We examined whether red-winged blackbirds modulate their vocalizations in response to experimental highway noise, alternating between ambient-control and noise-playback periods. Our measures of song duration were shorter, and with a lower value of freq5% (a measure of energy distribution), during noise-playback; however, we interpret these results as noise-induced artefacts. This apparent lack of vocal plasticity should be taken cautiously because we had a small sample size and most birds produced only one song type: song type-related vocal plasticity was unlikely to be found. We found no evidence of a shift in minimum frequency with noise when this was measured with a threshold method on power spectra, but it seemed to increase when measured by eye from spectrograms. Our results suggest that the by-eye practice can lead to bias, which is problematic as several previous studies have used this procedure. Use of the threshold method, over the by-eye practice, is encouraged.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Bermúdez-Cuamatzin E., Ríos-Chelén A.A., Gil D., Macías Garcia C. (2011). Experimental evidence for real-time song frequency shift in response to urban noise in a passerine bird. — Biol. Lett. 7: 36-38.
Brumm H., Slabbekoorn H. (2005). Acoustic communication in noise. — Adv. Stud. Behav. 35: 151-209.
Brumm H., Slater P.J.B. (2006). Ambient noise, motor fatigue, and serial redundancy in chaffinch song. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 60: 475-481.
Brumm H., Zollinger S.A. (2011). The evolution of the Lombard effect: 100 years of psychoacoustic research. — Behaviour 148: 1173-1198.
Brumm H., Zollinger S.A. (2013). Avian vocal production in noise. — In: Animal communication and noise ( Brumm H., ed.). Animal signals and communication 2. Springer, Berlin, p. 187-227.
Cardoso G.C., Atwell J.W. (2011). On the relation between loudness and the increased song frequency of urban birds. — Anim. Behav. 82: 831-836.
Cardoso G.C., Atwell J.W. (2012). On amplitude and frequency in birdsong: a reply to Zollinger et al. — Anim. Behav. 84: e10-e15.
Cartwright L.A., Taylor D.R., Wilson D.R., Chow-Fraser P. (2014). Urban noise affects song structure and daily patterns of song production in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). — Urban Ecosyst. 17: 561-572.
Catchpole C.K., Slater P.J.B. (2008). Bird song. Biological themes and variations, 2nd edn. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Diaz M., Parra A., Gallardo C. (2011). Serins respond to anthropogenic noise by increasing vocal activity. — Behav. Ecol. 22: 332-336.
Francis C.D., Ortega C.P., Cruz A. (2011). Different behavioural responses to anthropogenic noise by two closely related passerine birds. — Biol. Lett. 7: 850-852.
Fuller R.A., Warren P.H., Gaston K.J. (2007). Daytime noise predicts nocturnal singing in urban robins. — Biol. Lett. 3: 368-370.
Gil D., Brumm H. (2014). Acoustic communication in the urban environment: patterns, mechanisms, and potential consequences of avian song adjustments. — In: Avian urban ecology ( Gil D., Brumm H., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 69-83.
Grace M.K., Anderson R.C. (2015). No frequency shift in the “D” notes of Carolina chickadee calls in response to traffic noise. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69: 253-263.
Halfwerk W., Slabbekoorn H. (2009). A behavioural mechanism explaining noise-dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. — Anim. Behav. 78: 1301-1307.
Hanna D., Blouin-Demers G., Wilson D.R., Mennill D.J. (2011). Anthropogenic noise affects song structure in redwinged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). — J. Exp. Biol. 214: 3549-3556.
Hansen P. (1979). Vocal learning: its role in adapting sound structures to long-distance propagation, and a hypothesis on its evolution. — Anim. Behav. 27: 1270-1271.
Kroodsma D.E. (1982). Learning and the ontogeny of sound signals in birds. — In: Acoustic communication in birds ( Kroodsma D.E., Miller E.H., Ouellet H., eds). Academic Press, New York, NY, p. 1-23.
Lengagne T., Aubin T., Lauga J., Jouventin P. (1999). How do king penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) apply the mathematical theory of information to communicate in windy conditions? — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 266: 1623-1628.
Lengagne T., Slater P.J.B. (2002). The effects of rain on acoustic communication: tawny owls have good reason for calling less in wet weather. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 269: 2121-2125.
Leonard M.L., Horn A.G. (2005). Ambient noise and the design of begging signals. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 272: 651-656.
Leonard M.L., Horn A.G. (2008). Does ambient noise affect growth and begging call structure in nestling birds? — Behav. Ecol. 19: 502-507.
Luther D., Baptista L. (2010). Urban noise and the cultural evolution of bird songs. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 277: 469-473.
Luther D., Derryberry E.P. (2012). Birdsongs keep pace with city life: changes in song over time in an urban songbird affects communication. — Anim. Behav. 83: 1059-1066.
Marler P. (2004). Bird calls: a cornucopia for communication. — In: Nature’s music: the science of birdsong ( Marler P., Slabbekoorn H., eds). Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p. 132-177.
Partecke J., Gwinner E. (2007). Increased sedentariness in European blackbirds following urbanization: a consequence of local adaptation? — Ecology 88: 882-890.
Partecke J., Van’t Hof T., Gwinner E. (2004). Differences in the timing of reproduction between urban and forest European blackbirds (Turdus merula): result of phenotypic flexibility or genetic differences? — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 271: 1995-2001.
Partecke J., Gwinner E., Bensch S. (2006). Is urbanisation of European blackbirds (Turdus merula) associated with genetic differentiation? — J. Ornithol. 147: 549-552.
Patricelli G.L., Blickley J.L. (2006). Avian communication in urban noise: causes and consequences of vocal adjustment. — Auk 123: 639-649.
Peters S., Derryberry E.P., Nowicki S. (2012). Songbirds learn songs least degraded by environmental transmission. — Biol. Lett. 8: 736-739.
Podos J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). — Evolution 51: 537-551.
Pohl N.U., Leadbeater E., Slabbekoorn H., Klump G.M., Langemann U. (2012). Great tits in urban noise benefit from high frequencies in song detection and discrimination. — Anim. Behav. 83: 711-721.
Pohl N.U., Slabbekoorn H., Neubauer H., Heil P., Klump G.M., Langemann U. (2013). Why longer song elements are easier to detect: threshold level-duration functions in the great tit and comparison with human data. — J. Comp. Physiol. A 199: 239-252.
Potvin D.A., Parris K.M., Mulder R.A. (2011). Geographically pervasive effects of urban noise on frequency and syllable rate of songs and calls in silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis). — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 278: 2464-2469.
Potvin D.A., Parris K.M., Mulder R.A. (2013a). Limited genetic differentiation between acoustically divergent populations of urban and rural silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis). — Evol. Ecol. 27: 381-391.
Potvin D.A., Mulder R.A. (2013b). Immediate, independent adjustment of call pitch and amplitude in response to varying background noise by silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis). — Behav. Ecol. 24: 1363-1368.
Riebel K., Slater P.J.B. (1998). Male chaffinches (Fringilla coelebs) can copy calls from a tape tutor. — J. Ornithol. 139: 353-355.
Ríos-Chelén A.A. (2009). Bird song: the interplay between urban noise and sexual selection. — Oecol. Bras. 13: 153-164.
Ríos-Chelén A.A., Gavin L., Patricelli G.L. (2015). Anthropogenic noise is associated with changes in acoustic but not visual signals in red-winged blackbirds. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69: 1139-1159.
Ríos-Chelén A.A., Quirós-Guerrero E., Gil D., Macías Garcia C. (2013). Dealing with urban noise: vermilion flycatchers sing longer songs in noisier territories. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67: 145-152.
Ríos-Chelén A.A., Salaberria C., Barbosa I., Macías Garcia C., Gil D. (2012). The learning advantage: bird species that learn their song show a tighter adjustment of song to noisy environments than those that do not learn. — J. Evol. Biol. 25: 2171-2180.
Sewall K.B. (2009). Limited adult vocal learning maintains call dialects but permits pair distinctive calls in red crossbills. — Anim. Behav. 77: 1303-1311.
Sewall K.B., Kelsey T.R., Hahn T.P. (2004). Discrete variants of evening grosbeak flight calls. — Condor 106: 161-165.
Slabbekoorn H. (2013). Songs of the city: noise-dependent spectral plasticity in the acoustic phenotype of urban birds. — Anim. Behav. 85: 1089-1099.
Zollinger S.A., Podos J., Nemeth E., Goller F., Brumm H. (2012). On the relationship between, and measurement of, amplitude and frequency in bird song. — Anim. Behav. 84: e1-e9.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 722 | 145 | 6 |
Full Text Views | 212 | 11 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 107 | 13 | 0 |
We examined whether red-winged blackbirds modulate their vocalizations in response to experimental highway noise, alternating between ambient-control and noise-playback periods. Our measures of song duration were shorter, and with a lower value of freq5% (a measure of energy distribution), during noise-playback; however, we interpret these results as noise-induced artefacts. This apparent lack of vocal plasticity should be taken cautiously because we had a small sample size and most birds produced only one song type: song type-related vocal plasticity was unlikely to be found. We found no evidence of a shift in minimum frequency with noise when this was measured with a threshold method on power spectra, but it seemed to increase when measured by eye from spectrograms. Our results suggest that the by-eye practice can lead to bias, which is problematic as several previous studies have used this procedure. Use of the threshold method, over the by-eye practice, is encouraged.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 722 | 145 | 6 |
Full Text Views | 212 | 11 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 107 | 13 | 0 |