Work-themed play among young children in foraging and farming communities in Central Africa

in Behaviour
No Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

In small-scale societies children have great access to observing adult roles and this is often reflected in their play, however very few empirical studies of work-themed play have been conducted despite substantial implications that this type of play has for social learning. The current study describes the work-themed play patterns of 1 1/2- to 4-year-old Aka and Bofi foragers and Bofi farmers in Central Africa and examines the extent to which subsistence economy, age, and gender predicted how often children were observed engaging in work-themed play and characteristics of work-themed play. Overall, farmer children engaged in more work-themed play than forager children. Very few gender differences were observed in work-themed play. Age and subsistence economy predicted tendencies for children to be near adults while engaged in work-themed play and to use objects in their work-themed play.

Work-themed play among young children in foraging and farming communities in Central Africa

in Behaviour

Sections

References

AltmannJ. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. — Behaviour 49: 227-267.

BelskyJ.TaylorD.G.RovineM. (1984). The Pennsylvania infant and family development project, II: the development of reciprocal interaction in the mother–infant dyad. — Child Dev. 55: 706-717.

BlochM.N.AdlerS.M. (1994). African children’s play and the emergence of the sexual division of labor. — In: Children’s play in diverse cultures ( RoopnarineJ.L.JohnsonJ.E.HooperF.H. eds). SUNY PressAlbany, NY p.  148-178.

Blurton JonesN. (1993). The lives of hunter–gatherer children: effects of parental behavior and parental reproductive strategy. — In: Juvenile primates ( PereiraM.E.FairbanksL.A. eds). Oxford University PressNew York, NY p.  309-326.

BockJ. (2002). Learning, life history, and productivity. — Hum. Nat. 13: 161-197.

BockJ. (2005). Farming, foraging, and children’s play in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. — In: The nature of play: great apes and humans ( PellegriniA.D.SmithP.K. eds). Guilford PressNew York, NY p.  254-281.

BockJ.JohnsonS.E. (2004). Subsistence ecology and play among the Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana. — Hum. Nat. 15: 63-81.

BornsteinM.H.HaynesO.M.O’ReillyA.W.PainterK.M. (1996). Solitary and collaborative pretense play in early childhood: sources of individual variation in the development of representational competence. — Child Dev. 67: 2910-2929.

BoyetteA.H. (in press). Children’s play and culture learning in an egalitarian foraging society. — Child Dev.

BrethertonI. (1989). Pretense: the form and function of make-believe play. — Dev. Rev. 9: 383-401.

BurghardtG.M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. — MIT PressCambridge, MA.

DanskyJ.L. (1980). Make-believe: a mediator of the relationship between play and associative fluency. — Child Dev. 51: 576-579.

de LorimierS.DoyleA.-B.TessierO. (1995). Social coordination during pretend play: comparisons with nonpretend play and effects on expressive content. — Merrill-Palmer Q. 41: 497-516.

DraperP.CashdanE. (1988). Technological change and child behavior among the !Kung. — Ethnology 27: 339-365.

EdwardsC.P. (2000). Children’s play in cross-cultural perspective: a new look at the six cultures study. — Cross-Cult. Res. 34: 318-338.

FagenR. (1981). Animal play behavior. — Oxford University PressNew York, NY.

FeinG.G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: an integrative review. — Child Dev. 52: 1095-1118.

FoutsH.N. (2004). Social and emotional contexts of weaning among Bofi farmers and foragers. — Ethnology 43: 65-81.

FoutsH.N.LambM.E. (2009). Cultural and developmental variation in toddlers’ interactions with other children among two small-scale societies in Central Africa. — Eur. J. Dev. Sci. 3: 389-407.

FoutsH.N.HewlettB.S.LambM.E. (2005). Parent–offspring weaning conflicts among the Bofi farmers and foragers of Central Africa. — Curr. Anthropol. 46: 29-50.

FoutsH.N.HewlettB.S.LambM.E. (2012). A biocultural approach to breastfeeding interactions in Central Africa. — Am. Anthropol. 114: 123-136.

FoutsH.N.HallamR.A.PurandareS. (2013). Gender segregation in early-childhood social play among the Bofi foragers and Bofi farmers in Central Africa. — Am. J. Play 5: 333-356.

FracassoM.P.LambM.E.SchölmerichA.LeyendeckerB. (1997). The ecology of mother–infant interaction in Euro-American and immigrant Central American families living in the United States. — Int. J. Behav. Dev. 20: 207-217.

GaskinsS. (1999). Children’s daily lives in a Mayan village: a case study of culturally constructed roles and activities. — In: Children’s engagement in the world ( GöncüA. ed.). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  25-61.

GossoY.MoraisM.D.L.S.OttaE. (2007). Pretend play of Brazilian children: a window into different cultural worlds. — J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 38: 539-558.

GroosK. (1898). The play of animals. — AppletonNew York, NY.

HewlettB.S.BoyetteA.H. (2013). Play in hunter–gathers. — In: Evolution early experience and human development ( NarvaezD.PankseppJ.SchoreA.N.GleasonT.R. eds). Oxford University PressNew York, NY p.  388-393.

HewlettB.S.LambM.E.LeyendeckerD.SchölmerichA. (2000). Internal working models, trust, and sharing among foragers. — Curr. Anthropol. 41: 287-297.

HillK.HurtadoA.M. (1996). Ache life history. — Aldine de GruyterNew York, NY.

KameiN. (2005). Play among Baka children in Cameroon. — In: Hunter–gatherer childhoods ( HewlettB.S.LambM.E. eds). Transaction PublishersNew Brunswick, NJ p.  343-359.

KonnerM.J. (1972). Aspects of the developmental ethology of a foraging people. — In: Ethological studies of child behaviour ( Blurton JonesN. ed.). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  285-304.

LancyD.F. (1996). Playing on the motherground. — Guilford PressNew York, NY.

LancyD.F. (2002). Cultural constraints on children’s play. — In: Conceptual social-cognitive and contextual issues in the fields of play ( RoopnarineJ.L. ed.). AlbexWestport, CT p.  51-60.

LeyendeckerB.LambM.E.SchölmerichA. (1997a). Studying mother–infant interaction: the effects of context and length of observation in two subcultural groups. — Inf. Behav. Dev. 20: 325-337.

LeyendeckerB.LambM.E.SchölmerichA.FrickeD.M. (1997b). Contexts as moderators of observed interactions: a study of Costa Rican mothers and infants from differing socioeconomic backgrounds. — Int. J. Behav. Dev. 21: 15-34.

MartiniM. (1994). Peer interactions in Polynesia: a view from the Marquesas. — In: Children’s play in diverse cultures ( RoopnarineJ.JohnsonJ.HooperF. eds). SUNY PressAlbany, NY p.  73-103.

MorelliG.A.RogoffB.AngelilloC. (2003). Cultural variation in young children’s access to work or involvement in specialized child-focused activities. — Int. J. Behav. Dev. 27: 264-274.

MunroeR.H.MunroeR.L.ShimminH.S. (1984). Children’s work in four cultures: determinants and consequences. — Am. Anthropol. 86: 369-379.

PellegriniA.D. (2009). The role of play in human development. — Oxford University PressNew York, NY.

PiagetJ. (1951). Play dreams and imitation in childhood. — NortonNew York, NY.

RoopnarineJ.L.FoutsH.N.LambM.E.Lewis-ElliganT.Y. (2005). Mothers’ and fathers’ behaviors toward their 3- to 4-month-old infants in lower, middle, and upper socioeconomic African American families. — Dev. Psychol. 41: 723-732.

SmithP.K. (2005). Social and pretend play in children. — In: The nature of play: great apes and humans ( PellegriniA.D.SmithP.K. eds). Guilford PressNew York, NY p.  173-209.

WalkerR.HillK.KaplanH.McMillanG. (2002). Age dependency of strength, skill, and hunting ability among the Ache of Paraguay. — J. Hum. Evol. 42: 639-657.

WhitingB.EdwardsC.P. (1988). A cross-cultural analysis of sex differences in the behavior of children aged three through 11. — J. Soc. Psychol. 91: 171-188.

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 38 38 12
Full Text Views 146 146 63
PDF Downloads 12 12 8
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0