 396-405.

Carpenter, C.C. (1961b): Patterns of social behavior in Merriam’s canyon lizard (Sceloporus m. merriami-Iguanidae). Southwest. Nat. 1961: 138-148.

Carpenter, C.C. (1967): Aggression and social structure of iguanid lizards. In: Lizard Ecology: a Symposium, p. 87-105. Milstead, W.W., Ed., University of Missouri Press, Columbia.

Carpenter, C.C. (1978): Ritualistic social behaviors in lizards. In: Behavior and Neurology of Lizards, p. 253-267. Greenberg, N., MacLean, P.D., Eds, NIMH, Bethesda, Maryland.

Carpenter, C.C. (1982): The aggressive displays of Iguaninae lizards. In: Iguanas of the World: Their Behavior, Ecology and Conservation, p. 215-231. Burghardt, G., Rand, A.S., Eds, Noyes Publ., Park Ridge, New Jersey.

Carpenter, C.C., Ferguson, G.W. (1977): Variation and evolution of stereotyped behaviour in reptiles. In: Biology of Reptilia: Ecology and Behaviour, Vol. 7, p. 335-554. Gans, C., Tinkle, D.W., Eds, Academic Press, New York.

Carpenter, C.C., Grubitz III, G. (1961): Time-motion study of a lizard. Ecology 42: 199-200.

Dawkins, R., Krebs, J.R. (1978): Animal signals: information or manipulation?. In: Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach, p. 282-309. Davies, J.R., Davies, N.B., Eds, Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.

Decourcy, K.R., Jenssen, T.A. (1994): Structure and use of male territorial headbob signals by the lizard Anolis carolinensis. Anim. Behav. 47: 251-262.

Font, E., Carazo, P., Pérez i de Lanuza, G., Kramer, M. (2012): Predator-elicited foot shakes in wall lizards (Podarcis muralis): evidence for a pursuit-deterrent function. J. Comp. Psychol. 126: 87-96.

García-Roa, R., Carreira, S., López, P., Martín, J. (2016): Genders matters: sexual differences in chemical signals of Liolaemus wiegmannii lizards (Iguania, Liolaemidae). Biochem. Syst. Ecol. 69: 108-114.

García-Roa, R., Jara, M., Baeckens, S., López, P., Van Damme, R., Martín, J., Pincheira-Donoso, D. (2017): Macroevolutionary diversification of glands for chemical communication in squamate reptiles. Sci. Rep.-UK 7: 9288.

Halloy, M. (1996): Behavioral patterns in Liolaemus quilmes (Tropiduridae), a South American lizard. Bull. Maryland Herp. Soc. 32: 43-57.

Halloy, M. (2012): Visual display variations in Neotropical lizards, Liolaemus quilmes (Iguania: Liolaemidae): relation to sex and season. Herpetol. J. 22: 265-268.

Halloy, M., Robles, C. (2002): Spatial distribution in a Neotropical lizard, Liolaemus quilmes (Liolaemidae): site fidelity and overlapping among males and females. Bull. Maryland Herp. Soc. 38: 118-129.

Halloy, M., Robles, C. (2003): Patrones de actividad y abundancias relativas en un lagarto del noroeste argentino, Liolaemus quilmes (Sauria: Liolaemidae). Cuad. Herpetol. 17: 67-73.

Halloy, M., Robles, C., Cuezzo, F. (2006): Diet in two syntopic Neotropical lizard species of Liolaemus (Liolaemidae): interspecific and intersexual differences. Rev. Esp. Herpetol. 20: 47-56.

Hebets, E., Papaj, D.R. (2005): Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 57: 197-214.

Hews, D.K., Date, P., Hara, E., Castellano, M.J. (2011): Field presentation of male secretions alters social display in Sceloporus virgatus but not in S. undulatus lizards. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65: 1403-1410.

Husak, J.F. (2004): Signal use by collared lizards, Crotaphytus collaris: the effects of familiarity and threat. Behav. Ecol: Sociobiol. 55: 602-607.

Jenssen, T.A., Orrell, K.S., Lovern, M.B. (2000): Sexual dimorphisms in aggressive signal structure and use by a polygynous lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Copeia 2000: 140-149.

Johnstone, R.A. (1996): Multiple displays in animal communication: ‘backup signals’ and ‘multiple messages’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc., B 351: 329-338.

Juárez Heredia, V., Robles, C., Halloy, M. (2013): A new species of Liolaemus from the darwinii group (Iguania: Liolaemidae), Tucumán province, Argentina. Zootaxa 3681: 524-538.

Labra, A. (2011): Chemical stimuli and species recognition in Liolaemus lizards. J. Zool. 285: 215-221.

Labra, A., Carazo, P., Desfilis, E., Font, E. (2007): Agonistic interactions in a Liolaemus lizard: structure of headbob displays. Herpetologica 63: 11-18.

Leal, M., Rodríguez-Robles, J.A. (1997): Signalling displays during predator-prey interactions in a Puerto Rican anole, Anolis cristatellus. Anim. Behav. 54: 1147-1154.

Ligon, R.A. (2014): Defeated chameleons darken dynamically during dyadic disputes to decrease danger from dominants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 68: 1007-1017.

Lovern, M.B., Jenssen, T.A. (2003): Form emergence and fixation of head bobbing displays in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis): a reptilian model of signal ontogeny. J. Comp. Psychol. 117: 133-141.

Macedonia, J.M., Clark, D.L. (2003): Headbob display structure in the naturalized Anolis lizards of Bermuda: sex, context, and population effects. J. Herpol. 37: 266-276.

Macedonia, J.M., Clark, D.L., Riley, R.G., Kemp, D.J. (2013): Species recognition of color and motion signals in Anolis grahami: evidence from responses to lizard robots. Behav. Ecol. 24: 846-852.

Macedonia, J.M., Clark, D.L., Cherry, L.E., Mohamed, N.E., Bartel, B.W. (2015): Comparison of headbob displays in gray-dewlapped and red-dewlapped populations of Green Anoles (Anolis carolinensis). Herpetologica 71: 117-124.

Martin, P., Bateson, P. (2007): Measuring Behaviour. an Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Martín, J., López, P. (2015): Condition-dependent chemosignals in reproductive behavior of lizards. Horm. Behav. 68: 14-24.

Martins, E.P. (1991): Individual and sex differences in the use of push-up display by the sagebrush lizard, Sceloporus graciosus. Anim. Behav. 41: 403-416.

Martins, E.P. (1993): Contextual use of the push-up display by the sagebrush lizard, Sceloporus graciosus. Anim. Behav. 45: 25-36.

Martins, E.P., Bissell, A.N., Morgan, K.K. (1998): Population differences in a lizard communicative display: evidence for rapid change in structure and function. Anim. Behav. 56: 1113-1119.

Martins, E.P., Labra, A., Halloy, M., Thompson, J.T. (2004): Large-scale patterns of signal evolution: an interspecific study of Liolaemus lizard headbob displays. Anim. Behav. 68: 453-463.

Martins, E.P., Ossip-Klein, A.G., Zúñiga-Vega, J.J., García, C.V., Campos, S.M., Hews, D.K. (2015): Evolving from static to dynamic signals: evolutionary compensation between two communicative signals. Anim. Behav. 102: 223-229.

Ord, T.J., Stamps, J.A. (2009): Species identity cues in animal communication. Am. Nat. 174: 585-593.

Ord, T.J., Blumstein, D.T., Evans, C.S. (2001): Intrasexual selection predicts the evolution of signal complexity in lizards. Proc. R. Soc. B 268: 737-744.

Osborne, L., Umbers, K.D.L., Keogh, J.S. (2013): The effects of perch height, time in residence and distance from opponent on aggressive display in male lizards. Acta Ethol. 16: 41-46.

Ossip-Drahos, A.G., Morales, J.R.O., Vital-García, C., Zúñiga-Vega, J.J., Hews, D.K., Martins, E.P. (2016): Shaping communicative colour signals over evolutionary time. R. Soc. open sci. 3: 160728.

Ossip-Klein, A.G., Fuentes, J.A., Hews, D.K., Martins, E.P. (2013): Information content is more important than sensory system or physical distance in guiding the long-term evolutionary relationships between signaling modalities in Sceloporus lizards. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67: 1513-1522.

Partan, S.R., Marler, P. (2005): Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. Am. Nat. 166: 231-245.

Pérez i de Lanuza, G., Carazo, P., Font, E. (2014): Colours of quality: structural (but not pigment) coloration informs about male quality in a polychromatic lizard. Anim. Behav. 90: 73-81.

Pruett, J.A., Zúñiga-Vega, J.J., Campos, S.M., Soini, H.A., Novotny, M.V., Vital-García, C., Martins, E.P., Hews, D.K. (2016): Evolutionary interactions between visual and chemical signals: chemosignals compensate for the loss of a visual signal in male Sceloporus lizards. J. Chem. Ecol. 42: 1164-1174.

R Core Team (2015): R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. https://cran.r-project.org/.

Radder, R.S., Saidapur, S.K., Shine, R., Shanbhag, B.A. (2006): The language of lizards: interpreting the function of visual displays of the Indian rock lizard, Psammophilus dorsalis (Agamidae). J. Ethol. 24: 275-283.

Ramírez Pinilla, M.P. (1992): Ciclos reproductivos y de cuerpos grasos en dos poblaciones de Liolaemus darwinii (Reptilia: Sauria: Tropiduridae). Acta Zool. Lill. 42: 41-49.

Robles, C., Halloy, M. (2009): Home ranges and reproductive strategies in a Neotropical lizard, Liolaemus quilmes (Iguania: Liolaemidae). South Am. J. Herp. 4: 253-258.

Robles, C.I., Halloy, M. (2010): Core area overlap in a Neotropical lizard, Liolaemus quilmes: relationship with territoriality and reproductive strategy. Herp. J. 20: 243-248.

Roggenbuck, M.E., Jenssen, T.A. (1986): The ontogeny of display behaviour in Sceloporus undulatus (Sauria: Iguanidae). Ethology 71: 153-165.

Rothblum, L., Jenssen, T.A. (1978): Display repertoire analysis of Sceloporus undulatus hyacinthinus (Sauria: Iguanidae) from south-western Virginia. Anim. Behav. 26: 130-137.

Stuart-Fox, D., Goode, J.L. (2014): Female ornamentation influences male courtship investment in a lizard. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2: 1-9.

Uetz, P., Freed, P., Hošek, J. (2016): The Reptile Database. http://www.reptile-database.org. Accessed 25th August, 2017.

Van Dyk, D.A., Evans, C.S. (2008): Opponent assessment in lizards: examining the effect of aggressive and submissive signals. Behav. Ecol. 19: 895-901.

Vicente, N.S., Halloy, M. (2014): Liolaemus pacha. Diet. Herpetol. Rev. 45: 697.

Vicente, N.S., Halloy, M. (2015): Male headbob display structure in a Neotropical lizard, Liolaemus pacha (Iguania: Liolaemidae): relation to social context. Herpetol. J. 25: 49-53.

Vicente, N.S., Halloy, M. (2016a): Liolaemus ramirezae. Headbob display behavior. Herpetol. Rev. 47: 465-466.

Vicente, N.S., Halloy, M. (2016b): Chemical recognition of conspecifics in a Neotropical lizard, Liolaemus pacha (Iguania: Liolaemidae): relation to visual displays, season and sex. J. Ethol. 34: 329-335.

Vicente, N.S., Halloy, M. (2017): Interaction between visual and chemical cues in a Liolaemus lizard: a multimodal approach. Zoology 125: 24-28.

Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N., Walker, N.J., Saveliev, A.A., Smith, G.M. (2009): Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology With R. Springer-Verlag, New York.

Figures

  • Same-sex context: video captures showing different males which in presence of other males, performed headbob displays with back arching, lateral compression, and gular expansion (A-B). Female-male context: a male approaches a female by displaying headbobs, with a low posture (C and D). When the male is near to the female, he can press with his snout the female cloacal area (E). If female rejects him, she raises in her four limbs fully stretched, inflates her body, raises her tail and moves away hopping from the side (F). Broadcast context: Headbob displays can be produced in absence of conspecifics, with a resting posture, characterized by hind limbs flexed and forelimbs going from stretched to flexed. Male (G) and female (H) L. pacha.

    View in gallery
  • Analysis of headbob display structure. A) Headbob sequences, showing the initial level (y0) for each headbob bout, the intervals, and bout duration. From each sequence, the mean interval, the mean headbob bout duration and the number of bouts per sequence were obtained. B) Headbob bout, indicating the units that comprise: U1, U2, U3. From each bout, the amplitude and duration of each unit were also measured.

    View in gallery
  • Analyses of headbob sequences. Parameter estimates (PE ± SE), 95% confidence interval limits (CL) for explanatory variables (sex and/or context) describing variation in mean bout duration, the number of bouts per sequence, the presence of concurring behaviours (gular inflation, lateral compression and, back arching) and, mean interval. Estimates represent the difference between sexes and contexts, with respect to the reference values (females and same-sex context), when correspond.

    View in gallery
  • Effect of sex and social context in mean headbob bout duration, the number of bouts per sequence, and mean interval, showing standard errors. Means with the same letter are not significantly different (P>0.05).

    View in gallery
  • Liolaemus pacha headbob bouts forms: type A (top left), type B (bottom left), type C (top right) and type D (bottom left).

    View in gallery
  • Analyses of headbob bouts A and B. Parameter estimates (PE ± SE), and P-value of explanatory variables (sex and context), describing variation in amplitude and duration of units of headbob bouts A and B. Estimate values represent the difference between sexes and contexts, with respect to the reference values (females and same-sex context), when correspond.

    View in gallery
  • Amplitude (mm) and duration (s) of the headbob bout types of male and female L. pacha, showing n, mean values ± SE and, CV% of each unit. Bold values of each unit are significantly different between sexes (P0.05).

    View in gallery
  • Amplitude (mm) and duration (s) of the headbob bout types among different social contexts, showing n, mean values ± SE and, CV% of each unit. Bold values of each unit are significantly different between contexts (P0.05).

    View in gallery

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 20 20 19
Full Text Views 6 6 5
PDF Downloads 2 2 1
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0