Circular markings, called eyespots, on the wings of Lepidoptera have been shown to be protective against predators. We tested the ‘conspicuousness-hypothesis’ and ‘eye mimicry-hypothesis’ by examining how ‘sparkle’ and colour pattern of eyespots deter predators. The rationale was to test the deterring effect of shape and colour pattern of the eyespots’ elements that are assumed to mimic lens eyes, namely iris, pupil, and sparkle by simultaneous exposure of lepidopteran dummies with equally conspicuous eyespots that differed in their similarity to lens eyes. The results provide evidence that circular and crescent-shaped ‘sparkles’ were more deterring than rectangular-shaped ‘sparkles’. The ‘sparkle’s’ UV-reflection had no effect on the deterrence. Our results support recent findings on the deterrent effect of the eyespot’s ‘sparkle’ and show that colour is less important for deterrence. The characteristic colour pattern of eyespots and illusion of three-dimensionality created by the ‘sparkle’ might contribute to the deterrent effect.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Blest A.D. (1957). The function of eyespot patterns in the Lepidoptera. — Behaviour 11: 209-256.
Blut C., Wilbrandt J., Fels D., Girgel E.I., Lunau K. (2012). The ‘sparkle’ in fake eyes — the protective effect of mimic eyespots in lepidoptera. — Entomol. Exp. Appl. 143: 231-244.
Borgo R., Cignoni P., Scopigno R. (2001). An easy-to-use visualization system for huge cultural heritage meshes. — In: VAST 2001 Conference Proceedings, Athens, Greece, 28–30 November 2001 (Arnold, D., Chalmers, A. & Fellner, D., eds). ACM Siggraph, Athens, p. 121-130.
Breuker C.J., Brakefield P.M. (2002). Female choice depends on size but not symmetry of dorsal eyespots in the butterfly Bicyclus anynana. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 269: 1233-1239.
Brower J.V. (1958). Experimental studies of mimicry in some North American butterflies. I. The monarch, Danaus plexippus, and viceroy, Limenitis archippus archippus. — Evolution 12: 32-47.
Cavoto B.R., Cook R.G. (2006). The contribution of monocular depth cues to scene perception by pigeons. — Psychol. Sci. 17: 628-634.
Cuthill I.C., Bennett A.T.D. (1993). Mimicry and the eye of the beholder. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 253: 203-204.
Darwin C. (1874a). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, 2nd edn. — John Murray, London, p. 396-400.
Darwin C. (1874b). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, 2nd edn. — John Murray, London, p. 427-443.
Davison G.W.H. (1981). Diet and dispersion of the Great Argus. — Ibis 123: 485-494.
Davison G.W.H. (1982). Sexual displays of the great argus pheasant Argusianus argus. — Z. Tierpsychol. 58: 185-202.
Davison G.W.H. (1983). The eyes have it: ocelli in a rainforest pheasant. — Anim. Behav. 31: 1037-1042.
Edmunds M.E. (1974). Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. — Longman, Harlow.
Endler J.A. (1991). Interactions between predators and prey. — In: Behavioural ecology, 3rd edn. ( Krebs J.R., Davies N.B., eds). Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, p. 169-196.
Endler J.A., Endler L.C., Doerr N.R. (2010). Great bowerbirds create theaters with forced perspective when seen by their audience. — Curr. Biol. 20: 1679-1684.
Endler J.A., Mielke P.W. (2005). Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. — Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 86: 405-431.
Endler J.A., Gaburro J., Kelley L.A. (2014). Visual effects in great bowerbird sexual displays and their implications for signal design. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 281: 20140864.
Forkman B. (1998). Hens use occlusion to judge depth in a two-dimensional picture. — Perception 27: 861-867.
Gomez D., Théry M. (2007). Simultaneous crypsis and conspicuousness in color patterns: comparative analysis of a neotropical rainforest bird community. — Am. Nat. 169: 42-61.
Goto E. (2006). The brilliant beauty of the eye: light reflex from the cornea and tear film. — Cornea 25: 78-81.
Goto E., Dogru M., Sato E.A., Matsumoto Y., Takano Y., Tsubota K. (2011). The sparkle of the eye: the impact of ocular surface wetness on corneal light reflection. — Am. J. Ophthalmol. 151: 691-696.
Güntürkün O. (1985). Lateralization of visually controlled behavior in pigeons. — Physiol. Behav. 34: 575-577.
Hart N.S. (2001). The visual ecology of avian photoreceptors. — Prog. Ret. Eye Res. 20: 675-703.
Hossie T.J., Sherratt T.N. (2013). Defensive posture and eyespots deter avian predators from attacking caterpillar models. — Anim. Behav. 86: 383-389.
Hossie T.J., Sherratt T.N., Janzen D.H., Hallwachs W. (2013). An eyespot that “blinks”: an open and shut case of eye mimicry in Eumorpha caterpillars (Lepidoptera: Sphingidae). — J. Nat. Hist. 47: 2915-2926.
Howard I.P. (2012). Perceiving in depth, Vol. 3: Other mechanisms of depth perception. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Howard I.P., Rogers B.J. (1995). Binocular vision and stereopsis. — Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
Janzen D.H., Hallwachs W., Burns J.M. (2010). A tropical horde of counterfeit predator eyes. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107: 11659-11665.
Jeffery G., Erskine L. (2005). Variations in the architecture and development of the vertebrate optic chiasm. — Prog. Ret. Eye Res. 24: 721-753.
Kelley L.A., Kelley J.L. (2014). Animal visual illusion and confusion: the importance of a perceptual perspective. — Behav. Ecol. 25: 450-463.
Knüttel H., Fiedler K. (2001). Host-plant-derived variation in ultraviolet wing patterns influences mate selection by male butterflies. — J. Exp. Biol. 204: 2447-2459.
Kodandaramaiah U., Vallin A., Wiklund C. (2009). Fixed eyespot display in a butterfly thwarts attacking birds. — Anim. Behav. 77: 1415-1419.
Kodandaramaiah U., Lindenfors P., Tullberg B.S. (2013). Deflective and intimidating eyespots: a comparative study of eyespot size and position in Junonia butterflies. — Ecol. Evol. 3: 4518-4524.
König O. (1975). Urmotiv Auge. Neuentdeckte Grundzüge menschlichen Verhaltens. — Piper, München.
Lunau K. (2011). Warnen, Tarnen, Täuschen. Mimikry und Nachahmung bei Pflanze, Tier und Mensch. — Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft, Darmstadt.
Martin G.R. (2009). What is binocular vision for? A birds’ eye view. — J. Vision 9: 1-19.
Martin G.R. (2014). The subtlety of simple eyes: the tuning of visual fields to perceptual challenges in birds. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 369: 20130040.
Martinoya C., Le Houezec J., Bloch S. (1988). Depth resolution in the pigeon. — J. Comp. Physiol. A 163: 33-42.
Merilaita S., Vallin A., Kodandaramaiah U., Dimitrova M., Ruuskanen S., Laaksonen T. (2011). Number of eyespots and their intimidating effect on naive predators in the peacock butterfly. — Behav. Ecol. 22: 1326-1331.
Monteiro A., Brakefield P.M., French V. (1997). Butterfly eyespots: the genetics and development of the color rings. — Evolution 51: 1207-1216.
Olofsson M., Lovlie H., Tibblin J., Jakobsson S., Wiklund C. (2013). Eyespot display in the peacock butterfly triggers antipredator behaviors in naive adult fowl. — Behav. Ecol. 24: 305-310.
Olofsson M., Vallin A., Jakobsson S., Wiklund C. (2010). Marginal eyespots on butterfly wings deflect bird attacks under low light intensities with UV wavelengths. — Plos One 5: e10798.
Poulton E.B. (1890). The colours of animals: their meaning and use, especially considered in the case of insects. — Appelton, New York, NY.
Prudic K.L., Stoehr A.M., Wasik B.R., Monteiro A. (2015). Eyespots deflect predator attack increasing fitness and promoting the evolution of phenotypic plasticity. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 282: 20141531.
Ramachandran V.S. (1988). Perception of shape from shading. — Nature 331: 163-166.
Robbins R.K. (1981). The “false head” hypothesis: predation and wing pattern variation of lycaenid butterflies. — Am. Nat. 118: 770-775.
Robertson K.A., Monteiro A. (2005). Female Bicyclus anynana butterflies choose males on the basis of their dorsal UV-reflective eyespot pupils. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 272: 1541-1546.
Roulin A. (2014). Melanin-based colour polymorphism responding to climate change. — Glob. Change Biol. 20: 3344-3350.
Saenko S.V., Marialva M.S.P., Beldade P. (2011). Involvement of the conserved Hox gene Antennapedia in the development and evolution of a novel trait. — EvoDevo 2: 9.
Schenkel R. (1956). Zur Deutung der Balzleistungen einiger Phasianiden und Tetraoniden. — Ornithol. Beobacht. 53: 182-201.
Sczepek J. (2011). Visuelle Wahrnehmung: Einführung in die Konzepte Bildentstehung, Helligkeit und Farbe, Raumtiefe, Größe, Kontrast und Schärfe. — Books on Demand, Norderstedt.
Stevens M. (2005). The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. — Biol. Rev. 80: 573-588.
Stevens M., Cantor A., Graham J., Winney I.S. (2009). The function of animal ‘eyespots’: conspicuousness but not eye mimicry is the key. — Curr. Zool. 55: 319-326.
Stevens M., Hardman C.J., Stubbins C.L. (2008). Conspicuousness, not eye mimicry, makes “eyespots” effective antipredator signals. — Behav. Ecol. 19: 525-531.
Stevens M., Hopkins E., Hinde W., Adcock A., Connolly Y., Troscianko T., Cuthill I.C. (2007). Field experiments on the effectiveness of ‘eyespots’ as predator deterrents. — Anim. Behav. 74: 1215-1227.
Sword G.A., Simpson S.J., El Hadi O.T.M., Wilps H. (2000). Density-dependent aposematism in the desert locust. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 267: 63-68.
Vallin A., Jakobsson S., Lind J., Wiklund C. (2005). Prey survival by predator intimidation: an experimental study of peacock butterfly defence against blue tits. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 272: 1203-1207.
Vallin A., Jakobsson S., Wiklund C. (2007). An eye for an eye? On the generality of the intimidating quality of eyespots in a butterfly and a hawkmoth. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61: 1419-1424.
Wickler W. (1968). Mimicry in plants and animals. — McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
zur Strassen O. (1935). Plastisch wirkende Augenflecke und die “Geschlechtliche Zuchtwahl”. — Gustav Fischer, Jena, p. 1-48.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1252 | 143 | 17 |
Full Text Views | 405 | 18 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 349 | 37 | 0 |
Circular markings, called eyespots, on the wings of Lepidoptera have been shown to be protective against predators. We tested the ‘conspicuousness-hypothesis’ and ‘eye mimicry-hypothesis’ by examining how ‘sparkle’ and colour pattern of eyespots deter predators. The rationale was to test the deterring effect of shape and colour pattern of the eyespots’ elements that are assumed to mimic lens eyes, namely iris, pupil, and sparkle by simultaneous exposure of lepidopteran dummies with equally conspicuous eyespots that differed in their similarity to lens eyes. The results provide evidence that circular and crescent-shaped ‘sparkles’ were more deterring than rectangular-shaped ‘sparkles’. The ‘sparkle’s’ UV-reflection had no effect on the deterrence. Our results support recent findings on the deterrent effect of the eyespot’s ‘sparkle’ and show that colour is less important for deterrence. The characteristic colour pattern of eyespots and illusion of three-dimensionality created by the ‘sparkle’ might contribute to the deterrent effect.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1252 | 143 | 17 |
Full Text Views | 405 | 18 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 349 | 37 | 0 |