Documenting orang-utan sleep architecture: sleeping platform complexity increases sleep quality in captive Pongo

in Behaviour
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Of the extant primates, only 20 non-human species have been studied by sleep scientists. Notable sampling gaps exist, including large-bodied hominoids such as gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), orang-utans (Pongo spp.) and bonobos (Pan paniscus), for which data have been characterized as high priority. Here, we report the sleep architecture of three female and two male orang-utans housed at the Indianapolis Zoo. Sleep states were identified by scoring correlated behavioural signatures (e.g., respiration, gross body movement, muscle atonia, random eye movement, etc.). The captive orang-utans were focal subjects for a total of 70 nights (1013 h) recorded. We found that orang-utans slept an average of 9.11 h (range 5.85–11.2 h) nightly and were characterized by an average NREM of 8.03 h (range 5.47–10.2 h) and REM of 1.11 (range: 0.38–2.2 h) per night. In addition, using a sleeping platform complexity index (SPCI) we found that individuals that manufactured and slept in more complex beds were characterized by higher quality sleep. Sleep fragmentation (the number of brief awakenings greater than 2 min per hour), arousability (number of motor activity bouts per hour), and total time awake per night were reduced by greater quality sleep environments. Therefore, comfortable sleeping environments reduced arousability and improve sleep quality in captive orang-utans. These results support Fruth & Hohmann’s (1996) hypothesis, which purported that the tree-limb-to-sleeping platform transition in Miocene apes improved sleep quality.

Documenting orang-utan sleep architecture: sleeping platform complexity increases sleep quality in captive Pongo

in Behaviour

Sections

References

AltmannJ. (1974). Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. — Behaviour 49: 227-266.

Ancoli-IsraelS.ColeR.AlessiC.ChambersM.MoorcroftW.PollakC.P. (2003). The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. — Sleep 26: 342-392.

AndersonJ.R. (1998). Sleep, sleeping sites, and sleep-related activities: awakening to their signifiance. — Am. J. Primatol. 46: 63-75.

BaldwinP.J.Sabater PiJ.McGrewW.C.TutinC.E.G. (1981). Comparisons of nests made by different populations of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Primates 22: 474-486.

BalzamoE.BradleyR.J.RhodesJ.M. (1972). Sleep ontogeny in the chimpanzee: from two months to forty-one months. — Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurol. 33: 47-60.

BalzamoE.Van BeersP.LagardeD. (1998). Scoring of sleep and wakefulness by behavioral analysis from video recordings in rhesus monkeys: comparison with conventional EEG analysis. — Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurol. 106: 206-212.

CampbellS.S.ToblerI. (1984). Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across phylogeny. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8: 269-300.

CapelliniI.McNamaraP.PrestonB.T.NunnC.L.BartonR.A. (2009). Does sleep play a role in memory consolidation? A comparative test. — PLoS ONE 4: e4609.

FreemonF.R.McNewJ.J.AdeyW.R. (1969). Sleep of unrestrained chimpanzee — cortical and subcortical recordings. — Exp. Neurol. 25: 129-137.

FruthB.HohmannG. (1996). Nest building behaviour in the great apes: the great leap forward? — In: Great ape societies ( MarchantL.F.NishidaT. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  225-240.

GaisS.PilihalW.WagnerU.BornJ. (2000). Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. — Nature Neurosci. 3: 1335-1339.

GoodallJ.M. (1962). Nest building behavior in the free ranging chimpanzee. — Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 102: 455-467.

GoodallJ.M. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. — Anim. Behav. Monogr. 1: 161-311.

GrovesC.P. (2001). Primate taxonomy. — Smithsonian Institution PressWashington, DC.

HealeyJ.F. (2009). Statistics: a tool for social research8th edn. — Thomson/WadsworthBelmont, CA.

HobsonJ.A.Pace-SchottE.F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. — Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3: 679-693.

KarniA.SagiD. (1991). Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 88: 4966-4970.

KavanauJ.L. (1994). Sleep and dynamic stabilization of neural circuitry — a review and synthesis. — Behav. Brain Res. 63: 111-126.

KavanauJ.L. (1997). Origin and evolution of sleep: roles of vision and endothermy. — Brain Res. Bull. 42: 245-264.

KripkeD.F.BertJ. (1968). Techniques for telemetry of chimpanzee sleep electroencephalogram. — J. Appl. Physiol. 25: 461-462.

KripkeD.F.ReiteM.L.PegramG.V.StephensL.M.LewisO.F. (1968). Nocturnal sleep in Rhesus monkeys. — Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurol. 24: 581-586.

KruegerJ.M.RectorD.M.RoyS.Van DongenH.P.A.BelenkyG.PankseppJ. (2008). Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. — Nature Rev. Neurosci. 9: 910-919.

LeskuJ.A.RothT.C.RattenborgN.C.AmlanerC.J.LimaS.L. (2009). History and future of comparative analyses in sleep research. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33: 1024-1036.

Martin-OrdasG.CallJ. (2011). Memory processing in great apes: the effect of time and sleep. — Biol. Lett. 7: 829-832.

McCallC.McCallW.V. (2012). Comparison of actigraphy with polysomnography and sleep logs in depressed insomniacs. — J. Sleep Res. 21: 122-127.

McNamaraP.CapelliniI.HarrisE.NunnC.L.BartonR.A.PrestonB.T. (2008). The phylogeny of sleep database: a new resource for sleep scientists. — Open Sleep J. 1: 11-14.

McNamaraP.BartonR.A.NunnC.L. (2010). Evolution of sleep: phylogenetic and functional perspectives. — Cambridge University PressCambridge.

McNewJ.J. (1972). Sleep-wake cycle of an unrestrained isolated chimpanzee under entrained and free running conditions. — Aerospace Med. 43: 155.

McNewJ.J.HoweR.C.RossA.W. (1971). The sleep cycle and subcortical-cortical EEG relations in the unrestrained chimpanzee. — Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurol. 30: 489-503.

MizunoY.TakeshitaH.MatsuzawaT. (2006). Behavior of infant chimpanzees during the night in the first 4 months of life: smiling and suckling in relation to behavioral state. — Infancy 9: 215-324.

MorimuraN.FujisawaM.MoriY.TeramotoM. (2012). Environmental influences on sleep behavior in captive male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Int. J. Primatol. 33: 822-829.

MunozdelgadoJ. (1995). Behavioral characterization of sleep in stumptail Macaques (Macaca artoides) in exterior captivity by means of high-sensitivity videorecording. — Am. J. Primatol. 36: 245-249.

NunnC.L.McNamaraP.CapelliniI.PrestonB.T.BartonR.A. (2010). Primate sleep in phylogenetic perspective. — In: Evolution and sleep: phylogenetic and functional perspectives ( McNamaraP.BartonR.A.NunnC.L. eds). Cambridge University PressNew York, NY p.  123-144.

PeigneuxP.LaureysS.DelbeuckX.MaquetP. (2001). Sleeping brain, learning brain: the role of sleep for memory systems. — NeuroReport 12: A111-A124.

PrestonB.CapelliniI.McNamaraP.BartonR.A.NunnC. (2009). Parasite resistance and the adaptive significance of sleep. — BMC Evol. Biol. 9: 7.

ReimaoR.SouzaJ.C.GaudiosoC.E.V.GuerraH.D.AlvesA.D.OliveiraJ.C.F.SilverioD.C.G. (2000). Nocturnal sleep pattern in native Brazilian Terena adults. — Arq. Neuro-Psiquiatr. 58: 233-238.

SamsonD.R. (2012). The chimpanzee nest quantified: morphology and ecology of arboreal sleeping platforms within the dry habitat site of Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. — Primates 53: 357-364.

ShumakerR.W.WichS.A.PerkinsL. (2008). Reproductive life history traits of female orangutans (Pongo spp.). — In: Primate reproductive aging: cross-taxon perspectives ( AtsalisS.MargulisS. eds). KargerBasel p. 147-161.

ShumakerR.W.WalkupK.R.BeckB.B. (2011). Animal tool behavior: the use and manufacture of tools by animals. — Johns Hopkins University PressBaltimore, MD.

SiegelJ.M. (2008). Do all animals sleep?Trends Neurosci. 31: 208-213.

SiegmundR.TittelM.SchiefenhovelW. (1998). Activity monitoring of the inhabitants in Tauwema, a traditional melanesian village: rest/activity behaviour of Trobriand islanders (Papua New Guinea). — Biol. Rhythm Res. 29: 49-59.

Sri KanthaS.SuzukiJ. (2006). Sleep quantitation in common marmoset, cotton top tamarin and squirrel monkey by non-invasive actigraphy. — Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 144: 203-210.

ToblerI. (2005). Phylogeny of sleep regulation. — In: Principles and practice of sleep medicine ( KrygerM.H.RothT.DementW.C. eds). Elsevier/SaundersPhiladephia, PA p.  77-91.

VideanE.N. (2006). Sleep in captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): the effects of individual and environmental factors on sleep duration and quality. — Behav. Brain Res. 169: 187-192.

WalkerM.P. (2009). The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. — Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1156: 168-197.

WalkerM.P.StickgoldR. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. — Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57: 139-166.

WeitzmanE.D. (1965). Cyclic activity in Sleep of Macaca mulatta. — Arch. Neurol. 12: 463.

WorthmanC.M. (ed.) (2008). After dark: the evolutionary ecology of human sleep. — Oxford University PressOxford.

WorthmanC.M.MelbyM.K. (eds) (2002). Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep. — Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  69-117.

ZepelinH. (ed.) (1989). Mammalian sleep. — W.B. SaundersPhiladelphia, PA.

ZepelinH.SiegelJ.M.ToblerI. (2005). Mammalian sleep. — In: Principles and practice of sleep medicine ( KrygerM.H.RothT.DementW.C. eds). Elsevier/SaundersPhiladephia, PA p.  91-100.

ZhdanovaI.V.GeigerD.A.SchwagerlA.L.LeclairO.U.KillianyR.TaylorJ.A.MadrasB.K. (2002). Melatonin promotes sleep in three species of diurnal nonhuman primates. — Physiol. Behav. 75: 523-529.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Infrared videography was used to monitor and score sleep states (left: Azy sleeping in the prone position; right: Lucy sleeping on her right side).

  • View in gallery

    Flow chart illustrating the behavioural criteria used in assessment of sleep and awake states. In large-bodied great apes, respiration is of primary importance given its identifiability.

  • View in gallery

    Sleep architecture categorized by individuals (N=5).

  • View in gallery

    There is a significant negative linear correlation between the sleeping platform complexity index (SPCI) and sleep fragmentation (the number of awakenings per hour during a sleep session; individuals N=5, nights N=61). In addition, there is a significant negative linear correlation between SPCI and arousability (the number of subject movements exhibited per hour during a sleep session; N=5, nights N=61).

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 25 25 11
Full Text Views 35 35 31
PDF Downloads 5 5 3
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0