Pops are a low-frequency, pulsed vocalization produced by Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops cf. aduncus) in Shark Bay, Western Australia and are often heard when male alliances are consorting or ‘herding’ a female. Previous research indicated that pops produced in this context are an agonistic ‘come-hither’ demand produced by males and directed at female consorts. Here we examine pop occurrence during focal follows on bottlenose dolphin alliances with and without female consorts present. Regression analysis was conducted to determine if pop numbers were higher in the presence of female consorts, and if variables including group size alone and the interaction between presence/absence of a consortship and group size, influenced pop production. While the presence or absence of a consortship significantly affected the number of pops, average group size had no significant effect on pop production. Our research provides further evidence that the pop vocalization plays an important role in consortships.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Boyer R.T., Kitchen D.W. (1987). Sex and age-class differences in vocalizations of Roosevelt elk during rut. — Am. Mid. Nat. 118: 225-235.
Bruck J.N. (2013). Decades-long social memory in bottlenose dolphins. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 280: 20131726.
Caldwell M.C., Caldwell D.K. (1977). Cetaceans. — In: How animals communicate ( Sebeok T.A., ed.). Indiana University Press, Bloomington, IN, p. 794-808.
Caldwell M.C., Haugen R.M., Caldwell D.K. (1962). High-energy sound associated with fright in the dolphin. — Science 138: 907-908.
Connor R.C. (2007). Complex alliance relationships in bottlenose dolphins and a consideration of selective environments for extreme brain size evolution in mammals. — Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 362: 587-602.
Connor R.C., Krützen M. (2015). Male dolphin alliances in Shark Bay: changing perspectives in a 30-year study. — Anim. Behav. 103: 223-235.
Connor R.C., Mann J. (2006). Social cognition in the wild: Machiavellian dolphins? — In: Rational animals ( Hurley S., Nudd M., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 329-367.
Connor R.C., Smolker R.A. (1996). ‘Pop’ goes the dolphin: a vocalization male bottlenose dolphins produce during consortships. — Behaviour 133: 643-662.
Connor R.C., Vollmer N.L. (2009). Sexual coercion in dolphin consortships: a comparison with chimpanzees. — In: Sexual coercion in primates ( Muller M., Wrangham R.W., eds). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, p. 218-243.
Connor R.C., Smolker R.A., Richards A.F. (1992a). Two levels of alliance formation among male bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.). — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89: 987-990.
Connor R.C., Smolker R.A., Richards A.F. (1992b). Dolphin alliances and coalitions. — In: Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals ( Harcourt A.H., de Waal F.B.M., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 415-443.
Connor R.C., Richards A.F., Smolker R.A., Mann J. (1996). Patterns of female attractiveness in Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins. — Behaviour 133: 37-69.
Connor R.C., Heithaus R.M., Barre L.M. (1999). Superalliance of bottlenose dolphins. — Nature 371: 571-572.
Connor R.C., Wells R., Mann J., Read A. (2000). The bottlenose dolphin: social relationships in a fission–fusion society. — In: Cetacean societies: field studies of whales and dolphins ( Mann J., Connor R., Tyack P., Whitehead H., eds). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, p. 91-126.
Connor R.C., Heithaus M.R., Barre L.M. (2001). Complex social structure, alliance stability and mating access in a bottlenose dolphin ‘super-alliance’. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 268: 263-267.
Connor R.C., Watson-Capps J.J., Sherwin W.B., Krützen M. (2011). A new level of complexity in the male alliance networks of Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.). — Biol. Lett. 7: 623-626.
Emery N.J., Seed A.M., von Bayern A.M.P., Clayton N.S. (2007). Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. — Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 362: 489-505.
Finneran J.J., Oliver C.W., Schaefer K.M., Ridgway S.H. (2000). Source levels and estimated yellowfin tuna (Thunnus albacares) detection ranges for dolphin jaw pops, breaches, and tail slaps. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107: 649-656.
Gerber G.R., Buenau K.E., Vanblaricom G.R. (2004). Density dependence and risk of extinction in a small population of sea otters. — Biodivers. Conserv. 13: 2741-2757.
Goodall J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. — Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Gotelli N.J., Ellison A.M. (2004). A primer of ecological statistics. — Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.
Herman L.M. (2010). What laboratory research has told us about dolphin cognition. — Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 23: 310-330.
Herzing D.L. (1996). Vocalizations and associated underwater behavior of free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. — Aquat. Mamm. 22(2): 61-79.
Huggenberger S., Rauschmann M.A., Oelschläger H.H.A. (2008). Functional morphology of the hyolaryngeal complex of the harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena): implications for its role in sound production and respiration. — Anat. Rec. 291: 1262-1270.
Janik V.M. (2009). Acoustic communication in delphinids. — Adv. Stud. Behav. 40: 123-157.
Janik V.M. (2014). Cetacean vocal learning and communication. — Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28: 60-65.
King S.L., Janik V.M. (2013). Bottlenose dolphins can use learned vocal labels to address each other. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110: 13216-13221.
King S.L., Sayigh L.S., Wells R.S., Fellner W., Janik V.M. (2013). Vocal copying of individually distinctive signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 280: 20130053.
Lilly J.C. (1962). Vocal behavior of the bottlenose dolphin. — Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 106: 520-529.
Marino L. (2002). Convergence of complex cognitive abilities in cetaceans and primates. — Brain Behav. Evol. 59: 21-32.
Marino L., Connor R.C., Fordyce R.E., Herman L.M., Hof P.R., Lefebvre L., Lusseau D., McCowan B., Nimchinsky E., Pack A.A., Rendell L., Reidenberg J.S., Reiss D., Uhen M.D., Van der Gucht E., Whitehead H. (2007). Cetaceans have complex brains for complex cognition. — PLoS Biol. 5: 966-972.
Matthews J.N., Rendell L.E., Gordon J.C.D., Macdonald D.W. (1999). A review of frequency and time parameters of cetacean tonal calls. — Bioacoustics 10: 47-71.
McCowan B., Reiss D. (1995). Maternal aggressive contact vocalizations in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): wide band, low frequency signals during mother/aunt-infant interactions. — Zoo Biol. 14: 293-309.
Morisaka T., Connor R.C. (2007). Predation by killer whales (Orcinus orca) and the evolution of whistle loss and narrow-band high frequency clicks in odontocetes. — J. Evol. Biol. 20: 1439-1458.
Nowacek D.P. (2002). Sequential foraging behaviour of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, in Sarasota Bay, FL. — Behaviour 139: 1125-1145.
Nowacek D.P. (2005). Acoustic ecology of foraging bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), habitat-specific use of three sound types. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 21: 587-602.
Paulos R.D., Dudzinski K.M., Kuczaj S.A. II (2008). The role of touch in select social interactions of Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). — J. Ethol. 26: 153-164.
Quick N.J., Janik V.M. (2012). Bottlenose dolphins exchange signature whistles when meeting at sea. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: 2539-2545.
Randić S., Connor R.C., Sherwin W.B., Krützen M. (2012). A novel mammalian social structure in Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.): complex male alliances in an open social network. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: 3083-3090.
Remage-Healey L., Nowacek D.P., Bass A.H. (2006). Dolphin foraging sounds suppress calling and elevate stress hormone levels in a prey species, the Gulf toadfish. — J. Exp. Biol. 209: 4444-4451.
Richards D.G., Wolz J.P., Herman L.M. (1984). Vocal mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. — J. Comp. Psychol. 98: 10-28.
SAS Institute (2008). SAS Online Doc. Version 9.2. — SAS Institute, Cary, NC.
Sayigh L.S., Wells R.S., Tyack P.L. (1993). Recording underwater sounds of free-ranging dolphins while underway in a small boat. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 9: 209-213.
Sayigh L.S., Tyack P.L., Wells R.S., Solow A.R., Scott M.D., Irvine A.B. (1999). Individual recognition in wild bottlenose dolphins: a field test using playback experiments. — Anim. Behav. 57: 41-50.
Schusterman R.J. (1977). Temporal patterning in sea lion barking (Zalophuscalifornianus). — Behav. Biol. 20: 404-408.
Simard P., Lace N., Gowans S., Quintana-Rizzo E., Kuczaj S.A. II, Wells R.S., Mann D.A. (2011). Low frequency narrow-band calls in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): signal properties, function, and conservation implications. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130: 3068-3076.
Smolker R.A., Mann J., Smuts B.B. (1993). Use of signature whistles during separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33: 393-402.
Smolker R.A., Richards A.F., Connor R.C., Pepper J.W. (1992). Sex differences in patterns of association among Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins. — Behaviour 123: 38-69.
Smuts B.B., Smuts R.W. (1993). Male aggression and sexual coercion of females in non-human primates and other mammals: evidence and theoretical implications. — Adv. Stud. Behav. 93: 1-102.
Tyack P.L. (1986). Whistle repertoires of two bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: mimicry of signature whistles. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18: 251-257.
Watson-Capps J. (2005). Female mating behavior in the context of sexual coercion and female ranging behavior of bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay, Western Australia. — PhD thesis, Georgetown University, Washington, DC.
Whitehead H., Rendell L. (2015). The cultural lives of whales and dolphins. — University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 596 | 113 | 12 |
Full Text Views | 223 | 12 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 77 | 14 | 0 |
Pops are a low-frequency, pulsed vocalization produced by Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops cf. aduncus) in Shark Bay, Western Australia and are often heard when male alliances are consorting or ‘herding’ a female. Previous research indicated that pops produced in this context are an agonistic ‘come-hither’ demand produced by males and directed at female consorts. Here we examine pop occurrence during focal follows on bottlenose dolphin alliances with and without female consorts present. Regression analysis was conducted to determine if pop numbers were higher in the presence of female consorts, and if variables including group size alone and the interaction between presence/absence of a consortship and group size, influenced pop production. While the presence or absence of a consortship significantly affected the number of pops, average group size had no significant effect on pop production. Our research provides further evidence that the pop vocalization plays an important role in consortships.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 596 | 113 | 12 |
Full Text Views | 223 | 12 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 77 | 14 | 0 |