Vervet monkey (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) alarm calls to leopards (Panthera pardus) function as a predator deterrent

in Behaviour
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Behavioural predator–prey interactions are difficult to study, especially when predators avoid humans. To gain greater understanding of their dynamism, we conducted a 14-month field study in which we minimized human presence by employing acoustic recorders and camera traps, along with GPS collars deployed on vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) and leopards (Panthera pardus) in Laikipia, Kenya. Recordings at the vervets’ sleeping site revealed that they gave ‘leopard’ alarm calls most frequently near dusk and dawn, whereas photographs showed that leopards approached vervets more closely at night, when the monkeys alarm-called less often. GPS data showed that after vervets alarm-called, leopards within 200 m quickly moved away, changing direction, but when vervets did not alarm-call, leopards continued moving forward. These results reveal that vervets’ leopard alarm calls function as a predator deterrent in addition to a conspecific warning call.

Vervet monkey (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) alarm calls to leopards (Panthera pardus) function as a predator deterrent

in Behaviour

Sections

References

AthreyaV.OddenM.LinnellJ.D.C.KrishnaswamyJ.KaranthU. (2013). Big cats in our backyards: persistence of large carnivores in a human dominated landscape in India. — PLoS ONE 8: e57872.

BaileyT.N. (1993). The African leopard: ecology and behavior of a solitary felid. — Columbia University PressNew York, NY.

BerrymanA.A. (1992). The origins and evolution of predator–prey theory. — Ecology 73: 1530-1535.

BusseC. (1980). Leopard and lion predation upon chacma baboons living in the Moremi Wildlife Reserve. — Botswana Notes Rec. 12: 15-21.

CamposF.A.FediganL.M. (2014). Spatial ecology of perceived predation risk and vigilance behavior in white-faced capuchins. — Behav. Ecol. 25: 477-486.

CaroT.M. (1989). Missing links in predator and antipredator behavior. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 4: 333-334.

CaroT.M. (1995). Pursuit-deterrence revisited. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 10: 500-503.

CaroT.M. (2005). Anti-predator defenses in birds and mammals. — University of Chicago PressChicago, IL.

Chase GreyJ.N.KentV.T.HillR.A. (2013). Evidence of a high density population of harvested leopards in a montane environment. — PLoS ONE 8: e82832.

CheneyD.L.SeyfarthR.M. (1981). Selective forces affecting the predator alarm calls of vervet monkeys. — Behaviour 76: 25-61.

CheneyD.L.SeyfarthR.M. (1990). How monkeys see the world. — University of Chicago PressChicago, IL.

CheneyD.L.SeyfarthR.M.FischerJ.BeehnerJ.BergmanT.JohnsonS.E.KitchenD.M.PalombitR.A.RendallD.SilkJ.B. (2004). Factors affecting reproduction and mortality among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. — Int. J. Primatol. 25: 401-428.

ClarkR.W. (2005). Pursuit-deterrent communication between prey animals and timber rattlesnakes (Crotalus horridus): the response of snakes to harassment displays. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59: 258-261.

ColemanB.T.HillR.A. (2013). Living in a landscape of fear: the impact of predation, resource availability and habitat structure on primate range use. — Anim. Behav. 88: 165-173.

CowlishawG. (1994). Vulnerability to predation in baboon populations. — Behaviour 131: 293-304.

EnstamK.L.IsbellL.A. (2002). Comparison of responses to alarm calls by patas (Erythrocebus patas) and vervet (Cercopithecus aethiops) monkeys in relation to habitat structure. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 119: 3-14.

FrankL.SimpsonD.WoodroffeR. (2003). Foot snares: an effective method for capturing African lions. — Wildl. Soc. Bull. 31: 309-314.

GroblerJ.P.TurnerT.R. (2010). A novel trap design for the capture and sedation of vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops). — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 40: 163-168.

HammondJ.I.LuttbegB.BrodinT.SihA. (2012). Spatial scale influences the outcome of the predator–prey space race between tadpoles and predatory dragonflies. — Funct. Ecol. 26: 522-531.

HartD.L.SussmanR.W. (2005). Man the hunted: primates predators and human evolution. — Westview PressBoulder, CO.

HassonO. (1991). Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 6: 325-329.

IsbellL.A. (1990). Sudden short-term increase in mortality of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) due to leopard predation in Amboseli National Park, Kenya. — Amer. J. Primatol. 21: 41-52.

IsbellL.A. (1994). Predation on primates: ecological patterns and evolutionary consequences. — Evol. Anthropol. 3: 61-71.

IsbellL.A.YoungT.P. (1993). Human presence reduces predation in a free-ranging vervet monkey population in Kenya. — Anim. Behav. 45: 1233-1235.

IsbellL.A.YoungT.P.JaffeK.E.CarlsonA.A.ChancellorR.L. (2009). Demography and life histories of sympatric patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) and vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) in Laikipia, Kenya. — Int. J. Primatol. 30: 103-124.

KiffnerC.NdibalemaV.KiokoJ. (2012). Leopard (Panthera pardus) aggregation and interactions with Olive baboons (Papio anubis) in Serengeti National Park, Tanzania. — Afr. J. Ecol. 51: 168-171.

KitchenD.M.BergmanT.J.CheneyD.L.NicholsonJ.R.SeyfarthR.M. (2010). Comparing responses of four ungulate species to playbacks of baboon alarm calls. — Anim. Cogn. 13: 861-870.

LeaA.J.BarreraJ.P.TomL.M.BlumsteinD.T. (2008). Heterospecific eavesdropping in a nonsocial species. — Behav. Ecol. 19: 1041-1046.

LimaS.L. (2002). Putting predators back into behavioral predator–prey interactions. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 17: 70-75.

MüllerC.A.ManserM.B. (2008). The information banded mongooses extract from heterospecific alarms. — Anim. Behav. 75: 897-904.

NgoprasertD.LynamA.J.GaleG.A. (2007). Human disturbance affects habitat use and behaviour of Asiatic leopard Panthera pardus in Kaeng Krachan National Park, Thailand. — Oryx 41: 343-351.

OddenM.AthreyaV.RattanS.LinnellJ.D.C. (2014). Adaptable neighbours: movement patterns of GPS-collared leopards in human dominated landscapes in India. — PLoS ONE 9: e112044.

PriceT.NdiayeO.HammerschmidtK.FischerJ. (2014). Limited geographic variation in the acoustic structure of and responses to adult male alarm barks of African green monkeys. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 68: 815-825.

SavinoJ.F.SteinR.A. (1982). Predator–prey interaction between largemouth bass and bluegills as influenced by simulated, submersed vegetation. — Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 111: 255-266.

SchelA.M.CandiottiA.ZuberbühlerK. (2010). Predator-deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. — Anim. Behav. 80: 799-808.

SeyfarthR.M.CheneyD.L.MarlerP. (1980a). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. — Science 210: 801-803.

SeyfarthR.M.CheneyD.L.MarlerP. (1980b). Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. — Anim. Behav. 28: 1070-1094.

ShelleyE.L.BlumsteinD.T. (2005). The evolution of vocal alarm communication in rodents. — Behav. Ecol. 16: 169-177.

SikesR.S.GannonW.L. & Animal Care and Use Committee (2011). Guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild animals in research. — J. Mammal. 92: 235-253.

StankowichT.CossR.G. (2008). Alarm walking in Columbian black-tailed deer: its characterization and possible antipredatory signaling functions. — J. Mammal. 89: 636-645.

StruhsakerT.T. (1967). Ecology of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) in the Masai-Amboseli Game Reserve, Kenya. — Ecology 48: 891-904.

WillemsE.P.HillR.A. (2009). Predator-specific landscapes of fear and resource distribution: effects on spatial range use. — Ecology 90: 546-555.

WoodlandD.J.JaafarZ.KnightM.-L. (1980). The “pursuit deterrent” function of alarm signals. — Am. Nat. 115: 748-753.

ZuberbühlerK.NoëR.SeyfarthR.M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. — Anim. Behav. 53: 589-604.

ZuberbühlerK.JennyD.BsharyR. (1999). The predator deterrent function of primate alarm calls. — Ethology 105: 477-490.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Number of 1-h audio files during which vervets gave leopard alarm calls in a given hour during the sampling period vs. number of times in a given hour in which camera traps photographed leopards at the sleeping site. N, audio files per h = 298–305.

  • View in gallery

    Proximity of collared leopards to collared vervets on the three nights when the leopards approached the vervets’ sleeping site, and on the one evening when vervets approached the leopard, before and after vervets gave alarm calls. Open symbols = proximity before alarm calls began; solid symbols = proximity after alarm calls began. Solid lines = nights leopard moved away >200 m from vervets after alarm calls; dashed line = night vervets moved >200 m away from leopard before giving alarm calls. Distance between consecutive symbols reflects speed of travel.

  • View in gallery

    Leopard movements on nights when vervets gave alarm calls. AC (top right), time of first alarm call; solid circles, location of leopard at 15-min intervals; V, location of collared vervet.

  • View in gallery

    Leopard movements on nights when vervets did not give alarm calls. No AC (top right), no alarm calls; solid circles, location of leopard at 15-min intervals; V, location of collared vervet.

Index Card

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 19 19 10
Full Text Views 11 11 11
PDF Downloads 3 3 3
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0