Stone handling, an object play behaviour in macaques: welfare and neurological health implications of a bio-culturally driven tradition

In: Behaviour
View More View Less
  • 1 Department of Sociology and Anthropology, University of Sri Jayewardenepura, Nugegoda, Sri Lanka
  • 2 Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, 4401 University Drive W, Lethbridge, AB, Canada T1K 6T5
  • 3 Department of Ecology and Social Behavior, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, 41-2 Kanrin, Inuyama, Aichi 484, Japan

Purchase instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):

€25.00$30.00

Object play in primates is viewed as generally having no immediate functional purpose, limited for the most part to immature individuals. At the proximate level, the occurrence of object play in immatures is regarded as being intrinsically self-rewarding, with the ultimate function of supporting motoneuronal development and the acquisition of skills necessary to prepare them for survival as adults. Stone handling (SH), a solitary object play behaviour occurs, and has been studied, in multiple free-ranging and captive troops of provisioned Japanese macaques, as well as rhesus and long-tailed macaques for over 35 years now. A review of our combined findings from these observations reveal that infants acquire SH in the first 3-4 months of life and exhibit increasingly more complex and varied behavioural patterns with age. The longitudinal data shows that many individuals maintain this activity throughout life, practicing it under relaxed ecological and social conditions. The ultimate function may be bimodal, promoting motor development in young and neural maintenance and regeneration in adult and aging individuals.

  • Armand J., Olivier E., Edgley S.A., Lemon R.N. (1996). The postnatal development of corticospinal projections from motor cortex to the cervical enlargement in the macaque monkey. — J. Neurosci. 17: 251-266.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baldwin J.D., Baldwin J.I. (1977). The role of learning phenomena in the ontogeny of exploration and play. — In: Primate bio-social development ( Chevalier-Skolnikoff S., Poirier F.E., eds). Garland, New York, NY, p.  343-406.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barber N. (1991). Play and energy regulation in mammals. — Q. Rev. Biol. 66: 129-147.

  • Bateson P., Mendl M., Feaver J. (1990). Play in the domestic cat is enhanced by rationing of the mother during lactation. — Anim. Behav. 40: 514-525.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bekoff M. (1995). Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids. — Behaviour 132: 419-429.

  • Bekoff M., Byers J.A. (1981). A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny and phylogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: an ethological hornets nest. — In: Behavioral development ( Immelmann K., Barlow G., Main M., Petrinovich L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  296-337.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bell H.C., McCaffrey D., Forgie M.L., Kolb B., Pellis S.M. (2009). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the play fighting in rats. — Behav. Neurosci. 123: 1158-1168.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Biben M. (1982). Object play and social treatment of prey in bush dogs and crab-eating foxes. — Behaviour 79: 201-211.

  • Boissy A., Manteuffel G., Jensen M.B., Oppermann M.R., Spruijt B., Keeling L.J., Winckler C., Forkan B., Dimitrov I., Langbein J., Bakken M., Veisser I., Aubert A. (2007). Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. — Physiol. Behav. 92: 375-397.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bortoff G.A., Strick P.L. (1993). Corticospinal terminations in two New World primates: further evidence that corticoneuronal connections provide part of the neural substrate for manual dexterity. — J. Neurosci. 13: 5105-5118.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bourgeois J.-P., Goldman-Rakic P.S., Rakic P. (1994). Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. — Cereb. Cortex 4: 78-96.

  • Burghardt G.M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. — MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

  • Burton F.D., Bick M.J.A. (1972). A drift in time can define a deme: the implications of tradition drift in primate societies for hominid evolution. — J. Hum. Evol. 1: 53-59.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Byers J.A., Walker C. (1995). Refining the motor training hypothesis for the evolution of play. — Am. Nat. 146: 25-40.

  • Cotman C.W., Berchtold N.C. (2002). Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. — Trends Neurosci. 25: 295-301.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coyle J.T. (2003). Use it or lose it — do effortful mental activities protect against dementia?New Engl. J. Med. 348: 2489-2490.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Darwish M., Korányi L., Nyakas C., Ferenz A. (2001). Induced social interactions reduces corticosterone stress response to anxiety in adult and aged rats. — Klin. Kisérl. Lab. Med. 28: 108-111.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dawkins M.S. (2006). A user’s guide to animal welfare science. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 21: 77-82.

  • de Waal F.B.M. (2001). The ape and the sushi-master: cultural reflections of a primatologist. — Basic Books, New York, NY.

  • Dean L., Vale G., Laland K.N., Flynn E., Kendal R. (2014). Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. — Biol. Rev. 89: 284-301.

  • Fagen R. (1981). Animal play behavior. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.

  • Fairbanks L.A. (2000). The developmental timing of primate play: a neural selection model. — In: Biology, brains and behavior: the evolution of human development ( Parker S.T., Langer J., McKinney M.L., eds). School of American Research Press, Oxford, p.  131-158.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fedigan L. (1972). Social and solitary play in a colony of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). — Primates 13: 347-364.

  • Fragaszy D.M., Adams-Curtis L.E. (1991). Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). — J. Comp. Psychol. 105: 387-397.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fraser D., Duncan I.J.H. (1998). ‘Pleasures’, ‘pains’ and animal welfare: toward a natural history of affect. — Anim. Welf. 7: 383-396.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Galea M.P., Darian-Smith I. (1995). Postnatal maturation of the direct corticospinal projections in the macaque monkey. — Cereb. Cortex 5: 518-540.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Göncü A., Gaskins S. (2007). Play and development: evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives. — Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, London.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Graham K.L., Burghardt G.M. (2010). Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress. — Q. Rev. Biol. 85: 393-418.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Groos K. (1898). The play of animals. — D. Appleton, New York, NY.

  • Groos K. (1901). The play of man. — D. Appleton, New York, NY.

  • Gumert M.D., Kluck M., Malaivijitnond S. (2007). The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone age and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region. — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 594-608.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gumert M.D., Malaivijitnond S. (2012). Marine prey processed with stone tools by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) in intertidal habitats. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 149: 447-457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hall G.S. (1904). Adolescence: its psychology and its relations to physiology, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime, religion and education. — D. Appleton, New York, NY.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hall S.L. (1998). Object play by adult animals. — In: Animal play: evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives ( Bekoff M., Byers J.A., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  45-60.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hauser M.D. (1988). Invention and social transmission: new data from wild vervet monkeys. — In: Machiavelian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans ( Byrne R.W., Whiten A., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p.  327-343.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Held S.D.E., Spinka M. (2011). Animal play and animal welfare. — Anim. Behav. 81: 891-899.

  • Hiraiwa M. (1975). Pebble-collecting behavior by juvenile Japanese monkeys. — Monkey 19: 24-25 (in Japanese).

  • Hof P.R., Duan H. (2001). Age-related morphologic alteration in the brain of Old World and New World anthropoid monkeys. — In: Functional neurobiology of aging ( Hof P.R., Mobbs C.V., eds). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p.  435-445.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A. (1984). Stone-play of Macaca fuscata in Arashiyama B troop: transmission of a non-adaptive behavior. — J. Hum. Evol. 13: 725-735.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A. (1991). History of Arashiyama Japanese macaques in Kyoto, Japan. — In: The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the West ( Fedigan L.M., Asquith P.J., eds). SUNY Press, Albany, NY, p.  21-53.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A. (1996). Acquisition of innovative cultural behaviors in non-human primates: a case study of stone handling, a socially transmitted behavior in Japanese macaques. — In: Social learning in animals: the roots of culture ( Galef B., Heyes C., eds). Academic Press, Orlando, FL, p.  267-289.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A., Quiatt D. (1986). Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): implications for tool use of stones. — Primates 27: 413-423.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A., Hirata S. (2003). Biological and ecological foundations of primate behavioral tradition. — In: The biology of tradition: models and evidence ( Fragaszy D.M., Perry S., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  267-296.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A., Nahallage C.A.D., Leca J.-B. (2008). Cultured monkeys, social learning cast in stones. — Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17: 410-414.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Huffman M.A., Leca J.-B., Nahallage C.A.D. (2010). Cultured Japanese macaques: a multidisciplinary approach to stone handling behavior and its implications for the evolution of behavioral tradition in nonhuman primates. — In: The Japanese macaques ( Nakagawa N., Nakamichi M., Sugiura H., eds). Springer, Tokyo, p.  191-219.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jaman F.M., Huffman M.A. (2008). Enclosure environment affects the activity budgets of captive Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 1133-1144.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jennings P.J., Janowsky J.S., Orwoll E. (1998). Estrogen and sequential movement. — Behav. Neurosci. 112: 154-159.

  • Joseph J.A., Kochman K., Roth G.S. (1989). Reduction of motor behavioural deficits in senescence via chronic prolactin or estrogen administration: time course and putative mechanisms of action. — Brain Res. 505: 195-202.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kerckhoff A.C. (2002). A theory of hysterical contagion. — Hum. Nat. Coll. Behav. 21: 81-93.

  • Kritzer M.F., Kohama S.G. (1998). Ovarian hormones influence the morphology, distribution, and density of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactive axons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of adult rhesus monkeys. — J. Comp. Neurol. 395: 1-17.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kummer H., Goodall J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 308: 203-214.

  • Kuypers H.G.J.M. (1981). Anatomy of the descending pathways. — In: Handbook of physiology: the nervous system II ( Brookhart J.M., Mountcastle V.B., eds). American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD, p.  597-666.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lacreuse A., Diehl M.M., Goh M.Y., Hall M.J., Volk A.M., Chhabra R.K., Herndon J.G. (2005). Sex differences in age-related motor slowing in the rhesus monkey: behavioral and neuroimaging data. — Neurobiol. Aging 26: 543-551.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laurin D., Verreault M.D., Lindsay J., MacPherson K., Rockwood K. (2001). Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. — Arch. Neurol. 58: 498-504.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lawrence A. (1987). Consumer demand theory and the assessment of animal welfare. — Anim. Behav. 35: 293-295.

  • Lawrence D.G., Hopkins D.A. (1976). The development of motor control in rhesus monkeys: evidence concerning the role of corticomotoneuronal connections. — Brain 99: 235-254.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B. (2012). Integrative approach to cultural behaviour: the case of stone handling in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Techn. Cult. 57: 142-163 (in French).

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B. (2015). Arbitrary cultural behavior patterns are not unique to humans. [Invited commentary on Nakagawa et al.]Curr. Anthropol. 56: 112-113.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2007a). Age-related differences in the performance, diffusion, and maintenance of stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques. — J. Hum. Evol. 53: 691-708.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2007b). Japanese macaque cultures: inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops. — Behaviour 144: 251-281.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2008a). Of stones and monkeys: testing ecological constraints on stone handling, a behavioral tradition in Japanese macaques. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 135: 233-244.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2008b). Food provisioning and stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of ten troops. — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 803-813.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2010). Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Anim. Behav. 79: 117-126.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2011). Complexity in object manipulation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): a cross-sectional analysis of manual coordination in stone handling patterns. — J. Comp. Psychol. 125: 61-71.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Huffman M.A. (2012). Thirty years of stone handling tradition in Arashiyama-Kyoto macaques: implications for cumulative culture and tool use in non-human primates. — In: The monkeys of Stormy Mountain: 60 years of primatological research on the Japanese macaques of Arashiyama ( Leca J.-B., Huffman M.A., Vasey P.L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  223-257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leca J.-B., Gunst N., Ottenheimer Carrier L., Vasey P.L. (2014). Inter-group variation in non-conceptive sexual activity in female Japanese macaques: could it be cultural?Anim. Behav. Cogn. 1: 387-409.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lewis K.P., Barton R.A. (2006). Amygdala size and hypothalamus size predict social play frequency in non-human primates: a comparative analysis using independent contrasts. — J. Comp. Psychol. 120: 31-37.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lockman J. (2000). A perception–action perspective on tool use development. — Child Dev. 71: 137-144.

  • Matsuzawa T., Biro D., Humle T., Inoue-Nakamura N., Tonooka R., Yamakoshi G. (2001). Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. — In: Primate origins of human cognition and behaviour ( Matsuzawa T., ed.). Springer, Tokyo, p.  557-574.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McGrew W.C., Collins D.A. (1985). Tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to obtain termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. — Am. J. Primatol. 9: 47-62.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mitchell R.W. (2002). Pretending in animals, children, and adult humans. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

  • Mustoe A.C., Taylor J.H., Birnie A.K., Huffman M.C., French J.A. (2014). Gestational cortisol and social play shape development of marmosets’ HPA functioning and behavioral responses to stressors. — Dev. Psychobiol. 56: 1229-1243.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nahallage C.A.D., Huffman M.A. (2007a). Acquisition and development of stone handling behavior in infant Japanese macaques. — Behaviour 144: 1193-1215.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nahallage C.A.D., Huffman M.A. (2007b). Age-specific functions of stone handling, a solitary-object play behavior, in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Am. J. Primatol. 69: 267-281.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nahallage C.A.D., Huffman M.A. (2008a). Proximate factors associated with the occurrence of stone handling behavior in a captive troop of Japanese macaques. — Int. J. Primatol. 29: 795-806.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nahallage C.A.D., Huffman M.A. (2008b). Comparison of stone handling behavior in two macaque species: implications for the role of phylogeny and environment in primate cultural variation. — Am. J. Primatol. 70: 1124-1132.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nahallage C.A.D., Huffman M.A. (2012). Stone handling behavior in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), a behavioral propensity for solitary object play shared with Japanese macaques. — Primates 53: 71-78.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norscia I., Palagi E. (2001). When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. — Primates 52: 101-104.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nunes S., Muecke E.-M., Anthony J.A., Batterbee A.S. (1999). Endocrine and energetic mediation of play behavior in free-ranging Belding’s ground squirrels. — Horm. Behav. 36: 153-165.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Olivier E., Edgley S.A., Armand J., Lemon R.N. (1997). An electrophysiological study of the postnatal development of the corticospinal system in the macaque monkey. — J. Neurosci. 17: 267-276.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Palagi E., Cordoni G., Borgonini Tarli S.M. (2004). Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Ethology 110: 949-962.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parker S.T., Gibson K.R. (1977). Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. — J. Hum. Evol. 6: 623-641.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pellis S.M. (1991). How motivationally distinct is play? A preliminary case study. — Anim. Behav. 42: 851-853.

  • Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C., Bell H.C. (2010). The function of play in the development of the social brain. — Am. J. Play 2: 278-296.

  • Pereira M.E., Fairbanks L.A. (1993). Juvenile primates: life history, development, and behavior. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.

  • Phillips C.G. (1971). Evolution of the corticospinal tract in primates with special reference to the hand. — In: Proceedings of the 3rd international congress in primatology. Karger, Zurich, p. 22-23.

  • Poirier F.E., Bellisari A., Haines L. (1978). Functions of primate play behavior. — In: Social play in primates ( Smith E.O., ed.). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p.  143-168.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quiatt D., Huffman M.A. (1993). On home bases, nesting sites, activity centers, and new analytic perspectives. — Curr. Anthropol. 34: 68-70.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rakic P., Bourgeois J.-P., Eckenhoff M.F., Zecevic N., Goldman-Rakic P.S. (1986). Concurrent over-production of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. — Science 232: 232-235.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ramsey J.K., McGrew W.C. (2005). Object play in great apes: studies in nature and captivity. — In: The nature of play: great apes and humans ( Pellegrini A.D., Smith P.K., eds). Guilford Press, New York, NY, p.  89-112.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Scheurer J., Thierry B. (1985). A further food-washing tradition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). — Primates 26: 491-496.

  • Siviy S.M. (1998). Neurobiological substrates of play behavior: glimpses into the structure and function of mammalian playfulness. — In: Animal play: evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives ( Bekoff M., Byers J.A., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  221-242.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Siviy S.M. (2010). Play and adversity: how the playful mammalian brain withstands threats and anxieties. — Am. J. Play 2: 297-314.

  • Smith E.O. (1978). A historical view on the study of play: statement of the problem. — In: Social play in primates ( Smith E.O., ed.). Academic Press, New York, NY, p.  1-32.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Špinka M., Newberry R.C., Bekoff M. (2001). Play: training for the unexpected. — Q. Rev. Biol. 76: 141-168.

  • Starin E.D. (1990). Object manipulation by wild red colobus monkeys living in the Abuko Nature Reserve, the Gambia. — Primates 31: 385-391.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Taylor J.H., Mustoe A.C., Hochfelder B., French J.A. (2015). Reunion behavior after social separation is associated with enhanced HPA recovery in young marmoset monkeys. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 57: 93-101.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thierry B. (1994). Social transmission, tradition and culture in primates: from the epiphenomenon to the phenomenon. — Techn. Cult. 23/24: 91-119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thierry B. (2007). Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. — Evol. Anthropol. 16: 224-238.

  • Verghese J., Lipton R.B., Hall C.B., Kuslansky G., Katz M.J. (2003). Low blood pressure and the risk of dementia in very old individuals. — Neurology 61: 1667-1672.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vig S. (2007). Young children’s object play: a window on development. — J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 19: 201-215.

  • Von Frijtag J.C., Schot M., van den Bos R., Spruijt B.M. (2002). Individual housing during the play period results in changed responses to and consequences of a psychosocial stress situation in rats. — Dev. Psychobiol. 41: 58-69.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 276 150 11
Full Text Views 184 11 1
PDF Downloads 31 10 0