Why do wild bonobos not use tools like chimpanzees do?

in Behaviour
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

One of the most conspicuous behavioural differences among great apes is the paucity of tool use among wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) in comparison to chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) who are one of the most prolific and skilled tool users in the animal kingdom. This is in spite of the fact that bonobo tool use repertories are as large and diverse as chimpanzees’ in captive settings. In this study, we compared tool using behaviours and potential drivers of these behaviours in the Wamba bonobo population located in central Democratic Republic of Congo with the Goualougo chimpanzee population of northern Republic of Congo. The tool use repertoire of wild bonobos was comprised of only 13 behaviours, compared to 42 for chimpanzees. However, the number of tool behaviours observed in each study site was similar between bonobos and chimpanzees, and many types of tool use for social, self-grooming/stimulation, and comfort/protection functions were commonly used by both species. A marked difference is that 25 of 42 tool behaviours exhibited by chimpanzees are performed for feeding, in contrast to a single report of bonobos using a leaf sponge to drink water. We examined whether the differences in tool use repertoires can be explained by the necessity, opportunity, relative profitability, or invention hypotheses. We found that habitat composition and fluctuation of fruit production at these two sites were similar, particularly when compared with variation observed between sites within each species. Thus it was unlikely that the necessity hypothesis explains the lack of tool use for feeding in bonobos. Though further study at Wamba is needed, we did not identify any obvious differences in prey availability that would indicate differences in tool using opportunities between the sites. This study could not test the relative profitability hypothesis, and further research is needed on whether tool use is the most efficient means of calorie or protein intake for wild apes. Bonobos at Wamba formed much larger and stable parties than chimpanzees at Goualougo, which was contrary to the prediction by the invention hypothesis. Another explanation is that differences in tool use behaviour between bonobos and chimpanzees might not be explained by the current ecological or social conditions, but rather by circumstances during the Pleistocene Epoch. The observed species differences might also reflect divergent behavioural predispositions, rather than actual differences in cognitive abilities.

Why do wild bonobos not use tools like chimpanzees do?

in Behaviour

Sections

References

BeauneD.BollacheL.FruthB.BretagnolleF. (2012). Bush pig (Potamochoerus porcus) seed predation of bush mango (Irvingia gabonensis) and other plant species in Democratic Republic of Congo. — Afr. J. Ecol. 50: 509-512.

BeauneD.BretagnolleF.BollacheL.HohmannG.SurbeckM.BoursonC.FruthB. (2013). The bonobo–Dialium positive interactions: seed dispersal mutualism. — Am. J. Primatol. 75: 394-403.

BoeschC.Boesch-AchermannH. (2000). The chimpanzees of the Tai forest: behavioral ecology and evolution. — Oxford University PressNew York, NY.

BogartS.L.PruetzJ.D. (2011). Insectivory of savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Fongoli, Senegal. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 145: 11-20.

BooseK.J.WhiteF.J.MeineltA. (2013). Sex differences in tool use acquisition in bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Am. J. Primatol. 75: 917-926.

BoysenS.KuhlmeierV.A.HallidayP.HallidayY.M. (1999). Tool use in captive gorillas. — In: The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans: comparative perspectives ( ParkerS.T.MitchellR.W.MilesH.L. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  179-187.

ChapmanC.A.WranghamR.ChapmanL. (1994). Indices of habitat-wide fruit abundance in tropical forests. — Biotropica 26: 160-171.

DeblauweI. (2009). Temporal variation in insect-eating by chimpanzees and gorillas in Southeast Cameroon: extension of niche differentiation. — Int. J. Primatol. 30: 229-252.

DevosC.SanzC.MorganD.OnonongaJ.R.LaporteN.HuynenM.C. (2008). Comparing ape densities and habitats in northern congo: surveys of sympatric gorillas and chimpanzees in the odzala and ndoki regions. — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 439-451.

EnomotoT. (1990). Social play and sexual-behavior of the bonobo (Pan paniscus) with special reference to flexibility. — Primates 31: 469-480.

FoxE.A.SitompulA.F.van SchaikC.P. (1999). Intelligent tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans. — In: The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans ( ParkerS.T.MitchellR.W.MilesH.L. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  99-116.

FoxE.A.van SchaikC.P.SitompulA.WrightD.N. (2004). Intra- and interpopulational differences in orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) activity and diet: implications for the invention of tool use. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 125: 162-174.

FruthB.HohmannG.McGrewW.C. (1999). The Pan species. — In: The nonhuman primates ( DolhinowP.FuentesA. eds). Mayfield PublishingMountain View, CA p.  64-72.

FuruichiT. (1997). Agonistic interactions and matrifocal dominance rank of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. — Int. J. Primatol. 18: 855-875.

FuruichiT. (2009). Factors underlying party size differences between chimpanzees and bonobos: a review and hypotheses for future study. — Primates 50: 197-209.

FuruichiT.HashimotoC.TashiroY. (2001). Fruit availability and habitat use by chimpanzees in the Kalinzu forest, Uganda: examination of fallback foods. — Int. J. Primatol. 22: 929-945.

FuruichiT.IdaniG.IhobeH.HashimotoC.TashiroY.SakamakiT.MulavwaM.N.YangozeneK.KurodaS. (2012). Long-term studies on wild bonobos at Wamba, Luo Scientific Reserve, D. R. Congo: towards the understanding of female life history in a male-philopatric species. — In: Long-term field studies of primates ( KappelerP.M.WattsD.P. eds). SpringerBerlin p.  413-433.

GoldK.C. (2002). Ladder use and clubbing by a bonobo (Pan paniscus) in Apenheul Primate Park. — Zoo Biology 21: 607-611.

GruberT.ClayZ.ZuberbuhlerK. (2010). A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. — Anim. Behav. 80: 1023-1033.

GruberT.PottsK.B.KrupenyeC.ByrneM.-R.Mackworth-YoungC.McGrewW.C.ReynoldsV.ZuberbuhlerK. (2012). The influence of ecology on chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) cultural behavior: a case study of five Ugandan chimpanzee communities. — J. Comp. Psychol. 126: 446-457.

GuntherM.BoeschC. (1993). Energetic costs of nut-cracking behavior in wild chimpanzees. — In: Evolution of hands ( ChiversD.PreuschoftH. eds). Gustav FischerStuttgart p.  109-129.

HarrisonM.E.MarshallA.J. (2011). Strategies for the use of fallback foods in apes. — Int. J. Primatol. 32: 531-565.

HashimotoC.FuruichiT.TashiroY. (2001). What factors affect the size of chimpanzee parties in the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda? Examination of fruit abundance and number of estrous females. — Int. J. Primatol. 22: 947-959.

HashimotoC.FuruichiT.TashiroY.KimuraD. (1999). Vegetation of the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda: ordination of forest types using principal component analysis. — Afr. Stud. Monogr. 20: 229-239.

HashimotoC.TashiroY.KimuraD.EnomotoT.IngmansonJ.IdaniG.FuruichiT. (1998). Habitat use and ranging of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. — Int. J. Primatol. 19: 1045-1060.

Hernandez-AguilarR.A. (2009). Chimpanzee nest distribution and site reuse in a dry habitat: implications for early hominin ranging. — J. Hum. Evol. 57: 350-364.

Hernandez-AguilarR.A.MooreJ.PickeringT.R. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of plants. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104: 19210-19213.

HerrmannE.HareB.CallJ.TomaselloM. (2010). Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. — PLoS ONE 5: e12438.

HerrmannE.WobberV.CallJ. (2008). Great apes’ (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) understanding of tool functional properties after limited experience. — J. Comp. Psychol. 122: 220-230.

HohmannG.FruthB. (2003a). Culture in bonobos? Between-species and within-species variation in behavior. — Curr. Anthropol. 44: 563-571.

HohmannG.FruthB. (2003b). Lui Kotal — a new site for field research on bonobos in the Salonga National Park. — Pan Africa News 10: 25-27.

HowardP.C. (1991). Nature conservation in Uganda’s tropical forest reserves. — IUCNGland.

HumleT.MatsuzawaT. (2004). Oil palm use by adjacent communities of chimpanzees at Bossou and Nimba Mountains, West Africa. — Int. J. Primatol. 25: 551-581.

HuntK.D.McGrewW.C. (2002). Chimpanzees in the dry habitats of Assirik, Senegal and Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. — In: Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos ( BoeschC.HohmannG.MarchantL.F. eds). Cambridge University PressNew York, NY p.  35-51.

IdaniG.KurodaS.KanoT.AsatoR. (1994). Flora and vegetation of Wamba forest, central Zaire with reference to bonobo (Pan paniscus) foods. — Tropics 3: 309-332.

IngmansonE.J. (1996). Tool-using behaviors in wild Pan paniscus: social and ecological considerations. — In: Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes ( RussonA.E.BardK.A.ParkerS.T. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  190-210.

InogwabiniB.I.BewaM.LongwangoM.AbokomeM.VuvuetM. (2008). The bonobos of the Lake Tumba — Lake Maindombe Hinterland: threats and opportunities for population conservation. — In: The bonobos: behavior ecology and conservation ( FuruichiT.ThompsonJ. eds). SpringerNew York, NY p.  273-290.

JordanC. (1982). Object manipulation and tool-use in captive pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). — J. Hum. Evol. 11: 35-39.

KanoT. (1982a). The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba. — Primates 23: 171-188.

KanoT. (1982b). The use of leafy twigs for rain cover by the pygmy chimpanzees of Wamba. — Primates 23: 453-457.

KanoT. (1992). The last ape: pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. — Stanford University PressStanford, CA.

KanoT. (1997). Leaf-dropping sexual display exhibited by a male bonobo at Wamba. — Pan Africa News 4: 3-4.

KanoT.MulavwaM. (1984). Feeding ecology of the pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba. — In: The pygmy chimpanzee ( SusmanR.L. ed.). PlenumNew York, NY p.  233-274.

KawamotoY.TakemotoH.HiguchiS.SakamakiT.HartJ.A.HartT.B.TokuyamaN.ReinartzG.E.GuislainP.DupainJ.CobdenA.K.MulavwaM.N.YangozeneK.DarrozeS.DevosC.FuruichiT. (2013). Genetic structure of wild bonobo populations: diversity of mitochondrial DNA and geographical distribution. — PLoS ONE 8: e59660.

KoopsK. (2011). Chimpanzees in the Seringbara region of the Nimba mountains. — In: The chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba ( MatsuzawaT.HumleT.SugiyamaY. eds). SpringerTokyo p.  277-287.

KoopsK.McGrewW.C.de VriesH.MatsuzawaT. (2012). Nest-building by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Seringbara, Nimba mountains: antipredation, thermoregulation, and antivector hypotheses. — Int. J. Primatol. 33: 356-380.

KoopsK.McGrewW.C.MatsuzawaT. (2013). Ecology of culture: do environmental factors influence foraging tool use in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus?Anim. Behav. 85: 175-185.

LaioloP.JovaniR. (2007). The emergence of animal culture conservation. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 22: 5.

LonsdorfE.V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. — Anim. Behav. 70: 673-683.

LonsdorfE.V. (2006). What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)?Anim. Cogn. 9: 36-46.

LonsdorfE.V.RossS.R.LinickS.A.MilsteinM.S.MelberT.N. (2009). An experimental, comparative investigation of tool use in chimpanzees and gorillas. — Anim. Behav. 77: 1119-1126.

MarshallA.J.WranghamR.W. (2007). Evolutionary consequences of fallback foods. — Int. J. Primatol. 28: 1218-1235.

MayrE.OharaR.J. (1986). The biolgeographic evidence supporting the pleistocene forest refuge hypothesis. — Evolution 40: 55-67.

McGrewW.C. (1979). Evolutionary implication of sex differences in chimpanzee predation and tool use. — In: The great apes ( HamburgDA.McCownE.R. eds). Benjamin/CummingsMenlo Park, CA p.  41-463.

McGrewW.C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: implications for human evolution. — Cambridge University PressCambridge.

McGrewW.C.CollinsD.A. (1985). Tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to obtain termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. — Am. J. Primatol. 9: 47-62.

McGrewW.C.TutinC.E.G.BaldwinP.J. (1979). Chimpanzees, tools and termites: cross-cultural comparisons of Senegal, Tanzania, and Rio Muni. — Man 14: 185-214.

McGrewW.C.BaldwinP.J.TutinC.E.G. (1981). Chimpanzees in a hot, dry and open habitat: Mt. Assirik, Senegal, West Africa. — J. Hum. Evol. 10: 237-244.

McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F.BeuerleinM.M.VranckenD.FruthB.HohmannG. (2007). Prospects for bonobo insectivory: Lui kotal, democratic republic of congo. — Int. J. Primatol. 28: 1237-1252.

MooreJ. (1992). “Savanna” chimpanzees. — In: Topics in PrimatologyVol. 1: human origins ( NishidaT.McGrewW.C.MarlerP.PickfordM.de WaalF.B.M. eds). University of Tokyo PressTokyo p.  99-118.

MorganD.B. (2007). Socio-ecology of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in the Goualougo Triangle Republic of Congo. — PhD thesis Cambridge University Cambridge.

MorganD.SanzC. (2006). Chimpanzee feeding ecology and comparisons with sympatric gorillas in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. — In: Primate feeding ecology in apes and other primates: ecological physiological and behavioural aspects ( HohmannG.RobbinsM.BoeschC. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  97-122.

MorganD.SanzC.OnonongaJ.R.StrindbergS. (2006). Ape abundance and habitat use in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. — Int. J. Primatol. 27: 147-179.

MulavwaM.FuruichiT.YangozeneK.Yamba-YambaM.Motema-SaloB.IdaniG.IhobeH.HashimotoC.TashiroY.MwanzaN. (2008). Seasonal changes in fruit production and party size of bonobos at Wamba. — In: The bonobos: ecology behavior and conservation ( FuruichiT.ThompsonJ. eds). SpringerNew York, NY p.  121-134.

MulavwaM.N.YangozeneK.Yamba-YambaM.Motema-SaloB.MwanzaN.N.FuruichiT. (2010). Nest groups of wild bonobos at Wamba: selection of vegetation and tree species and relationships between nest group size and party size. — Am. J. Primatol. 72: 575-586.

Myers ThompsonJ.A. (2002). Bonobos of the Lukuru Wildlife Research Project. — In: Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos ( BoeschC.HohmannG.MarchantL.F. eds). Cambridge University PressNew York, NY p.  61-70.

NishidaT. (1990). A quarter century of research in the Mahale Mountains: an overview. — In: The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: sexual and life history strategies ( NishidaT. ed.). University of Tokyo PressTokyo p.  3-35.

NishieH. (2011). Natural history of Camponotus ant-fishing by the M group chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. — Primates 52: 329-342.

NishiharaT. (1995). Feeding ecology of western lowland gorillas in the Nouabale-Ndoki National Park, Congo. — Primates 36: 151-168.

PalagiE. (2006). Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for natural social systems and interindividual relationships. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 129: 418-426.

PandolfiS.S.van SchaikC.P.PuseyA.E. (2003). Sex differences in termite fishing among Gombe chimpanzees. — In: Animal social complexity: intelligence culture and individualized societies ( de WaalF.B.M.TyackP.L. eds). Harvard University PressCambridge, MA p.  414-418.

PlanaV. (2004). Mechanisms and tempo of evolution in the African Guineo-Congolian rainforest. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 359: 1585-1594.

ReynoldsV. (2005). The chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: ecology behaviour and conservation. — Oxford University PressNew York, NY.

RutzC.BluffL.A.ReedN.TrosciankoJ.NewtonJ.IngerR.KacelnikA.BearhopS. (2010). The ecological significance of tool use in New Caledonian crows. — Science 329: 1523-1526.

RutzC.St ClairJ.J.H. (2012). The evolutionary origins and ecological context of tool use in New Caledonian crows. — Behav. Proc. 89: 153-165.

Sabater-PiJ. (1979). Feeding behaviour and diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in the Okorobiko Mountains of Rio Muni (West Africa). — Z. Tierpsychol. 50: 265-281.

Sabater PiJ. (1974). An elementary industry of the chimpanzees in the Okorobiko Mountains, Rio Muni (Republic of Equatorial Guinea), West Africa. — Primates 15: 351-364.

SamsonD.R.HuntK.D. (2012). A thermodynamic comparison of arboreal and terrestrial sleeping sites for dry-habitat chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at the Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. — Am. J. Primatol. 74: 811-818.

SanzC.MorganD. (2007). Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, republic of Congo. — J. Hum. Evol. 52: 420-433.

SanzC.M.MorganD.B. (2009). Flexible and persistent tool-using strategies in honey-gathering by wild chimpanzees. — Int. J. Primatol. 30: 411-427.

SanzC.MorganD. (2010). Complexity of chimpanzee tool using behaviors. — In: The mind of the chimpanzee: ecological and experimental perspectives ( LonsdorfE.V.RossS.R.MatsuzawaT. eds). University of Chicago PressChicago, IL p.  127-140.

SanzC.M.MorganD.B. (2013a). The social context of chimpanzee tool use. — In: Tool use in animals ( SanzC.M.CallJ.BoeschC. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  161-175.

SanzC.M.MorganD.B. (2013b). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee tool use. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B 368: 20120416.

SanzC.MorganD.GulickS. (2004). New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo basin. — Am. Nat. 164: 567-581.

SanzC.M.SchoningC.MorganD.B. (2010). Chimpanzees Prey on army ants with specialized tool set. — Am. J. Primatol. 72: 17-24.

SerckxA.HuynenM.-C.BastinJ.-F.HambuckersA.Beudels-JamarR.C.VimondM.RaynaudE.KühlH.S. (2014). Nest grouping patterns of bonobos (Pan paniscus) in relation to fruit availability in a forest-savannah mosaic. — PLoS ONE 9: e93742.

ShumakerR.W.WalkupK.R.BeckB.B. (2011). Animal tool behavior: the use and manufacture of tools by animals. — Johns Hopkins University PressBaltimore, MD.

SpagnolettiN.VisalberghiE.OttoniE.IzarP.FragaszyD. (2012). Stone tool use in wild bearded capuchin monkeys, Cebus libidinosus: is it a strategy to overcome food scarcity?Anim. Behav. 83: 1285-1294.

StanfordC.B. (1998). The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos — empirical evidence and shifting assumptions. — Curr. Anthropol. 39: 399-420.

SurbeckM.HohmannG. (2013). Intersexual dominance relationships and the influence of leverage on the outcome of conflicts in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.: 1-14.

SuzukiS.KurodaS.NishiharaT. (1995). Tool-set for termite-fishing by chimpanzees in the Ndoki forest, Congo. — Behaviour 132: 219-234.

TakasakiH.NishidaT.UeharaS.NorikoshiK.KawanakaK.TakahataY.Hiraiwa-HasegawaM.HasegawaT.HayakiH.MasuiK.HuffmanM.A. (1990). Summary of meteorological data at Mahale research camp, 1973–1988. — In: The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: sexual and life history strategies ( NishidaT. ed.). University of Tokyo PressTokyo p.  291-300.

TakemotoH. (2004). Seasonal change in terrestriality of chimpanzees in relation to microclimate in the tropical forest. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 124: 81-92.

TebbichS.TaborskyM.FesslB.DvorakM. (2002). The ecology of tool-use in the woodpecker finch (Cactospiza pallida). — Ecol. Lett. 5: 656-664.

van SchaikC.P. (2001). Fragility of traditions: the disturbance hypothesis for the loss of local traditions in orangutans. — Int. J. Primatol. 23: 527-538.

WallisJ. (2002). Seasonal aspects of reproduction and sexual behavior in two chimpanzee populations: a comparison of Gombe (Tanzania) and Budongo (Uganda). — In: Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos ( BoeschC.HohmannG.MarchantL.F. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  181-191.

WattsD.P. (2012). Long-term research on chimpanzee behavioral ecology in Kibale national park, Uganda. — In: Long-term field studies of primates ( KappelerP.M.WattsD.P. eds). SpringerHeidelberg p.  313-338.

WhiteF. (1986). The vegetation of Africa: a descriptive memoir to accompany the UNESCO/AETFAT/UNSO vegetation map of Africa. — UNESCOParis.

WhiteF.J. (1992). Activity budgets, feeding behavior, and habitat use of pygmy chimpanzees at Lomako, Zaire. — Am. J. Primatol. 26: 215-223.

WhiteheadH. (2010). Conserving and managing animals that learn socially and share cultures. — Learn. Behav. 38: 329-336.

WhiteheadH.RendellL.OsborneR.W.WursigB. (2004). Culture and conservation of non-humans with reference to whales and dolphins: review and new directions. — Biol. Conserv. 120: 427-437.

WhitenA.GoodallJ.McGrewW.C.NishidaT.ReynoldsV.SugiyamaY.TutinC.E.G.WranghamR.W.BoeschC. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. — Nature 399: 682-685.

WhitenA.GoodallJ.McGrewW.C.NishidaT.ReynoldsV.SugiyamaY.TutinC.E.G.WranghamR.W.BoeschC. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. — Behaviour 138: 1481-1516.

WhitenA.SpiteriA.HornerV.BonnieK.E.LambethS.P.SchapiroS.J.de WaalF.B.M. (2007). Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. — Curr. Biol. 17: 1038-1043.

WilsonM.L.HauserM.D.WranghamR.W. (2001). Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees?Anim. Behav. 61: 1203-1216.

WonY.J.HeyJ. (2005). Divergence population genetics of chimpanzees. — Mol. Biol. Evol. 22: 297-307.

WranghamR.W.ClarkA.P.Isabirye-BasutaG. (1992). Female social relationships and social organization of Kibale Forest chimpanzees. — In: Human origins ( NishidaT.McGrewW.C.MarlerP.PickfordM.de WaalF.B.M. eds). University of Tokyo PressTokyo p.  81-98.

YamagiwaJ.MaruhashiT.YumotoT.MwanzaN. (1996). Dietary and ranging overlap in sympatric gorillas and chimpanzees in Kahuzi-Biega National Park, Zaire. — In: Great ape societies ( McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F.NishidaT. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  82-98.

YamakoshiG. (1998). Dietary responses to fruit scarcity of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: possible implications for ecological importance of tool use. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 106: 283-295.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Comparison of ecological variables at Wamba and Goualougo. (a) Seasonal fluctuation in availability of ripe fruit resources at Wamba and Goualougo. Values for Wamba show the number of clusters of ripe fallen fruit per km of transect. Values for Goualougo show proportion of food trees with ripe fruit on the ground. (b) Monthly mean of daily party sizes for the E1 group of bonobos at Wamba and chimpanzees at Goualougo. Party size included adults, adolescents, and juveniles, but excluded infants.

Index Card

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 32 32 31
Full Text Views 4 4 4
PDF Downloads 1 1 1
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0