Male behavioral displays (e.g., branch-shaking) are common across Anthropoidea, but their function remains unclear. We examined free-ranging rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago, Puerto Rico, to test three major hypotheses for the function of male displays: (1) mate attraction, (2) mate guarding and (3) male–male dominance competition. Focal and ad libitum behavioural data were recorded for 21 adult males across 9 groups during the mating season. Display rates were calculated for each male in each context (i.e., agonistic, mating). In stable groups, males with high mating success displayed more during consortships than in other contexts and displays were more likely to follow than to precede copulation, whereas males in unstable groups were more likely to displays in agonistic contexts. These results suggest that mate guarding and male–male dominance competition are the primary functions of male display behaviours in rhesus macaques.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Altmann J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. — Behaviour 49: 227-267.
Briffa M., Hardy I.C., Gammell M.P., Jennings D.J., Clarke D.D., Goubault M. (2013). Analysis of animal contest data. — In: Animal contests ( Hardy I.C., Briffa M., eds). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, p. 47-85.
Byers J., Hebets E., Podos J. (2010). Female mate choice based upon male motor performance. — Anim. Behav. 79: 771-778.
Coleman S.W., Patricelli G.L., Borgia G. (2004). Variable female preferences drive complex male displays. — Nature 428: 742-745.
Collins S.A. (1994). Male displays: cause or effect of female preference? — Anim. Behav. 48: 371-375.
Hardy I.C., Briffa M. (2013). Animal contests. — Cambridge University Press, New York, NY.
Hausfater G. (1972). Intergroup behavior of free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). — Folia Primatol. 18: 78-107.
Hedrick A.V. (1986). Female preferences for male calling bout duration in a field cricket. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19: 73-77.
Higham J.P., Heistermann M., Maestripieri D. (2011). The energetics of male–male endurance rivalry in free-ranging rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta. — Anim. Behav. 81: 1001-1007.
Hoffman C.L., Ruiz-Lambides A.V., Davila E., Maldonado E., Gerald M.S., Maestripieri D. (2008). Sex differences in survival costs of reproduction in a promiscuous primate. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62: 1711-1718.
Jackson R.R. (1982). The biology of Portia fimbriata, a web-building jumping spider (Araneae, Salticidae) from Queensland: intraspecific interactions. — J. Zool. 196(2): 295-305.
Jackson R.R. (1988). The biology of Tauala lepidus, a jumping spider (Araneae: Salticidae) from Queensland: display and predatory behaviour. — N. Z. J. Zool. 15(3): 347-364.
Johnstone R.A., Norris K. (1993). Badges of status and the cost of aggression. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32: 127-134.
Kemp D.J. (2013). Contest behaviour in butterflies: fighting without weapons. — In: Animal contests ( Hardy I.C., Briffa M., eds). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, p. 134-146.
Maynard Smith J., Harper D.G.C., Brookfield J.F.Y. (1988). The evolution of aggression: can selection generate variability? — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 319: 557-570 [and discussion].
Mehlman P.T. (1996). Branch shaking and related displays in wild Barbary macaques. — In: Evolution and ecology of macaque societies ( Fa J.E., Lindburg D.G., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 503-526.
Modahl K.B., Eaton G.G. (1977). Display behaviour in a confined troop of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Anim. Behav. 25: 525-535.
Mowles S.L., Ord T.J. (2012). Repetitive signals and mate choice: insights from contest theory. — Anim. Behav. 84: 295-304.
Muehlenbein M.P., Watts D.P., Whitten P.L. (2004). Dominance rank and fecal testosterone levels in adult male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. — Am. J. Primatol. 64: 71-82.
Muller M.N. (2002). Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. — In: Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos ( Boesch C., Hohmann G., Machant L.F., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 112-123.
Patricelli G.L., Uy J.A., Walsh G., Borgia G. (2002). Sexual selection: male displays adjusted to female’s response. — Nature 415: 279-280.
Pryke S.R. (2013). Bird contests: from hatching to fertilisation. — In: Animal contests ( Hardy I.C., Briffa M., eds). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, p. 287-303.
Rawlins R.G., Kessler M.J. (1985). Climate and seasonal reproduction in the Cayo Santiago macaques. — Am. J. Primatol. 9: 87-99.
Rawlins R.G., Kessler M.J. (1986). Demography of the free-ranging Cayo Santiago macaques (1976-1983). — In: The Cayo Santiago macaques: history, behavior, and biology ( Rawlins R.G., Kessler M.J., eds). State University of New York Press, Albany, NY, p. 47-72.
Rohwer S. (1975). The social significance of avian winter plumage variability. — Evolution 29: 593-610.
Rostal D.C., Glick B.B., Eaton G.G., Resko J.A. (1986). Seasonally of adult male Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): androgens and behavior in a confined troop. — Horm. Behav. 20: 452-462.
Sapolsky R.M. (1993). The physiology of dominance in stable versus unstable social hierarchies. — In: Primate social conflict ( Mason W.A., Mendoza S.P., eds). State University of New York Press, Albany, NY, p. 171-204.
Smith L.C., John-Alder H.B. (1999). Seasonal specificity of hormonal, behavioral, and coloration responses to within- and between-sex encounters in male lizards (Sceloporus undulatus). — Horm. Behav. 36: 39-52.
Taylor P.W., Jackson R.R. (1999). Habitat-adapted communication in Trite planiceps, a New Zealand jumping spider (Araneae, Salticidae). — N. Z. J. Zool. 26: 127-154.
Taylor P.W., Hasson O., Clark D.L. (2001). Initiation and resolution of jumping spider contests: roles for size, proximity, and early detection of rivals. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50: 403-413.
Wilson J.D. (1992). Correlates of agonistic display by great tits Parus major. — Behaviour 121: 168-214.
Wolfe L.D. (1981). Display behavior of three troops of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). — Primates 22: 24-32.
Wyman M.T., Mooring M.S., McCowan B., Penedo M.C.T., Hart L.A. (2008). Amplitude of bison bellows reflects male quality, physical condition and motivation. — Anim. Behav. 76: 1625-1639.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 301 | 96 | 24 |
Full Text Views | 197 | 13 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 30 | 5 | 0 |
Male behavioral displays (e.g., branch-shaking) are common across Anthropoidea, but their function remains unclear. We examined free-ranging rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago, Puerto Rico, to test three major hypotheses for the function of male displays: (1) mate attraction, (2) mate guarding and (3) male–male dominance competition. Focal and ad libitum behavioural data were recorded for 21 adult males across 9 groups during the mating season. Display rates were calculated for each male in each context (i.e., agonistic, mating). In stable groups, males with high mating success displayed more during consortships than in other contexts and displays were more likely to follow than to precede copulation, whereas males in unstable groups were more likely to displays in agonistic contexts. These results suggest that mate guarding and male–male dominance competition are the primary functions of male display behaviours in rhesus macaques.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 301 | 96 | 24 |
Full Text Views | 197 | 13 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 30 | 5 | 0 |