Early recognition and response to predator, heterospecific, and conspecific visual cues by multiple species of poeciliid fry

in Behaviour
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Recognition and response to cues from conspecific and heterospecific individuals is critical to survival and reproduction and therefore should be learned early in life. Young fry of two live-bearing fishes, Poecilia mexicana and Xiphophorus birchmanni, were exposed to visual cues of three different species in order to measure shoal tightening, an antipredator defence. Both species responded to a putative predator with shoal tightening, but response to a con- and hetero-specific poeciliid did not differ, though inter-fry distances became larger in the presence of both species compared to distances prior to exposure. There were also size-dependent effects on the pairwise distance between fry. These results suggest that fry are able to learn to distinguish among species based on visual cues alone at an early age, that A. mexicanus is perceived as a threat, but that adult poeciliids are likely not perceived as a predator by fry as some studies have posited.

Early recognition and response to predator, heterospecific, and conspecific visual cues by multiple species of poeciliid fry

in Behaviour

Sections

References

AbramoffM.D.MagalhaesP.J.RamS.J. (2004). Image processing with ImageJ. — Biophot. Int. 11: 36-43.

AbramsP.A.RoweL. (1996). The effects of predation on the age and size of maturity of prey. — Evolution 50: 1052-1061.

AgorretaA.Domínguez-DomínguezO.ReinaR.G.MirandaR.BerminghamE.DoadrioI. (2013). Phylogenetic relationships and biogeography of Pseudoxiphophorus (Teleostei: Poeciliidae) based on mitochondrial and nuclear genes. — Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 66: 80-90.

BasoloA.L. (2006). Genetic linkage and color polymorphism in the southern platyfish (Xiphophorus maculatus): a model system for studies of color pattern evolution. — Zebrafish 3: 65-83.

BerglundA. (1993). Risky sex: male pipefishes mate at random in the presence of a predator. — Anim. Behav. 46: 169-175.

BrodieE.D.III (1992). Correlational selection for color pattern and antipredator behavior in the garter snake Thamnophis ordinoides. — Evolution 46: 1284-1298.

BuckelJ.A.ConoverD.O.SteinbergN.D.McKownK.A. (1999). Impact of age-0 bluefish (Pomatomus saltatrix) predation on age-0 fishes in the Hudson River estuary: evidence for density-dependent loss of juvenile striped bass (Morone saxatilis). — Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 56: 275-287.

ChiversD.P.MirzaR.S.BryerP.J.KieseckerJ.M. (2001). Threat-sensitive predator avoidance by slimy sculpins: understanding the importance of visual versus chemical information. — Can. J. Zool. 79: 867-873.

ColemanS.W.RosenthalG.G. (2006). Swordtail fry attend to chemical and visual cues in detecting predators and conspecifics. — PloS One 1: e118.

CrowlT.A.CovichA.P. (1990). Predator-induced life-history shifts in a freshwater snail. — Science 247: 949-951.

CulumberZ.W.KeithC.SchumerM.MonksS.ToblerM. (2015). Environmental heterogeneity generates opposite gene-by-environment interactions for two fitness-related traits within a population. — Evolution 69: 541-550.

DixsonD.L.MundayP.L.JonesG.P. (2010). Ocean acidification disrupts the innate ability of fish to detect predator olfactory cues. — Ecol. Lett. 13: 68-75.

EvansJ.P.PilastroA.SchluppI. (2011). Ecology and evolution of poeciliid fishes. — University of Chicago PressChicago, IL.

FitzGibbonC.D. (1990). Mixed-species grouping in Thomson’s and Grant’s gazelles: the antipredator benefits. — Anim. Behav. 39: 1116-1126.

HoareD.J.KrauseJ.PeuhkuriN.GodinJ.-G.J. (2000). Body size and shoaling in fish. — J. Fish Biol. 57: 1351-1366.

KatsL.B.DillL.M. (1998). The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. — Ecoscience 5: 361-394.

KelleyJ.L.MagurranA.E. (2003). Learned predator recognition and antipredator responses in fishes. — Fish Fish. 4: 216-226.

KrauseJ.GodinJ.-G.J. (1994). Shoal choice in the banded killifish (Fundulus diaphanus, Teleostei, Cyprinodontidae): effects of predation risk, fish size, species composition and size of shoals. — Ethology 98: 128-136.

KrauseJ.LoaderS.P.McDermottJ.RuxtonG.D. (1998). Refuge use by fish as a function of body length-related metabolic expenditure and predation risks. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 265: 2373-2379.

LangerhansR.B.LaymanC.A.ShokrollahiA.DeWittT.J. (2004). Predator-driven phenotypic diversification in Gambusia affinis. — Evolution 58: 2305-2318.

LimaN.R.W.VrijenhoekR.C. (1996). Avoidance of filial cannibalism by sexual and clonal forms of Poeciliopsis (Pisces: Poeciliidae). — Anim. Behav. 51: 293-301.

LimaS.L.DillL.M. (1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. — Can. J. Zool. 68: 619-640.

MeffeG.K.CrumpM.L. (1987). Possible growth and reproductive benefits of cannibalism in the mosquitofish. — Am. Nat. 129: 203-212.

MeffeG.K.SnelsonF.F. (1989). An ecological overview of poeciliid fishes. — In: Ecology and evolution of livebearing fishes (Poeciliidae). Prentice HallEnglewood Cliffs, NJ p. 13-31.

MikheevV.N.WanzenböckJ.PasternakA.F. (2006). Effects of predator-induced visual and olfactory cues on 0+ perch (Perca fluviatilis L.) foraging behaviour. — Ecol. Freshw. Fish 15: 111-117.

MillerR.R.MinckleyW.L.NorrisS.M.GachM.H. (2005). Freshwater fishes of Mexico. — University of Chicago PressChicago, IL.

MundayP.L.DixsonD.L.DonelsonJ.M.JonesG.P.PratchettM.S.DevitsinaG.V.DãvingK.B. (2009). Ocean acidification impairs olfactory discrimination and homing ability of a marine fish. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106: 1848-1852.

NesbitD.H.MeffeG.K. (1993). Cannibalism frequencies in wild populations of the eastern mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki: Poeciliidae) in South Carolina. — Copeia: 867-870.

PeresC.A. (1993). Anti-predation benefits in a mixed-species group of Amazonian tamarins. — Folia Primatol. 61: 61-76.

PeuhkuriN. (1997). Size-assortative shoaling in fish: the effect of oddity on foraging behaviour. — Anim. Behav. 54: 271-278.

ReznickD.A.BrygaH.EndlerJ.A. (1990). Experimentally induced life-history evolution in a natural population. — Nature 346: 357-359.

RobertsG. (1996). Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. — Anim. Behav. 51: 1077-1086.

RosenthalG.G. (2000). The behavioral ecology of visual signaling in swordtails. — PhD thesis University of Texas Austin TX.

RosenthalG.G.MartinezT.Y.F.de LeonF.J.G.RyanM.J. (2001). Shared preferences by predators and females for male ornaments in swordtails. — Am. Nat. 158: 146-154.

ShierD.M.OwingsD.H. (2007). Effects of social learning on predator training and postrelease survival in juvenile black-tailed prairie dogs, Cynomys ludovicianus. — Anim. Behav. 73: 567-577.

SmithM.E.BelkM.C. (2001). Risk assessment in western mosquitofish (Gambusia affinis): do multiple cues have additive effects?Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51: 101-107.

SogardS.M. (1997). Size-selective mortality in the juvenile stage of teleost fishes: a review. — Bull. Mar. Sci. 60: 1129-1157.

SuhonenJ. (1993). Predation risk influences the use of foraging sites by tits. — Ecology 74: 1197-1203.

SullivanK.A. (1989). Predation and starvation: age-specific mortality in juvenile juncos (Junco phaenotus). — J. Anim. Ecol. 58: 275-286.

TamaruC.S.ColeB.BaileyR.BrownC.AkoH. (2001). A manual for commercial production of the swordtail Xiphpophorus helleri. — CTSA Publication Number 128 Texas State University San Marcos TX.

TheodorakisC.W. (1989). Size segregation and the effects of oddity on predation risk in minnow schools. — Anim. Behav. 38: 496-502.

Utne-PalmA.C. (2001). Response of naive two-spotted gobies Gobiusculus flavescens to visual and chemical stimuli of their natural predator, cod Gadus morhua. — Mar. Ecol. 218: 267-274.

VilellaF.B.S.BeckerF.G.HartzS.M. (2002). Diet of Astyanax species (Teleostei, Characidae) in an Atlantic forest river in Southern Brazil. — Braz. Arch. Biol. Technol. 45: 223-232.

WarburtonK.LeesN. (1996). Species discrimination in guppies: learned responses to visual cues. — Anim. Behav. 52: 371-378.

WardA.J.W.MehnerT. (2010). Multimodal mixed messages: the use of multiple cues allows greater accuracy in social recognition and predator detection decisions in the mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. — Behav. Ecol. 21: 1315-1320.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Fry responded to predator stimuli with significant shoal tightening (mean ± SE), but exhibited no such response to conspecific adults or the heterospecific poeciliid. Species means (P. mexicana, N=18; X. birchmanni, N=17) were not significantly different within the factor ‘stimulus type’ but are shown here for comparative purposes. Different letters above/below bars indicate significance in the overall repeated measures ANCOVA model.

  • View in gallery

    The absolute difference in body size of pairs of fry interacted significantly with ‘stimulus type’ to affect shoal tightening responses (p=0.031). During exposure to conspecifics and adult heterospecifics, fry with larger differences in body size tended to move farther apart. In the presence of the predator, this tendency disappeared and fry shoaling was independent of their body sizes.

  • View in gallery

    Experimental set-up for behavioural trials. Fry testing tanks were placed adjacent to a central stimulus tank. Tanks were placed with only enough space between them such that an opaque divider could be inserted and removed, respectively, for the acclimatization and exposure periods of the trials.

  • View in gallery

    Within trials for single pairs of fry, activity of stimulus species did not differ as tested by paired t-tests among all pairwise comparisons of stimulus species (for all, p>0.6). Shown here is the mean (± SE) active time of each stimulus species across all trials.

Index Card

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 22 22 8
Full Text Views 4 4 4
PDF Downloads 2 2 2
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0