Agonistic encounters featuring ritualized displays precede the establishment of dominance relationships in many animals. We investigated the predictive value of the amount of display behaviour (number and duration of displays) vs. quickness to display (latency to express a display behaviour) in determining the outcome of aggressive interactions and establishment of dominant vs. subordinate status in male green anole lizards, Anolis carolinensis. Similar-sized males were paired and observed for ninety minutes as they established social status. We recorded the number, duration (where appropriate) and latencies to first expression for multiple behavioural display components (head bob A, B and C, dewlap displays, open mouth displays, pushups, lateral displays) and colour changes (eyespot presence and body colour changes). Males that eventually won and became the dominant male had significantly higher counts and durations with the exception of Bob C counts, pushup counts and open mouth duration. Future dominants also maintained a green body colour longer than future subordinates, which had brown shades more often and for longer durations. Latency to first express a display component was shorter in future dominants for head bobs A and C, dewlap and open mouth displays when all data were considered. However, all significant latency differences disappeared when data only from pairs in which both males displayed a behaviour were included in the analysis. Counts, durations, and latencies were highly correlated with each other within individuals. The results indicate that behavioural display patterns during an initial display contest predict the outcome of the interaction, with the amount of display behaviour being the best predictor of whether a male will win or lose the contest and hence become dominant or subordinate. These results are consistent with the idea that displays are honest signals of a male’s physiological capacity or stamina, and hence fighting and resource holding ability.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Adkins E., Schlesinger L. (1979). Androgens and the social behavior of male and female lizards (Anolis carolinensis). — Horm. Behav. 13: 139-152.
Andersson M. (1994). Sexual selection. — Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Arnott G., Elwood R.W. (2009). Assessment of fighting ability in animal contests. — Anim. Behav. 77: 991-1004.
Baxter L.R. Jr., Ackermann R.F., Clark E.C., Baxter J.E. (2001a). Brain mediation of Anolis social dominance displays. I. Differential basal ganglia activation. — Brain Behav. Evol. 57: 169-183.
Baxter L.R. Jr., Clark E.C., Ackermann R.F., Lacan G., Melega W.P. (2001b). Brain mediation of Anolis social dominance displays. II. Differential forebrain serotonin turnover, and effects of specific 5-HT receptor agonists. — Brain Behav. Evol. 57: 184-201.
Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. (2000). On the adaptive control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing with independent statistics. — J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 25: 60-83.
Benjamini Y., Krieger A.M., Yekutieli D. (2006). Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate. — Biometrika 93: 491-507.
Brick O., Jakobsson S. (2002). Individual variation in risk taking: the effect of a predatory threat on fighting behavior in Nannacara anomala. — Behav. Ecol. 13: 439-442.
Carazo P., Font E., Desfilis E. (2008). Beyond ‘nasty neighbours’ and ‘dear enemies’? Individual recognition by scent marks in a lizard (Podarcis hispanica). — Anim. Behav. 76: 1953-1963.
Cowlishaw G., Dunbar R.I.M. (1991). Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. — Anim. Behav. 41: 1045-1056.
Cox R.M., Stenquist D.S., Henningsen J.P., Calsbeek R. (2009). Manipulating testosterone to assess links between behavior, morphology, and performance in the brown anole Anolis sagrei. — Phys. Biochem. Zool. 82: 686-698.
Crews D. (1975). Inter-and intraindividual variation in display patterns in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. — Herpetologica 31: 37-47.
Dahlbom S.J., Lagman D., Lundstedt-Enkel K., Sundstrom L.F., Winberg S. (2011). Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio rerio). — Plos One 6: e23565.
David M., Auclair Y., Cezilly F. (2011). Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context. — Anim. Behav. 81: 219-224.
Decourcy K.R., Jenssen T.A. (1994). Structure and use of male territorial headbob signals by the lizard Anolis carolinensis. — Anim. Behav. 47: 251-262.
Ellis L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: a cross-species comparison. — Ethol. Sociobiol. 16: 257-333.
Elwood R.W., Arnott G. (2012). Understanding how animals fight with Lloyd Morgan’s canon. — Anim. Behav. 84: 1095-1102.
Fawcett T.W., Mowles S.L. (2013). Assessments of fighting ability need not be cognitively complex. — Anim. Behav. 86: e1-e7.
Forster G.L., Watt M.J., Korzan W.J., Renner K.J., Summers C.H. (2005). Opponent recognition in male green anoles, Anolis carolinensis. — Anim. Behav. 69: 733-740.
Fox R.A., Ladage L.D., Roth T.C., Pravosudov V.V. (2009). Behavioural profile predicts dominance status in mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli. — Anim. Behav. 77: 1441-1448.
Garcia M.J., Paiva L., Lennox M., Sivaraman B., Wong S.C., Earley R.L. (2012). Assessment strategies and the effects of fighting experience on future contest performance in the green anole (Anolis carolinensis). — Ethology 118: 821-834.
Garcia M.J., Murphree J., Wilson J., Earley R.L. (2014). Mechanisms of decision making during contests in green anole lizards: prior experience and assessment. — Anim. Behav. 92: 45-54.
Garland T., Hankins E., Huey R.B. (1990). Locomotor capacity and social dominance in male lizards. — Funct. Ecol. 4: 243-250.
Greenberg B., Noble G.K. (1944). Social behavior of the American chameleon (Anolis carolinensis Voigt). — Phys. Zool. 17: 392-439.
Greenberg N. (2002). Ethological aspects of stress in a model lizard, Anolis carolinensis. — Integrat. Comp. Biol. 42: 526-540.
Greenberg N., Crews D. (1990). Endocrine and behavioral responses to aggression and social dominance in the green anole lizard, Anolis carolinensis. — Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 77: 246-255.
Greenberg N., Chen T., Crews D. (1984). Social status, gonadal state, and the adrenal stress response in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. — Horm. Behav. 18: 1-11.
Hadley M.E., Goldman J.M. (1969). Physiological color changes in reptiles. — Am. Zool. 9: 223-231.
Henningsen J.P., Irschick D.J. (2012). An experimental test of the effect of signal size and performance capacity on dominance in the green anole lizard. — Funct. Ecol. 26: 3-10.
Hsu Y.Y., Earley R.L., Wolf L.L. (2006). Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: mechanisms and contest outcomes. — Biol. Rev. 81: 33-74.
Huntingford F.A. (1976). The relationship between anti-predator behaviour and aggression among conspecifics in the three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. — Anim. Behav. 24: 245-260.
Hurd P.L. (1997). Is signalling of fighting ability costlier for weaker individuals? — J. Theor. Biol. 184: 83-88.
Husak J.F., Irschick D.J., Meyers J.J., Lailvaux S.P., Moore I.T. (2007). Hormones, sexual signals, and performance of green anole lizards (Anolis carolinensis). — Horm. Behav. 52: 360-367.
Huyghe K., Vanhooydonck B., Scheers H., Molina-Borja M., Van Damme R. (2005). Morphology, performance and fighting capacity in male lizards, Gallotia galloti. — Funct. Ecol. 19: 800-807.
Jenssen T., Nunez S. (1998). Spatial and breeding relationships of the lizard, Anolis carolinensis: evidence of intrasexual selection. — Behaviour 135: 981-1003.
Jenssen T.A., Decourcy K.R., Congdon J.D. (2005). Assessment in contests of male lizards (Anolis carolinensis): how should smaller males respond when size matters? — Anim. Behav. 69: 1325-1336.
Jenssen T.A., Garrett S., Sydor W.J. (2012). Complex signal usage by advertising male green anoles (Anolis carolinensis): a test of assumptions. — Herpetologica 68: 345-357.
Kabelik D., Alix V.C., Burford E.R., Singh L.J. (2013). Aggression- and sex-induced neural activity across vasotocin populations in the brown anole. — Horm. Behav. 63: 437-446.
Kabelik D., Alix V.C., Singh L.J., Johnson A.L., Choudhury S.C., Elbaum C.C., Scott M.R. (2014). Neural activity in catecholaminergic populations following sexual and aggressive interactions in the brown anole, Anolis sagrei. — Brain Res. 1553: 41-58.
Koolhaas J.M., Korte S.M., De Boer S.F., van der Vegt B.J., Van Reenen C.G., Hopster H., de Jong I.C., Ruis M.A.W., Blokhuis H.J. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23: 925-935.
Koolhaas J.M., de Boer S.F., Buwalda B., van Reenen K. (2007). Individual variation in coping with stress: a multidimensional approach of ultimate and proximate mechanisms. — Brain Behav. Evol. 70: 218-226.
Koolhaas J.M., de Boer S.F., Coppens C.M., Buwalda B. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. — Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31: 307-321.
Korzan W.J., Summers T.R., Ronan P.J., Summers C.H. (2000). Visible sympathetic activity as a social signal in Anolis carolinensis: changes in aggression and plasma catecholamines. — Horm. Behav. 38: 193-199.
Korzan W.J., Summers T.R., Ronan P.J., Renner K.J., Summers C.H. (2001). The role of monoaminergic nuclei during aggression and sympathetic social signalling. — Brain Behav. Evol. 57: 317-327.
Korzan W.J., Summers T.R., Summers C.H. (2002). Manipulation of visual sympathetic sign stimulus modifies social status and plasma catecholamines. — Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 128: 153-161.
Korzan W.J., Forster G.L., Watt M.J., Summers C.H. (2006a). Dopaminergic activity modulation via aggression, status, and a visual social signal. — Behav. Neurosci. 120: 93-102.
Korzan W.J., Øverli O., Summers C.H. (2006b). Future social rank: forecasting status in the green anole (Anolis carolinensis). — Acta Ethol. 9: 48-57.
Korzan W.J., Hoglund E., Watt M.J., Forster G.L., Øverli O., Lukkes J.L., Summers C.H. (2007). Memory of opponents is more potent than visual sign stimuli after social hierarchy has been established. — Behav. Brain Res. 183: 31-42.
Lailvaux S.P., Irschick D.J. (2007). The evolution of performance-based male fighting ability in Caribbean Anolis lizards. — Am. Nat. 170: 573-586.
Lailvaux S.P., Herrel A., VanHooydonck B., Meyers J.J., Irschick D.J. (2004). Performance capacity, fighting tactics and the evolution of life-stage male morphs in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis). — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 271: 2501-2508.
Ling T.J., Forster G.L., Watt M.J., Korzan W.J., Renner K.J., Summers C.H. (2009). Social status differentiates rapid neuroendocrine responses to restraint stress. — Physiol. Behav. 96: 218-232.
Lovern M.B., Jenssen T.A. (2003). Form emergence and fixation of head bobbing displays in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis): a reptilian model of signal ontogeny. — J. Comp. Psychol. 117: 133-141.
McGlothlin J.W., Jawor J.M., Greives T.J., Casto J.M., Phillips J.L., Ketterson E.D. (2008). Hormones and honest signals: males with larger ornaments elevate testosterone more when challenged. — J. Evol. Biol. 21: 39-48.
Øverli Ø., Korzan W.J., Hoglund E., Winberg S., Bollig H., Watt M., Forster G.L., Barton B.A., Øverli E., Renner K.J., Summers C.H. (2004). Stress coping style predicts aggression and social dominance in rainbow trout. — Horm. Behav. 45: 235-241.
Perry G., Levering K., Girard I., Garland T. (2004). Locomotor performance and social dominance in male Anolis cristatellus. — Anim. Behav. 67: 37-47.
Pike N. (2011). Using false discovery rates for multiple comparisons in ecology and evolution. — Meth. Ecol. Evol. 2: 278-282.
Plavicki J., Yang E.J., Wilczynski W. (2004). Dominance status predicts response to nonsocial forced movement stress in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis). — Physiol. Behav. 80: 547-555.
Poisbleau M., Fritz H., Valeix M., Perroi P.Y., Dalloyau S., Lambrechts M.M. (2006). Social dominance correlates and family status in wintering dark-bellied brent geese, Branta bernicla bernicla. — Anim. Behav. 71: 1351-1358.
Robson M.A., Miles D.B. (2000). Locomotor performance and dominance in male Tree Lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. — Funct. Ecol. 14: 338-344.
Ryan M.J., Wilczynski W. (2011). Introduction to animal behavior: an integrative approach. — Cold Spring Harbor Press, Cold Spring Harbor, NY.
Searcy W.A., Nowicki S. (2005). The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signalling systems. — Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Simon V.B. (2011). Communication signal rates predict interaction outcome in the brown anole lizard, Anolis sagrei. — Copeia: 38-45.
Smith M.J., Harper D.G.C. (1995). Animal signals: models and terminology. — J. Theor. Biol. 177: 305-311.
Smith M.J., Harper D. (2003). Animal signals. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Sperry T.S., Wacker D.W., Wingfield J.C. (2010). The role of androgen receptors in regulating territorial aggression in male song sparrows. — Horm. Behav. 57: 86-95.
Stamps J.A., Krishnan V.V. (1995). Territory acquisition in lizards. 3. Competing for space. — Anim. Behav. 49: 679-693.
Stamps J.A., Krishnan V.V. (1998). Territory acquisition in lizards. IV. Obtaining high status and exclusive home ranges. — Anim. Behav. 55: 461-472.
Summers C.H. (2001). Mechanisms for quick and variable responses. — Brain Behav. Evol. 57: 283-292.
Summers C.H., Greenberg N. (1994). Somatic correlates of adrenergic activity during aggression in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. — Hormo. Behav. 28: 29-40.
Sundstrom L.F., Petersson E., Höjesjö J., Johnsson J.I., Jarvi T. (2004). Hatchery selection promotes boldness in newly hatched brown trout (Salmo trutta): implications for dominance. — Behav. Ecol. 15: 192-198.
Temeles E.J. (1994). The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they ‘dear enemies’? — Anim. Behav. 47: 339-350.
Tokarz R.R. (1987). Effects of corticosterone treatment on male aggressive behavior in a lizard (Anolis sagrei). — Horm. Behav. 21: 358-370.
Wagner W.E. (1994). Deceptive or honest signalling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in call dominant frequency by male cricket frogs. — Anim. Behav. 44: 449-462.
Watt M.J., Forster G.L., Korzan W.J., Renner K.J., Summers C.H. (2007). Rapid neuroendocrine responses evoked at the onset of social challenge. — Phys. Behav. 90: 567-575.
Whiting M.J., Stuart-Fox D.M., O’Connor D., Firth D., Bennett N.C., Blomberg S.P. (2006). Ultraviolet signals ultra-aggression in a lizard. — Anim. Behav. 72: 353-363.
Wroblewski E.E., Murray C.M., Keele B.F., Schumacher-Stankey J.C., Hahn B.H., Pusey A.E. (2009). Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. — Anim. Behav. 77: 873-885.
Yang E.J., Wilczynski W. (2002). Relationships between hormones and aggressive behavior in green anole lizards: an analysis using structural equation modeling. — Horm. Behav. 42: 192-205.
Yang E.J., Wilczynski W. (2003). Interaction effects of corticosterone and experience on aggressive behavior in the green anole lizard. — Horm. Behav. 44: 281-292.
Yang E.J., Wilczynski W. (2007). Social experience organizes parallel networks in sensory and limbic forebrain. — Dev. Neurobiol. 67: 285-303.
Yang E.J., Phelps S.M., Crews D., Wilczynski W. (2001). The effects of social experience on aggressive behavior in the green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis). — Ethology 107: 777-793.
Yoon J., Sillett T.S., Morrison S.A., Ghalambor C.K. (2012). Breeding density, not life history, predicts interpopulation differences in territorial aggression in a passerine bird. — Anim. Behav. 84: 515-521.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 342 | 84 | 2 |
Full Text Views | 248 | 47 | 2 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 97 | 56 | 4 |
Agonistic encounters featuring ritualized displays precede the establishment of dominance relationships in many animals. We investigated the predictive value of the amount of display behaviour (number and duration of displays) vs. quickness to display (latency to express a display behaviour) in determining the outcome of aggressive interactions and establishment of dominant vs. subordinate status in male green anole lizards, Anolis carolinensis. Similar-sized males were paired and observed for ninety minutes as they established social status. We recorded the number, duration (where appropriate) and latencies to first expression for multiple behavioural display components (head bob A, B and C, dewlap displays, open mouth displays, pushups, lateral displays) and colour changes (eyespot presence and body colour changes). Males that eventually won and became the dominant male had significantly higher counts and durations with the exception of Bob C counts, pushup counts and open mouth duration. Future dominants also maintained a green body colour longer than future subordinates, which had brown shades more often and for longer durations. Latency to first express a display component was shorter in future dominants for head bobs A and C, dewlap and open mouth displays when all data were considered. However, all significant latency differences disappeared when data only from pairs in which both males displayed a behaviour were included in the analysis. Counts, durations, and latencies were highly correlated with each other within individuals. The results indicate that behavioural display patterns during an initial display contest predict the outcome of the interaction, with the amount of display behaviour being the best predictor of whether a male will win or lose the contest and hence become dominant or subordinate. These results are consistent with the idea that displays are honest signals of a male’s physiological capacity or stamina, and hence fighting and resource holding ability.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 342 | 84 | 2 |
Full Text Views | 248 | 47 | 2 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 97 | 56 | 4 |