There is currently substantial evidence indicating that oxytocin, a hypothalamus neuropeptide, modulates many forms of social behaviour and cognition in both human and non-human animals. The vast majority of animal research, however, has concentrated on maternal attachment and reproductive pair-bonds. In order to understand the neurochemical foundations of peaceful associations and sociality, oxytocin’s contribution to other types of social bonds, as well as to individual variation in sociality, should also be explored. Here, we summarise the most current studies that have investigated oxytocin’s role in regulating stable peaceful associations not directly related to mating. We also provide an overview on oxytocin’s role in support of specific social structures, and propose a novel research approach to evaluate the relationship between individual variation in social tendencies and variation in the oxytociergic system. We conclude by discussing avenues of future investigation in the biological substrates of sociality.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Adkins-Regan E. (2005). Hormones and social behavior. — Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Anacker A.M.J., Beery A.K. (2013). Life in groups: the roles of oxytocin in mammalian sociality. — Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7: 185.
Aplin L.M., Farine D.R., Morand-Ferron J., Sheldon B.C. (2012). Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: 4199-4205.
Auyeung B., Lombardo M.V., Heinrichs M., Chakrabarti B., Sule A., Deakin J.B., Bethlehem R.A.I., Dickens L., Mooney N., Sipple J.A.N. (2015). Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. — Transl. Psychol. 5: e507.
Bales K.L., Perkeybile A.M., Conley O.G., Lee M.H., Guoynes C.D., Downing G.M., Yun C.R., Solomon M., Jacob S., Mendoza S.P. (2013). Chronic intranasal oxytocin causes long-term impairments in partner preference formation in male prairie voles. — Biol. Psychol. 74: 180-188.
Barraza J.A., Grewal N.S., Ropacki S., Perez P., Gonzalez A., Zak P.J. (2013). Effects of a 10-day oxytocin trial in older adults on health and well-being. — Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 21: 85-92.
Bartz J.A., Lydon J.E., Kolevzon A., Zaki J., Hollander E., Ludwig N., Bolger N. (2015). Differential effects of oxytocin on agency and communion for anxiously and avoidantly attached individuals. — Psychol. Sci. 26: 1177-1186.
Bartz J.A., Zaki J., Bolger N., Hollander E., Ludwig N.N., Kolevzon A., Ochsner K.N. (2010). Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. — Psychol. Sci. 21: 1426-1428.
Bartz J.A., Zaki J., Bolger N., Ochsner K.N. (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. — Trends Cogn. Sci. 15: 301-309.
Beery A.K., Lacey E.A., Francis D.D. (2008). Oxytocin and vasopressin receptor distributions in a solitary and a social species of tuco-tuco (Ctenomys haigi and Ctenomys sociabilis). — J. Comp. Neurol. 507: 1847-1859.
Beery A.K., Zucker I. (2010). Oxytocin and same-sex social behavior in female meadow voles. — Neuroscience 169: 665-673.
Borgatti S.P., Mehra A., Brass D.J., Labianca G. (2009). Network analysis in the social sciences. — Science 323: 892-895.
Bourke A.F.G. (2011). Principles of social evolution. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Brent L.J.N. (2015). Friends of friends: are indirect connections in social networks important to animal behaviour? — Anim. Behav. 103: 211-222.
Brent L.J.N., Chang S.W.C., Gariépy J.F., Platt M.L. (2014). The neuroethology of friendship. — Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1316: 1-17.
Brent L.J.N., Heilbronner S.R., Horvath J.E., Gonzalez-Martinez J., Ruiz-Lambides A., Robinson A.G., Skene J.H.P., Platt M.L. (2013). Genetic origins of social networks in rhesus macaques. — Sci. Rep. 3: 1042.
Brosnan S.F., Talbot C.F., Essler J.L., Leverett K., Flemming T., Dougall P., Heyler C., Zak P.J. (2015). Oxytocin reduces food sharing in capuchin monkeys by modulating social distance. — Behaviour 152: 941-961.
Cameron E.Z., Setsaas T.H., Linklater W.L. (2009). Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106: 13850-13853.
Campbell A. (2008). Attachment, aggression and affiliation: the role of oxytocin in female social behavior. — Biol. Psychol. 77: 1-10.
Carter C.S., Grippo A.J., Pournajafi-Nazarloo H., Ruscio M.G., Porges S.W. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin and sociality. — Progr. Brain Res. 170: 331-336.
Carter G.G., Wilkinson G.S. (2015). Intranasal oxytocin increases social grooming and food sharing in the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus. — Horm. Behav. 75: 150-153.
Chang S.W.C., Barter J.W., Ebitz B., Watson K.K., Platt M. (2012). Inhaled oxytocin amplifies both vicarious reinforcement and self reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109: 959-964.
Crockford C., Deschner T., Ziegler T.E., Wittig R.M. (2014). Endogenous peripheral oxytocin measures can give insight into the dynamics of social relationships: a review. — Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8: 68.
Crockford C., Wittig R.M., Langergraber K.E., Ziegler T.E., Zuberbuhler K., Deschner T. (2013). Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 280: 20122765.
Dal Monte O., Noble P.L., Costa V.D., Averbeck B.B. (2014). Oxytocin enhances attention to the eye region in rhesus monkeys. — Front. Neurosci. 8: 41.
Dantzer R., Bluthe R.M., Koob G.F., Le Moal M. (1987). Modulation of social memory in male rats by neurohypophyseal peptides. — Psychopharmacology 91: 363-368.
de Dreu C.K.W. (2012). Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: an integrative review and research agenda. — Horm. Behav. 61: 419-428.
de Dreu C.K.W., Greer L.L., Handgraaf M.J.J., Shalvi S., Van Kleef G.A. (2011). Oxytocin modulates selection of allies in intergroup conflict. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: rspb20111444.
de Dreu C.K.W., Greer L.L., Handgraaf M.J.J., Shalvi S., van Kleef G.A., Baas M., Velden F.S.T., van Dijk E., Feith S.W.W. (2010). The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. — Science 328: 1408-1411.
de Oliveira D.C.G., Zuardi A.W., Graeff F.G., Queiroz R.H.C., Crippa J.A.S. (2012). Anxiolytic-like effect of oxytocin in the simulated public speaking test. — J. Psychopharmacol. 26: 497-504.
Declerck C.H., Boone C., Kiyonari T. (2010). Oxytocin and cooperation under conditions of uncertainty: the modulating role of incentives and social information. — Horm. Behav. 57: 368-374.
Declerck C.H., Boone C., Kiyonari T. (2013). The effect of oxytocin on cooperation in a prisoner’s dilemma depends on the social context and a person’s social value orientation. — Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. DOI:10.1093/scan/nst040.
Dingemanse N.J., Wolf M. (2010). Recent models for adaptive personality differences: a review. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 365: 3947-3958.
Domes G., Heinrichs M., Gläscher J., Büchel C., Braus D.F., Herpertz S.C. (2007a). Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. — Biol. Psychol. 62: 1187-1190.
Domes G., Heinrichs M., Michel A., Berger C., Herpertz S.C. (2007b). Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. — Biol. Psychol. 61: 731-733.
Donaldson Z.R., Young L.J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. — Science 322: 900-904.
Ebitz B., Watson K.K., Platt M.L. (2013). Oxytocin blunts social vigilance in the rhesus macaque. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 110: 11630-11635.
Estevez I., Andersen I.-L., Nævdal E. (2007). Group size, density and social dynamics in farm animals. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 103: 185-204.
Feifel D., MacDonald K., McKinney R., Heisserer N., Serrano V. (2010). A randomized, placebo-controlled investigation of intranasal oxytocin in patients with anxiety. — Neuropsychopharmacology 36: S324-S449.
Feldman R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. — Horm. Behav. 61: 380-391.
Ferguson J.N., Aldag J.M., Insel T.R., Young L.J. (2001). Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. — J. Neurosci. 21: 8278-8285.
Ferguson J.N., Young L.J., Hearn E.F., Matzuk M.M., Insel T.R., Winslow J.T. (2000). Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. — Nature Genet. 25: 284-288.
Gamer M., Zurowski B., Buchel C. (2010). Different amygdala sub-regions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107: 9400-9405.
Gimpl G., Fahrenholz F. (2001). The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function and regulation. — Physiol. Rev. 81: 629-683.
Goodson J.L. (2013). Deconstructing sociality, social evolution and relevant nonapeptide functions. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 38: 465-478.
Goodson J.L., Thompson R.R. (2010). Nonapeptide mechanisms of social cognition, behavior and species-specific social systems. — Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20: 784-794.
Gruber C.W. (2014). Physiology of invertebrate oxytocin and vasopressin neuropeptides. — Exp. Physiol. 99: 55-61.
Guastella A.J., MacLeod C. (2012). A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: evidence and future directions. — Horm. Behav. 61: 410-418.
Hamilton W.D. (1971). Geometry for the selfish herd. — J. Theor. Biol. 31: 295-311.
Heinrichs M., Baumgartner T., Kirschbaum C., Ehlert U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. — Biol. Psychol. 54: 1389-1398.
Heinrichs M., von Dawans B., Domes G. (2009). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. — Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30: 548-557.
Holt-Lunstad J., Birmingham W.A., Light K.C. (2008). Influence of a “warm touch” support enhancement intervention among married couples on ambulatory blood pressure, oxytocin, alpha amylase, and cortisol. — Psychosomat. Med. 70: 976-985.
Holt-Lunstad J., Smith T.B., Layton J.B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. — PLoS Med. 7: e1000316.
Insel T.R. (2010). The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. — Neuron 65: 768-779.
Insel T.R., Shapiro L.E. (1992). Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89: 5981-5985.
Insel T.R., Gelhard R., Shapiro L.E. (1991). The comparative distribution of forebrain receptors for neurohypophyseal peptides in monogamous and polygamous mice. — Neuroscience 43: 623-630.
Kalamatianos T., Faulkes C.G., Oosthuizen M.K., Poorun R., Bennett N.C., Coen C.W. (2010). Telencephalic binding sites for oxytocin and social organization: a comparative study of eusocial naked mole-rats and solitary cape mole-rats. — J. Comp. Neurol. 518: 1792-1813.
Kirsch P., Esslinger C., Chen Q., Mier D., Lis S., Siddhanti S., Gruppe H., Mattay V.S., Gallhofer B., Meyer-Lindenberg A. (2005). Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. — J. Neurosci. 25: 11489-11493.
Klatt J.D., Goodson J.L. (2013). Oxytocin-like receptors mediate pair bonding in a socially monogamous songbird. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 280: 20122396.
Kosfeld M., Heinrichs M., Zak P.J., Fischbacher U., Fehr E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. — Nature 435: 673-676.
Krause J., Ruxton G.D. (2002). Living in groups. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Labuschagne I., Phan K.L., Wood A., Angstadt M., Chua P., Heinrichs M., Stout J.C., Nathan P.J. (2010). Oxytocin attenuates amygdala reactivity to fear in generalized social anxiety disorder. — Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 2403-2413.
Lea A.J., Blumstein D.T., Wey T.W., Martin J.G.A. (2010). Heritable victimization and the benefits of agonistic relationships. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107: 21587-21592.
Lehmann J., Andrews K., Dunbar R.I.M. (2010). Social networks and social complexity in female-bonded primates. — In: Social brain, distributed mind ( Dunbar R.I.M., Gamble C., Gowlett J.A., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 57-83.
Leng G., Onaka T., Caquineau C., Sabatier N., Tobin V.A., Takayanagi Y. (2008). Oxytocin and appetite. — Progr. Brain Res. 170: 137-151.
Lim M.M., Young L.J. (2006). Neuropeptidergic regulation of affiliative behavior and social bonding in animals. — Horm. Behav. 50: 506-517.
Liu N., Hadj-Bouziane F., Jones K.B., Turchi J.N., Averbeck B.B., Ungerleider L.G. (2015). Oxytocin modulates fMRI responses to facial expression in macaques. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112: E3123-E3130.
Lukas M., Toth I., Reber S.O., Slattery D.A., Veenema A.H., Neumann I.D. (2011). The neuropeptide oxytocin facilitates pro-social behavior and prevents social avoidance in rats and mice. — Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 2159-2168.
MacDonald K., Feifel D. (2013). Helping oxytocin deliver: considerations in the development of oxytocin-based therapeutics for brain disorders. — Front. Neurosci. 7: 35.
Madden J.R., Clutton-Brock T.H. (2011). Experimental peripheral administration of oxytocin elevates a suite of cooperative behaviours in a wild social mammal. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 278: 1189-1194.
Madison D.M., FitzGerald R.W., McShea W.J. (1984). Dynamics of social nesting in overwintering meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus): possible consequences for population cycling. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 15: 9-17.
McCann S.M., Antunes-Rodrigues J., Jankowski M., Gutkowska J. (2002). Oxytocin, vasopressin and atrial natriuretic peptide control body fluid homeostasis by action on their receptors in brain, cardiovascular system and kidney. — Progr. Brain Res. 139: 309-328.
McCarthy M.M., McDonald C.H., Brooks P.J., Goldman D. (1996). An anxiolytic action of oxytocin is enhanced by estrogen in the mouse. — Physiol. Behav. 60: 1209-1215.
Mikolajczak M., Pinon N., Lane A., de Timary P., Luminet O. (2015). Oxytocin not only increases trust when money is at stake, but also when confidential information is in the balance. — Biol. Psychol. 85: 182-184.
Morhenn V.B., Park J.W., Piper E., Zak P.J. (2008). Monetary sacrifice among strangers is mediated by endogenous oxytocin release after physical contact. — Evol. Hum. Behav. 29: 375-383.
Nagasawa M., Kikusui T., Onaka T., Ohta M. (2009). Dog’s gaze at its owner increases owner’s urinary oxytocin during social interaction. — Horm. Behav. 55: 174-180.
Nagasawa M., Mitsui S., En S., Ohtani N., Ohta M., Sakuma Y., Onaka T., Mogi K., Kikusui T. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. — Science 348: 333-336.
Neumann I.D. (2009). The advantage of social living: brain neuropeptides mediate the beneficial consequences of sex and motherhood. — Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30: 483-496.
Neumann I.D., Landgraf R. (2012). Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. — Trends Neurosci. 35: 649-659.
Neumann I.D., Torner L., Wigger A. (2000a). Brain oxytocin: differential inhibition of neuroendocrine stress responses and anxiety-related behaviour in virgin, pregnant and lactating rats. — Neuroscience 95: 567-575.
Neumann I.D., Wigger A., Torner L., Holsboer F., Landgraf R. (2000b). Brain oxytocin inhibits basal and stress-induced activity of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis in male and female rats: partial action within the paraventricular nucleus. — J. Neuroendocrinol. 12: 235-244.
Norman G.J., Cacioppo J.T., Morris J.S., Malarkey W.B., Berntson G.G., DeVries A.C. (2011). Oxytocin increases autonomic cardiac control: moderation by loneliness. — Biol. Psychol. 86: 174-180.
Odendaal J.S.J., Meintjes R.A. (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behavior between humans and dogs. — Vet. J. 165: 296-301.
Oldfield R.G., Hofmann H.A. (2011). Neuropeptide regulation of social behavior in a monogamous cichlid fish. — Physiol. Behav. 102: 296-303.
Olff M., Frijling J.L., Kubzansky L.D., Bradley B., Ellenbogen M.A., Cardoso C., Bartz J.A., Yee J.R., van Zuiden M. (2013). The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: an update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 38: 1883-1894.
Ophir A.G., Gessel A., Zheng D.J., Phelps S.M. (2012). Oxytocin receptor density is associated with male mating tactics and social monogamy. — Horm. Behav. 61: 445-453.
Parker K.J., Buckmaster C.L., Schatzberg A.F., Lyons D.M. (2005). Intranasal oxytocin administration attenuates the ACTH stress response in monkeys. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 30: 924-929.
Parr L.A., Modi M., Siebert E., Young L.J. (2013). Intranasal oxytocin selectively attenuates rhesus monkeys’ attention to negative facial expressions. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 38: 1748-1756.
Pedersen A., Tomaszycki M.L. (2012). Oxytocin antagonist treatments alter the formation of pair relationships in zebra finches of both sexes. — Horm. Behav. 62: 113-119.
Popik P., van Ree J.M. (1991). Oxytocin but not vasopressin facilitates social recognition following injection into the medial preoptic area of the rat brain. — Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1: 555-560.
Prato-Previde E., Custance D.M., Spiezio C., Sabatini F. (2003). Is the dog-human relationship an attachment bond? An observational study using Ainsworth’s strange situation. — Behaviour 140: 225-254.
Pulliam H.R. (1973). On the advantages of flocking. — J. Theor. Biol. 38: 419-422.
Rilling J.K., Young L.J. (2014). The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. — Science 345: 771-776.
Ring R.H., Malberg J.E., Potestio L., Ping J., Boikess S., Luo B., Schechter L.E., Rizzo S., Rahman Z., Rosenzweig-Lipson S. (2006). Anxiolytic-like activity of oxytocin in male mice: behavioral and autonomic evidence, therapeutic implications. — Psychopharmacology 185: 218-225.
Romero T. (2015). Proximate mechaniss underlying cooperation in carnivores. — Jpn. J. Anim. Psychol. 65: 35-43.
Romero T., Konno A., Hasegawa T. (2013). Familiarity bias and physiological responses in contagious yawning by dogs support link to empathy. — PLoS ONE 8: e71365.
Romero T., Nagasawa M., Mogi K., Hasegawa T., Kikusui T. (2014). Oxytocin promotes social bonding in dogs. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111: 9085-9090.
Romero T., Nagasawa M., Mogi K., Hasegawa T., Kikusui T. (2015). Intranasal administration of oxytocin promotes social play in domestic dogs. — Communic. Integrat. Biol. 8: e1017157.
Rosenblum L.A., Smith E.L.P., Altemus M., Scharf B.A., Owens M.J., Nemeroff C.B., Gorman J.M., Coplan J.D. (2002). Differing concentrations of corticotropin-releasing factor and oxytocin in the cerebrospinal fluid of bonnet and pigtail macaques. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 27: 651-660.
Ross H.E., Young L.J. (2009). Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. — Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30: 534-547.
Rotzinger S., Lovejoy D.A., Tan L.A. (2010). Behavioral effects of neuropeptides in rodent models of depression and anxiety. — Peptides 31: 736-756.
Savaskan E., Ehrhardt R., Schulz A., Walter M., Schächinger H. (2008). Post-learning intranasal oxytocin modulates human memory for facial identity. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 33: 368-374.
Seyfarth R.M., Cheney D.L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. — Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63: 153-177.
Seyfarth R.M., Silk J.B., Cheney D.L. (2012). Variation in personality and fitness in wild female baboons. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109: 16980-16985.
Shahrestani S., Kemp A.H., Guastella A.J. (2013). The impact of a single administration of intranasal oxytocin on the recognition of basic emotions in humans: a meta-analysis. — Neuropsychopharmacology 38: 1929-1936.
Sih A., Bell A., Johnson J.C. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 19: 372-378.
Silk J.B., Alberts S.C., Altmann J. (2003). Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. — Science 302: 1331-1334.
Smith A.S., Ågmo A., Birnie A.K., French J.A. (2010). Manipulation of the oxytocin system alters social behavior and attraction in pair-bonding primates, Callithrix penicillata. — Horm. Behav. 57: 255-262.
Soares M.C., Bshary R., Fusani L., Goymann W., Hau M., Hirschenhauser K., Oliveira R.F. (2010). Hormonal mechanisms of cooperative behaviour. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 365: 2737-2750.
Stoop R. (2012). Neuromodulation by oxytocin and vasopressin. — Neuron 76: 142-159.
Topal J., Miklosi A., Csanyi V., Doka A. (1998). Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) strange situation test. — J. Comp. Psychol. 3: 219-229.
Turner R.A., Altemus M., Enos T., Cooper B., McGuinness T. (1999). Preliminary research on plasma oxytocin in normal cycling women: investigating emotion and interpersonal distress. — Psychiatry 62: 97-113.
van Anders S.M., Goodson J.L., Kingsbury M.A. (2013). Beyond “oxytocin = good”: neural complexities and the flipside of social bonds. — Arch. Sex. Behav. 42: 1115-1118.
Van Schaik C.P., Kappeler P.M. (1997). Infanticide risk and the evolution of male–female association in primates. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 264: 1687-1694.
Weidt A., Hofmann S.E., Konig B. (2008). Not only mate choice matters: fitness consequences of social partner choice in female house mice. — Anim. Behav. 75: 801-808.
Weinstein T.A.R., Bales K.L., Maninger N., Hostetler C.M., Capitanio J.P. (2014). Early involvement in friendships predicts later plasma concentrations of oxytocin and vasopressin in juvenile rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). — Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8: 295.
Windle R.J., Shanks N., Lightman S.L., Ingram C.D. (1997). Central oxytocin administration reduces stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety behavior in rats. — Endocrinology 138: 2829-2834.
Wittig R.M., Crockford C., Deschner T., Langergraber K.E., Ziegler T.E., Zuberbühler K. (2014). Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 281: 20133096.
Yee J.R., Cavigelli S.A., Delgado B., McClintock M. (2008). Reciprocal affiliation among adolescent rats during a mild group stressor predicts mammary tumors and lifespan. — Psychosomat. Med. 70: 1050-1059.
Young C., Majolo B., Heistermann M., Schülke O., Ostner J. (2014). Responses to social and environmental stress are attenuated by strong male bonds in wild macaques. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111: 18195-18200.
Zak P.J., Barraza J.A. (2013). The neurobiology of collective action. — Front. Neurosci. 7: 211.
Zak P.J., Stanton A.A., Ahmadi S.M. (2007). Oxytocin increases generosity in humans. — PLoS ONE 2: e1128.
Zink C.F., Meyer-Lindenberg A. (2012). Human neuroimaging of oxytocin and vasopressin in social cognition. — Horm. Behav. 61: 400-409.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 7401 | 494 | 23 |
Full Text Views | 416 | 26 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 110 | 25 | 0 |
There is currently substantial evidence indicating that oxytocin, a hypothalamus neuropeptide, modulates many forms of social behaviour and cognition in both human and non-human animals. The vast majority of animal research, however, has concentrated on maternal attachment and reproductive pair-bonds. In order to understand the neurochemical foundations of peaceful associations and sociality, oxytocin’s contribution to other types of social bonds, as well as to individual variation in sociality, should also be explored. Here, we summarise the most current studies that have investigated oxytocin’s role in regulating stable peaceful associations not directly related to mating. We also provide an overview on oxytocin’s role in support of specific social structures, and propose a novel research approach to evaluate the relationship between individual variation in social tendencies and variation in the oxytociergic system. We conclude by discussing avenues of future investigation in the biological substrates of sociality.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 7401 | 494 | 23 |
Full Text Views | 416 | 26 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 110 | 25 | 0 |