Stone handling, an object play behaviour in macaques: welfare and neurological health implications of a bio-culturally driven tradition

in Behaviour
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Object play in primates is viewed as generally having no immediate functional purpose, limited for the most part to immature individuals. At the proximate level, the occurrence of object play in immatures is regarded as being intrinsically self-rewarding, with the ultimate function of supporting motoneuronal development and the acquisition of skills necessary to prepare them for survival as adults. Stone handling (SH), a solitary object play behaviour occurs, and has been studied, in multiple free-ranging and captive troops of provisioned Japanese macaques, as well as rhesus and long-tailed macaques for over 35 years now. A review of our combined findings from these observations reveal that infants acquire SH in the first 3-4 months of life and exhibit increasingly more complex and varied behavioural patterns with age. The longitudinal data shows that many individuals maintain this activity throughout life, practicing it under relaxed ecological and social conditions. The ultimate function may be bimodal, promoting motor development in young and neural maintenance and regeneration in adult and aging individuals.

Stone handling, an object play behaviour in macaques: welfare and neurological health implications of a bio-culturally driven tradition

in Behaviour

Sections

References

ArmandJ.OlivierE.EdgleyS.A.LemonR.N. (1996). The postnatal development of corticospinal projections from motor cortex to the cervical enlargement in the macaque monkey. — J. Neurosci. 17: 251-266.

BaldwinJ.D.BaldwinJ.I. (1977). The role of learning phenomena in the ontogeny of exploration and play. — In: Primate bio-social development ( Chevalier-SkolnikoffS.PoirierF.E. eds). GarlandNew York, NY p.  343-406.

BarberN. (1991). Play and energy regulation in mammals. — Q. Rev. Biol. 66: 129-147.

BatesonP.MendlM.FeaverJ. (1990). Play in the domestic cat is enhanced by rationing of the mother during lactation. — Anim. Behav. 40: 514-525.

BekoffM. (1995). Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids. — Behaviour 132: 419-429.

BekoffM.ByersJ.A. (1981). A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny and phylogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: an ethological hornets nest. — In: Behavioral development ( ImmelmannK.BarlowG.MainM.PetrinovichL. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  296-337.

BellH.C.McCaffreyD.ForgieM.L.KolbB.PellisS.M. (2009). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the play fighting in rats. — Behav. Neurosci. 123: 1158-1168.

BibenM. (1982). Object play and social treatment of prey in bush dogs and crab-eating foxes. — Behaviour 79: 201-211.

BoissyA.ManteuffelG.JensenM.B.OppermannM.R.SpruijtB.KeelingL.J.WincklerC.ForkanB.DimitrovI.LangbeinJ.BakkenM.VeisserI.AubertA. (2007). Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. — Physiol. Behav. 92: 375-397.

BortoffG.A.StrickP.L. (1993). Corticospinal terminations in two New World primates: further evidence that corticoneuronal connections provide part of the neural substrate for manual dexterity. — J. Neurosci. 13: 5105-5118.

BourgeoisJ.-P.Goldman-RakicP.S.RakicP. (1994). Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. — Cereb. Cortex 4: 78-96.

BurghardtG.M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. — MIT PressCambridge, MA.

BurtonF.D.BickM.J.A. (1972). A drift in time can define a deme: the implications of tradition drift in primate societies for hominid evolution. — J. Hum. Evol. 1: 53-59.

ByersJ.A.WalkerC. (1995). Refining the motor training hypothesis for the evolution of play. — Am. Nat. 146: 25-40.

CotmanC.W.BerchtoldN.C. (2002). Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. — Trends Neurosci. 25: 295-301.

CoyleJ.T. (2003). Use it or lose it — do effortful mental activities protect against dementia?New Engl. J. Med. 348: 2489-2490.

DarwishM.KorányiL.NyakasC.FerenzA. (2001). Induced social interactions reduces corticosterone stress response to anxiety in adult and aged rats. — Klin. Kisérl. Lab. Med. 28: 108-111.

DawkinsM.S. (2006). A user’s guide to animal welfare science. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 21: 77-82.

de WaalF.B.M. (2001). The ape and the sushi-master: cultural reflections of a primatologist. — Basic BooksNew York, NY.

DeanL.ValeG.LalandK.N.FlynnE.KendalR. (2014). Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. — Biol. Rev. 89: 284-301.

FagenR. (1981). Animal play behavior. — Oxford University PressOxford.

FairbanksL.A. (2000). The developmental timing of primate play: a neural selection model. — In: Biology brains and behavior: the evolution of human development ( ParkerS.T.LangerJ.McKinneyM.L. eds). School of American Research PressOxford p.  131-158.

FediganL. (1972). Social and solitary play in a colony of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). — Primates 13: 347-364.

FragaszyD.M.Adams-CurtisL.E. (1991). Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). — J. Comp. Psychol. 105: 387-397.

FraserD.DuncanI.J.H. (1998). ‘Pleasures’, ‘pains’ and animal welfare: toward a natural history of affect. — Anim. Welf. 7: 383-396.

GaleaM.P.Darian-SmithI. (1995). Postnatal maturation of the direct corticospinal projections in the macaque monkey. — Cereb. Cortex 5: 518-540.

GöncüA.GaskinsS. (2007). Play and development: evolutionary sociocultural and functional perspectives. — Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesLondon.

GrahamK.L.BurghardtG.M. (2010). Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress. — Q. Rev. Biol. 85: 393-418.

GroosK. (1898). The play of animals. — D. AppletonNew York, NY.

GroosK. (1901). The play of man. — D. AppletonNew York, NY.

GumertM.D.KluckM.MalaivijitnondS. (2007). The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone age and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region. — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 594-608.

GumertM.D.MalaivijitnondS. (2012). Marine prey processed with stone tools by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) in intertidal habitats. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 149: 447-457.

HallG.S. (1904). Adolescence: its psychology and its relations to physiology anthropology sociology sex crime religion and education. — D. AppletonNew York, NY.

HallS.L. (1998). Object play by adult animals. — In: Animal play: evolutionary comparative and ecological perspectives ( BekoffM.ByersJ.A. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  45-60.

HauserM.D. (1988). Invention and social transmission: new data from wild vervet monkeys. — In: Machiavelian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys apes and humans ( ByrneR.W.WhitenA. eds). Oxford University PressOxford p.  327-343.

HeldS.D.E.SpinkaM. (2011). Animal play and animal welfare. — Anim. Behav. 81: 891-899.

HiraiwaM. (1975). Pebble-collecting behavior by juvenile Japanese monkeys. — Monkey 19: 24-25 (in Japanese).

HofP.R.DuanH. (2001). Age-related morphologic alteration in the brain of Old World and New World anthropoid monkeys. — In: Functional neurobiology of aging ( HofP.R.MobbsC.V. eds). Academic PressSan Diego, CA p.  435-445.

HuffmanM.A. (1984). Stone-play of Macaca fuscata in Arashiyama B troop: transmission of a non-adaptive behavior. — J. Hum. Evol. 13: 725-735.

HuffmanM.A. (1991). History of Arashiyama Japanese macaques in Kyoto, Japan. — In: The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the West ( FediganL.M.AsquithP.J. eds). SUNY PressAlbany, NY p.  21-53.

HuffmanM.A. (1996). Acquisition of innovative cultural behaviors in non-human primates: a case study of stone handling, a socially transmitted behavior in Japanese macaques. — In: Social learning in animals: the roots of culture ( GalefB.HeyesC. eds). Academic PressOrlando, FL p.  267-289.

HuffmanM.A.QuiattD. (1986). Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): implications for tool use of stones. — Primates 27: 413-423.

HuffmanM.A.HirataS. (2003). Biological and ecological foundations of primate behavioral tradition. — In: The biology of tradition: models and evidence ( FragaszyD.M.PerryS. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  267-296.

HuffmanM.A.NahallageC.A.D.LecaJ.-B. (2008). Cultured monkeys, social learning cast in stones. — Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17: 410-414.

HuffmanM.A.LecaJ.-B.NahallageC.A.D. (2010). Cultured Japanese macaques: a multidisciplinary approach to stone handling behavior and its implications for the evolution of behavioral tradition in nonhuman primates. — In: The Japanese macaques ( NakagawaN.NakamichiM.SugiuraH. eds). SpringerTokyo p.  191-219.

JamanF.M.HuffmanM.A. (2008). Enclosure environment affects the activity budgets of captive Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 1133-1144.

JenningsP.J.JanowskyJ.S.OrwollE. (1998). Estrogen and sequential movement. — Behav. Neurosci. 112: 154-159.

JosephJ.A.KochmanK.RothG.S. (1989). Reduction of motor behavioural deficits in senescence via chronic prolactin or estrogen administration: time course and putative mechanisms of action. — Brain Res. 505: 195-202.

KerckhoffA.C. (2002). A theory of hysterical contagion. — Hum. Nat. Coll. Behav. 21: 81-93.

KritzerM.F.KohamaS.G. (1998). Ovarian hormones influence the morphology, distribution, and density of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactive axons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of adult rhesus monkeys. — J. Comp. Neurol. 395: 1-17.

KummerH.GoodallJ. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 308: 203-214.

KuypersH.G.J.M. (1981). Anatomy of the descending pathways. — In: Handbook of physiology: the nervous system II ( BrookhartJ.M.MountcastleV.B. eds). American Physiological SocietyBethesda, MD p.  597-666.

LacreuseA.DiehlM.M.GohM.Y.HallM.J.VolkA.M.ChhabraR.K.HerndonJ.G. (2005). Sex differences in age-related motor slowing in the rhesus monkey: behavioral and neuroimaging data. — Neurobiol. Aging 26: 543-551.

LaurinD.VerreaultM.D.LindsayJ.MacPhersonK.RockwoodK. (2001). Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. — Arch. Neurol. 58: 498-504.

LawrenceA. (1987). Consumer demand theory and the assessment of animal welfare. — Anim. Behav. 35: 293-295.

LawrenceD.G.HopkinsD.A. (1976). The development of motor control in rhesus monkeys: evidence concerning the role of corticomotoneuronal connections. — Brain 99: 235-254.

LecaJ.-B. (2012). Integrative approach to cultural behaviour: the case of stone handling in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Techn. Cult. 57: 142-163 (in French).

LecaJ.-B. (2015). Arbitrary cultural behavior patterns are not unique to humans. [Invited commentary on Nakagawa et al.]Curr. Anthropol. 56: 112-113.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2007a). Age-related differences in the performance, diffusion, and maintenance of stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques. — J. Hum. Evol. 53: 691-708.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2007b). Japanese macaque cultures: inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops. — Behaviour 144: 251-281.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2008a). Of stones and monkeys: testing ecological constraints on stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 135: 233-244.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2008b). Food provisioning and stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of ten troops. — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 803-813.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2010). Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Anim. Behav. 79: 117-126.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2011). Complexity in object manipulation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): a cross-sectional analysis of manual coordination in stone handling patterns. — J. Comp. Psychol. 125: 61-71.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.HuffmanM.A. (2012). Thirty years of stone handling tradition in Arashiyama-Kyoto macaques: implications for cumulative culture and tool use in non-human primates. — In: The monkeys of Stormy Mountain: 60 years of primatological research on the Japanese macaques of Arashiyama ( LecaJ.-B.HuffmanM.A.VaseyP.L. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  223-257.

LecaJ.-B.GunstN.Ottenheimer CarrierL.VaseyP.L. (2014). Inter-group variation in non-conceptive sexual activity in female Japanese macaques: could it be cultural?Anim. Behav. Cogn. 1: 387-409.

LewisK.P.BartonR.A. (2006). Amygdala size and hypothalamus size predict social play frequency in non-human primates: a comparative analysis using independent contrasts. — J. Comp. Psychol. 120: 31-37.

LockmanJ. (2000). A perception–action perspective on tool use development. — Child Dev. 71: 137-144.

MatsuzawaT.BiroD.HumleT.Inoue-NakamuraN.TonookaR.YamakoshiG. (2001). Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. — In: Primate origins of human cognition and behaviour ( MatsuzawaT. ed.). SpringerTokyo p.  557-574.

McGrewW.C.CollinsD.A. (1985). Tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to obtain termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. — Am. J. Primatol. 9: 47-62.

MitchellR.W. (2002). Pretending in animals children and adult humans. — Cambridge University PressCambridge.

MustoeA.C.TaylorJ.H.BirnieA.K.HuffmanM.C.FrenchJ.A. (2014). Gestational cortisol and social play shape development of marmosets’ HPA functioning and behavioral responses to stressors. — Dev. Psychobiol. 56: 1229-1243.

NahallageC.A.D.HuffmanM.A. (2007a). Acquisition and development of stone handling behavior in infant Japanese macaques. — Behaviour 144: 1193-1215.

NahallageC.A.D.HuffmanM.A. (2007b). Age-specific functions of stone handling, a solitary-object play behavior, in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Am. J. Primatol. 69: 267-281.

NahallageC.A.D.HuffmanM.A. (2008a). Proximate factors associated with the occurrence of stone handling behavior in a captive troop of Japanese macaques. — Int. J. Primatol. 29: 795-806.

NahallageC.A.D.HuffmanM.A. (2008b). Comparison of stone handling behavior in two macaque species: implications for the role of phylogeny and environment in primate cultural variation. — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 1124-1132.

NahallageC.A.D.HuffmanM.A. (2012). Stone handling behavior in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), a behavioral propensity for solitary object play shared with Japanese macaques. — Primates 53: 71-78.

NorsciaI.PalagiE. (2001). When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. — Primates 52: 101-104.

NunesS.MueckeE.-M.AnthonyJ.A.BatterbeeA.S. (1999). Endocrine and energetic mediation of play behavior in free-ranging Belding’s ground squirrels. — Horm. Behav. 36: 153-165.

OlivierE.EdgleyS.A.ArmandJ.LemonR.N. (1997). An electrophysiological study of the postnatal development of the corticospinal system in the macaque monkey. — J. Neurosci. 17: 267-276.

PalagiE.CordoniG.Borgonini TarliS.M. (2004). Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Ethology 110: 949-962.

ParkerS.T.GibsonK.R. (1977). Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. — J. Hum. Evol. 6: 623-641.

PellisS.M. (1991). How motivationally distinct is play? A preliminary case study. — Anim. Behav. 42: 851-853.

PellisS.M.PellisV.C.BellH.C. (2010). The function of play in the development of the social brain. — Am. J. Play 2: 278-296.

PereiraM.E.FairbanksL.A. (1993). Juvenile primates: life history development and behavior. — Oxford University PressOxford.

PhillipsC.G. (1971). Evolution of the corticospinal tract in primates with special reference to the hand. — In: Proceedings of the 3rd international congress in primatology. Karger Zurich p. 22-23.

PoirierF.E.BellisariA.HainesL. (1978). Functions of primate play behavior. — In: Social play in primates ( SmithE.O. ed.). Academic PressSan Diego, CA p.  143-168.

QuiattD.HuffmanM.A. (1993). On home bases, nesting sites, activity centers, and new analytic perspectives. — Curr. Anthropol. 34: 68-70.

RakicP.BourgeoisJ.-P.EckenhoffM.F.ZecevicN.Goldman-RakicP.S. (1986). Concurrent over-production of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. — Science 232: 232-235.

RamseyJ.K.McGrewW.C. (2005). Object play in great apes: studies in nature and captivity. — In: The nature of play: great apes and humans ( PellegriniA.D.SmithP.K. eds). Guilford PressNew York, NY p.  89-112.

ScheurerJ.ThierryB. (1985). A further food-washing tradition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Primates 26: 491-496.

SiviyS.M. (1998). Neurobiological substrates of play behavior: glimpses into the structure and function of mammalian playfulness. — In: Animal play: evolutionary comparative and ecological perspectives ( BekoffM.ByersJ.A. eds). Cambridge University PressCambridge p.  221-242.

SiviyS.M. (2010). Play and adversity: how the playful mammalian brain withstands threats and anxieties. — Am. J. Play 2: 297-314.

SmithE.O. (1978). A historical view on the study of play: statement of the problem. — In: Social play in primates ( SmithE.O. ed.). Academic PressNew York, NY p.  1-32.

ŠpinkaM.NewberryR.C.BekoffM. (2001). Play: training for the unexpected. — Q. Rev. Biol. 76: 141-168.

StarinE.D. (1990). Object manipulation by wild red colobus monkeys living in the Abuko Nature Reserve, the Gambia. — Primates 31: 385-391.

TaylorJ.H.MustoeA.C.HochfelderB.FrenchJ.A. (2015). Reunion behavior after social separation is associated with enhanced HPA recovery in young marmoset monkeys. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 57: 93-101.

ThierryB. (1994). Social transmission, tradition and culture in primates: from the epiphenomenon to the phenomenon. — Techn. Cult. 23/24: 91-119.

ThierryB. (2007). Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. — Evol. Anthropol. 16: 224-238.

VergheseJ.LiptonR.B.HallC.B.KuslanskyG.KatzM.J. (2003). Low blood pressure and the risk of dementia in very old individuals. — Neurology 61: 1667-1672.

VigS. (2007). Young children’s object play: a window on development. — J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 19: 201-215.

Von FrijtagJ.C.SchotM.van den BosR.SpruijtB.M. (2002). Individual housing during the play period results in changed responses to and consequences of a psychosocial stress situation in rats. — Dev. Psychobiol. 41: 58-69.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Stone handling troop site map in Japan. Site 2 holds a captive troop of Japanese macaques whereas provisioned free-ranging troops live at the other sites. Sources: sites 1, 3, 8, 10–12, Huffman & Hirata (2003); site 2, Nahallage & Huffman (2007a); site 4, Leca (2012); sites 5 and 7, Leca et al. (2007b); sites 6 and 9, Kunio Watanabe (personal communication).

  • View in gallery

    Initial transmission of SH behaviour from key innovator, 3.5 year-old female Glance 6476 to other kin-group members.

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 30 30 14
Full Text Views 93 93 60
PDF Downloads 7 7 3
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0