The Synoptic Art Experience

in Art & Perception
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

At the start of the 20th century, Moritz von Rohr invented the synopter: a device that removes 3D depth cues that arise from binocular disparities and vergence. In the absence of these visual cues, the observer is less aware of the physical flatness of the picture. This results in a surprisingly increased depth impression of pictorial space, historically known as the ‘plastic effect’. In this paper we present a practical design to produce a synopter and explore which elements of a painting influence the plastic effect. In the first experiment we showed 22 different paintings to a total of 35 observers, and found that they rate the synoptic effect rather consistent over the various paintings. Subsequent analyses indicated that at least three pictorial cues were relevant for the synoptic effect: figure–ground contrast, compositional depth and shadows. In experiment 2, we used manipulated pictures where we tried to strengthen or weaken these cues. In all three cases we found at least one effect that confirmed our hypothesis. We also found substantial individual differences: some observers experience little effect, while others are very surprised by the effect. A stereo acuity test revealed that these differences could not be attributed to how well disparities are detected. Lastly, we informally tested our newly designed synopter in museums and found similar idiosyncratic appraisal. But the device also turned out to facilitate discussions among visitors.

The Synoptic Art Experience

in Art & Perception

Sections

References

AmesA. (1925). The illusion of depth from single pictures. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 10137148.

BlackR. (2006). UK Patent application GB2487358A.

BlakeE. C. (2003). Zograscopes, Virtual Reality, and the Mapping of Polite Society in Eighteenth-Century England in: New Media 1740–1915GitelmanL. and PingreeG. (Eds) pp. 129MIT PressCambridge, MA, USA.

BrainardD. H. (1997). The psychophysics Ttoolbox. Spat. Vis. 10433436.

CiuffredaK. and EngberK. (2002). Is one eye better than two when viewing pictorial art?Leonardo 353740.

ClaparèdeE. (1904). Stéréoscopie monoculaire paradoxale. Ann. Ocul. 132465466.

Da VinciL. (1888). The Notebooks of Leonardo Da Vinci (J. P. Richter transl.). Retrieved from http://www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/5000.

EatonE. M. (1919). The visual perception of solid form. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 3399408.

EbbinghausH. (1902). Grundzüge der Psychologie Verlag von Veit and Comp. Leipzig, Germany pp. 424425.

EbyD. W. and BraunsteinM. L. (1995). The perceptual flattening of three-dimensional scenes enclosed by a frame. Perception 24981993.

HeineO. (2009). Bildschirm-Betrachtungsvorrichtung für ein Bildschirmgerät German Patent DE 202009002362 U1.

HennessyR. T.IidaT.ShiinaK. and LeibowitzH. W. (1976). The effect of pupil size on accommodation. Vis. Res. 16587589.

HigashiyamaA. and ShimonoK. (2012). Apparent depth of pictures reflected by a mirror: The plastic effect. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 7415221532.

HillH. H. (1898). Improved Graphoscope for Obtaining Stereoscopic Effects UK Patent 189815299–A.

HochbergJ. (1962). The psychophysics of pictorial perception. AV. Commun. Rev.102254.

HoltE. B. (1904). Reviews of: Die von M. von Rohr gegebene Theorie des Veranten, eines Apparats zur Richtigen Betrachtung von Photographien, E. Wandersleb; The Verant, a New Instrument for Viewing Photographs from the Correct Standpoint, M. von Rohr; Der Verant, ein Apparat zum Betrachten von Photogrammen in richtigen Abstande, A Köhler. J. Philos. Psychol. Sci. Meth. 1552553.

KaldenbachC. (1985). Optica prints. Print Q. 287104.

KempM. (1990). The Science of Art: Optical Themes in Western Art from Brunelleschi to SeuratYale University PressNew Haven, CT, USA.

KleinerM.BrainardD. and PelliD. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception36 Suppl. 14 (ECVP Abstract Suppl.).

KoenderinkJ.Van DoornA. and KappersA. M. L. (1994). On so-called paradoxical monocular stereoscopyPerception23583594.

KoenderinkJ.WijntjesM. and Van DoornA. (2013). Zograscopic viewing. i-Perception4192206.

LeveltW. (1966). The alternation process in binocular rivalry. Br. J. Psychol. 57225238.

LivingstoneM. (2002). Vision and Art: The Biology of Seeing Vol. 2Harry N. Abrams, New York, NY, USA.

PelliD. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10433436.

SchlosbergH. (1941). Stereoscopic depth from single pictures. Am. J. Psychol. 54601605.

VishwanathD. (2014). Toward a new theory of stereopsis. Psychol. Rev. 12115178.

VishwanathD. and HibbardP. B. (2013). Seeing in 3-d with just one eye: Stereopsis without binocular vision. Psychol. Sci. 24167385.

Von HelmholtzH. (1924). Treatise on Physiological OpticsCourier Dover Publications, New York.

Von RohrM. (1907). Reflecting Intruments Annulling the Perception of Depth in Binocular Vision UK Patent 190700916-A.

WestheimerG. and McKeeS. P. (1980). Stereoscopic acuity with defocused and spatially filtered retinal images. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 70772778.

WijntjesM. (2014). A new view through alberti’s window. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40488.

ZothO. (1916). Ein einfaches “Plastoskop”. Zeitschr. Sinnesphysiol. 498588.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Overview of viewing devices that enhance pictorial depth. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Original images from Von Rohrs’ patent claim. We have reshuffled the figures to match the order of Fig. 3, which uses a similar labelling A–C. The labelling ‘Fig. 4’ etc., refers to the original patent. Von Rohr drew the basic optics of the three designs in the left column. The drawings in the middle and right column are practical implementations of these three types.

  • View in gallery

    Overview of three different synopter designs by von Rohr. We recalculated the optics and found for a fixed distance of 20 mm to the closest mirror three different fields of view, ranging from 21° to 35.3°.

  • View in gallery

    Transmission spectrum of a perspex half mirror. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Left: the setup we used to make the photographs through both peepholes of the synopter. The camera was on a slider, so two parallel pictures were made with a 6.5 cm lateral translation difference. Right: right–left image pairs are shown. The placement of the right picture on the left side and vice versa was on purpose, to allow visual cross fusion for the trained reader. A) pictures through the synopter without the giraffe object. The images differ in brightness and colour, as predicted by the mirror characteristics. B) Same images but now with a toy giraffe in front to validate that there is no parallax between the two eyes. C) Images taken without the synopter. The relative position of the giraffe in the right eye image (shown left) remains unchanged, but in the left eye a parallactic displacement is visible. Cross fusion will reveal a clear binocular stereoscopic impression. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    The left column shows renderings of the model, including an ‘exploded view’. The middle and right columns show photographs of the synopter used in this research. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Results of group 1 (N = 15) shown in a box whisker plot. The grey bar denotes the second and third quartiles (i.e. 50% of the responses); the ‘whiskers’ denote the maximum scores. The black lines indicate the median values and the white lines the mean values. The paintings were ordered with respect to the means (white lines). Each painting is shown in thumbnail format at the specific data points. The dashed boxes denote paintings that were also measured in the other group, which can be used as intergroup comparison. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Results of group 2 (N = 20) shown in a box whisker plot. For an explanation see caption of figure 7. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Annotations of observers indicating in which parts of the painting the effect was particularly strong in painting set 2. The images are ordered with respect to synoptic effect (as in Fig. 8) and N denotes number of annotations made by observers. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Details of the dashed squared areas shown in Fig. 9. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Image manipulations to strengthen (7, 20, 4, 1, 22, 9, 21) or weaken (14, 6, 12, 15, 11) the plastic effect. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

  • View in gallery

    Relation between stereo acuity and overall rating of the plastic effect. Filled circles denote participants that correlated negatively with the group means and were discarded in the further analysis.

  • View in gallery

    Overview of the effect of various image manipulations on the synoptic experience. Left bars denote the original stimulus, right bars the manipulated versions. The plus and minus signs indicate the expected difference. Originals are shown in thumbnails, the edited version can be seen in Fig. 11. Significant differences are marked by an asterisk. This figure is published in colour in the online version.

Index Card

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 41 41 18
Full Text Views 35 35 25
PDF Downloads 9 9 7
EPUB Downloads 9 9 6