The metacommunication hypothesis asserts that some elements of play behaviour are associated with play elements borrowed from aggression and interpret these aggression-like elements as playful. Using data from free living Hanuman langurs (Semnopithecus entellus), we tested three predictions that follow from the metacommunication hypothesis: (i) aggression-like elements (ALEs) abbreviate play bouts; (ii) candidate signal elements are sequentially associated with ALEs; (iii) associations of candidate signal elements with ALEs prolong play bouts. Play face and five other candidate signal elements were evaluated in relation to nine ALEs. We confirmed all three predictions for play face, albeit only if the play face and/or the ALEs occurred at the start of the play bout. The other candidate elements were not associated with ALEs. We conclude that play face fulfils the metacommunicatory function in Hanuman langur play bouts, while other play specific elements may serve other signal or non-signal functions.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Bateson G. (1955). The message “this is play.”. — In: Group processes: transactions of the second conference ( Schaffner B., ed.). Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation, Madison, NJ, p. 145-242.
Bateson G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. — Balantine Books, New York, NY.
Bauer E.B., Smuts B.B. (2007). Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. — Anim. Behav. 73: 489-499.
Bekoff M. (1974). Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. — Am. Zool. 14: 323-340.
Bekoff M. (1975). The communication of play intention: are play signals functional? — Semiotica 15: 231-239.
Bekoff M. (1995). Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids. — Behaviour 132: 419-429.
Berger J. (1980). The ecology, structure and function of social play in bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis). — J. Zool. 192: 531-542.
Burghardt G.M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits, A Bradford Book. — The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Byers J.A. (1984). Play in ungulates. — In: Play in animals and humans ( Smith P.K., ed.). Blackwell, Oxford, p. 43-65.
Caro T.M. (1995). Short-term costs and correlates of play in cheetahs. — Anim. Behav. 49: 333-345.
Cordoni G., Palagi E. (2011). Ontogenetic trajectories of chimpanzee social play: similarities with humans. — PLoS ONE 6: e27344.
Demuru E., Ferrari P., Palagi E. (2014). Emotionality and intentionality in bonobo playful communication. — Anim. Cogn. 17: 1-12.
Donaldson T.M., Newberry R.C., Špinka M., Cloutier S. (2002). Effects of early play experience on play behaviour of piglets after weaning. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 79: 221-231.
Fagen R. (1984). Play and behavioural flexibility. — In: Play in animals and humans ( Smith P.K., ed.). Blackwell, Oxford, p. 159-173.
Himmler B.T., Kisko T.M., Euston D.R., Kolb B., Pellis S.M. (2014). Are 50-khz calls used as play signals in the playful interactions of rats? I. Evidence from the timing and context of their use. — Behav. Process. 106: 60-66.
Horowitz A. (2009). Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. — Anim. Cogn. 12: 107-118.
Kisko T.M., Himmler B.T., Himmler S.M., Euston D.R., Pellis S.M. (2015). Are 50-kHz calls used as play signals in the playful interactions of rats? II. Evidence from the effects of devocalization. — Behav. Process. 111: 25-33.
Liebal K., Call J., Tomasello M. (2004). Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees. — Am. J. Primatol. 64: 377-396.
Mancini G., Ferrari P.F., Palagi E. (2013). In play we trust. Rapid facial mimicry predicts the duration of playful interactions in geladas. — PLoS ONE 8: e66481.
Mitchell R.W. (1991). Bateson concept of metacommunication in play. — New Ideas Psychol. 9: 73-87.
Newberry R.C., Wood-Gush D.M.G., Hall J.W. (1988). Playful behaviour of piglets. — Behav. Process. 17: 205-216.
Palagi E. (2007). Play at work: revisiting data focusing on chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — J. Anthropol. Sci. 85: 63-81.
Palagi E. (2008). Sharing the motivation to play: the use of signals in adult bonobos. — Anim. Behav. 75: 887-896.
Palagi E. (2009). Adult play fighting and potential role of tail signals in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). — J. Comp. Psychol. 123: 1-9.
Palagi E., Mancini G. (2011). Playing with the face: playful facial “chattering” and signal modulation in a monkey species (Theropithecus gelada). — J. Comp. Psychol. 125: 11-21.
Palagi E., Burghardt G.M., Smuts B., Cordoni G., Dall’olio S., Fouts H.N., Řeháková-Petrů M., Siviy S.M., Pellis S.M. (2015). Rough-and-tumble play as a window on animal communication. — Biol. Rev., in press.
Pellegrini A.D., Pellegrini A.F.A. (2012). Play, plasticity, and ontogeny in childhood. — In: Evolution, early experience and human development: from research to practice and policy ( Narvaez D., Panksepp J., Schore A.N., Gleason T.R., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 339-351.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C. (1996). On knowing it’s only play: the role of play signals in play fighting. — Aggress. Viol. Behav. 1: 249-268.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C. (1997). Targets, tactics, and the open mouth face during play fighting in three species of primates. — Aggress. Behav. 23: 41-57.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C. (2011). To whom the play signal is directed: a study of headshaking in black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). — J. Comp. Psychol. 125: 1-10.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C., Reinhart C.J., Thierry B. (2011). The use of the bared-teeth display during play fighting in tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana): sometimes it is all about oneself. — J. Comp. Psychol. 125: 393-403.
Petrů M., Špinka M., Lhota S., Šípek P. (2008). Head rotations in the play of Hanuman langurs (Semnopithecus entellus): a description and an analysis of their function. — J. Comp. Psychol. 122: 9-18.
Petrů M., Špinka M., Charvátová V., Lhota S. (2009). Revisiting play elements and self-handicapping in play: a comparative ethogram of five old world monkey species. — J. Comp. Psychol. 123: 250-263.
Poole T.B. (1978). Analysis of social play in polecats (Mustelidae) with comments on form and evolutionary history of open mouth play face. — Anim. Behav. 26: 36-49.
Preuschoft S., van Hooff J.A.R.A.M. (1995). Homologizing primate facial displays: a critical review of methods. — Folia Primatol. 65: 121-137.
Reinhart C.J., Pellis V.C., Thierry B., Gauthier C.A., Vanderlaan D.P., Vasey P.L., Pellis S.M. (2010). Targets and tactics of play fighting: competitive versus cooperative styles of play in Japanese and Tonkean macaques. — Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 23: 166-200.
Renouf D. (1993). Play in a captive breeding colony of harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) — constrained by time or by energy. — J. Zool. 231: 351-363.
Sade D.S. (1973). An ethogram for rhesus monkeys. I. Antithetical contrast in posture and movement. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 38: 537-542.
Špinka M., Newberry R.C., Bekoff M. (2001). Mammalian play: training for the unexpected. — Q. Rev. Biol. 76: 141-168.
van Hooff J.A.R.A.M. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. — In: Nonverbal communication ( Hinde R.A., ed.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 209-241.
Waller B.M., Dunbar R.I.M. (2005). Differential behavioural effects of silent bared teeth display and relaxed open mouth display in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Ethology 111: 129-142.
Waller B.M., Micheletta J. (2013). Facial expression in nonhuman animals. — Emotion Rev. 5: 54-59.
Waller B.M., Caeiro C.C., Davila-Ross M. (2015). Orangutans modify facial displays depending on recipient attention. — PeerJ 3: e827.
Wilson S.C., Kleiman D.G. (1974). Eliciting play: a comparative study. — Am. Zool. 14: 341-370.
Yanagi A., Berman C.M. (2014a). Body signals during social play in free-ranging rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a systematic analysis. — Am. J. Primatol. 76: 168-179.
Yanagi A., Berman C.M. (2014b). Functions of multiple play signals in free-ranging juvenile rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). — Behaviour 151: 1983-2014.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 864 | 101 | 8 |
Full Text Views | 255 | 6 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 79 | 17 | 0 |
The metacommunication hypothesis asserts that some elements of play behaviour are associated with play elements borrowed from aggression and interpret these aggression-like elements as playful. Using data from free living Hanuman langurs (Semnopithecus entellus), we tested three predictions that follow from the metacommunication hypothesis: (i) aggression-like elements (ALEs) abbreviate play bouts; (ii) candidate signal elements are sequentially associated with ALEs; (iii) associations of candidate signal elements with ALEs prolong play bouts. Play face and five other candidate signal elements were evaluated in relation to nine ALEs. We confirmed all three predictions for play face, albeit only if the play face and/or the ALEs occurred at the start of the play bout. The other candidate elements were not associated with ALEs. We conclude that play face fulfils the metacommunicatory function in Hanuman langur play bouts, while other play specific elements may serve other signal or non-signal functions.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 864 | 101 | 8 |
Full Text Views | 255 | 6 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 79 | 17 | 0 |