The concept of peace, with its corollary of behaviours, strategies and social implications, is commonly believed as a uniquely human feature. Through a comparative approach, we show how social play in animals may have paved the way for the emergence of peace. By playing fairly, human and nonhuman animals learn to manage their social dynamics in a more relaxed and tolerant way that results in a more effective management of conflicts. We show that play promotes tolerance, cooperation, fairness and reciprocity, which are essential elements of the so-called positive peace. This kind of peace is reached through an evolving process in which individuals continually modify social relationships to attain peaceful coexistence. In conclusion, we assume that the concept of peace has deep biological roots that constitute the basis for more sophisticated cultural constructions.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Adolphs R. (1999). The human amygdala and emotion. — Neuroscientist 5: 125-137.
Aldis O. (1975). Play fighting. — Academic Press, New York, NY.
Altmann S.A. (1962). A field study of the sociobiology of rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta. — Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 102: 338-435.
Anderson J.R., Myowa-Yamakoshi M., Matsuzawa T. (2004). Contagious yawning in chimpanzees. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 271: S468-S470.
Antonacci D., Norscia I., Palagi E. (2010). Stranger to familiar: wild strepsirhines manage xenophobia by playing. — PLoS One 5: e13218.
Aureli F., de Waal F.B.M. (2000). Natural conflict resolution. — University of California Press, Berkeley, CA.
Aureli F., Cords M., Van Schaik C.P. (2002). Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. — Anim. Behav. 64: 325-343.
Baan C., Bergmüller R., Smith D.W., Molnar B. (2014). Conflict management in free-ranging wolves, Canis lupus. — Anim. Behav. 90: 327-334.
Bartels A., Zeki S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. — Neuroreport 11: 3829-3834.
Behncke I. (2015). Play in the Peter Pan ape. — Curr. Biol. 25: R24-R27.
Bekoff M. (1974). Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. — Am. Zool. 14: 323-340.
Bekoff M. (1975). The communication of play intention: are play signals functional? — Semiotica 15: 231-240.
Bekoff M. (1977). Mammalian dispersal and the ontogeny of individual behavioral phenotypes. — Am. Nat. 111: 715-732.
Bekoff M. (1995). Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids. — Behaviour 132: 419-429.
Bekoff M. (2001). Social play behaviour. Cooperation, fairness, trust, and the evolution of morality. — J. Consciousness Stud. 8: 81-90.
Bekoff M. (2014). The significance of ethological studies: playing and peeing. — In: Dog behavior and cognition ( Horowitz A., ed.). Springer, New York, NY, p. 59-75.
Bekoff M., Allen C. (1998). Intentional communication and social play: how and why animals negotiate and agree to play. — In: Animal play: evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives ( Byers J.A., ed.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 97-114.
Bekoff M., Byers J.A. (1981). A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny and phylogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: an ethological hornet’s nest. — In: Behavioral development: the Bielefeld interdisciplinary project ( Immelmann K., Barlow G., Main M., Petrinovich L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 296-337.
Bekoff M., Pierce J. (2009). Wild justice: the moral lives of animals. — University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
Bell H.C., Pellis S.M., Kolb B. (2010). Juvenile play experience and the development of the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices. — Behav. Brain Res. 207: 7-13.
Berman C.M. (1982). The ontogeny of social relationships with group companions among free-ranging infant rhesus monkeys: II. Differentiation and attractiveness. — Anim. Behav. 30: 149-162.
Burghardt G.M. (2001). Play: attributes and neural substrates. — In: Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Vol. 13: Developmental psychobiology, developmental neurobiology and behavioral ecology: mechanisms and early principles ( Blass E.M., ed.). Kluwer Academic/Plenum, New York, NY, p. 317-356.
Burghardt G.M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. — MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Burghardt G.M., Burghardt L.S. (1972). Notes on the behavioral development of two female black bear cubs: the first eight months. — In: Bears: their biology and management, Vol. 2 ( Herrero S.M., ed.). IUCN Publications, Gland, p. 207-220.
Campbell M.W., de Waal F.B.M. (2011). Ingroup–outgroup bias in contagious yawning by chimpanzees supports link to empathy. — PLoS One 6: e18283.
Campbell M.W., de Waal F.B.M. (2014). Chimpanzees empathize with group mates and humans, but not with baboons or unfamiliar chimpanzees. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 281: 20140013.
Carr H.A. (1902). The survival values of play. — Investigations of the Department of Psychology and Education of the University of Colorado 1: 1-47, Boulder, CO.
Charles-Dominique P. (1977). Ecology and behaviour of nocturnal primates: prosimians of equatorial West Africa. — Duckworth, London.
Charles-Dominique P., Cooper H.M., Hladik A., Hladik C.M., Pages E., Pariente G.F., Petter-Rousseaux A., Schilling A., Petter J.J. (1980). Nocturnal malagasy primates: ecology, physiology and behaviour. — Academic Press, New York, NY.
Ciani F., Dall’Olio S., Stanyon R., Palagi E. (2012). Social tolerance and adult play in macaque societies: a comparison with different human cultures. — Anim. Behav. 84: 1313-1322.
Clay Z., de Waal F.B.M. (2013). Bonobos respond to distress in others: consolation across the age spectrum. — PLoS One 8: e55206.
Cordoni G. (2009). Social play in captive wolves (Canis lupus): not only an immature affair. — Behaviour 146: 1363-1385.
Cordoni G., Norscia I. (2014). Peace-making in marsupials: the first study in the red-necked wallaby (Macropus rufogriseus). — PLoS One 9: e86859.
Cordoni G., Palagi E. (2008). Reconciliation in wolves (Canis lupus): new evidence for a comparative perspective. — Ethology 114: 298-308.
Cordoni G., Palagi E. (2011). Ontogenetic trajectories of chimpanzee social play: similarities with humans. — PLoS One 6: e27344.
Cordoni G., Palagi E., Borgognini Tarli S.M. (2006). Reconciliation and consolation in captive western gorillas. — Int. J. Primatol. 27: 1365-1382.
Cozzi A., Sighieri C., Gazzano A., Nicol C.J., Baragli P. (2010). Post-conflict friendly reunion in a permanent group of horses (Equus caballus). — Behav. Process. 85: 185-190.
Darwin C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. — John Murray, London.
Darwin C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and other animals. — John Murray, London.
Das M. (2000). Conflict management via third parties. — In: Natural conflict resolution ( Aureli F., de Waal F.B.M., eds). University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, p. 263-280.
Davila-Ross M., Menzler S., Zimmermann E. (2008). Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play. — Biol. Lett. 4: 27-30.
de Waal F.B.M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. — Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59: 279-300.
de Waal F.B.M., Ferrari P.F. (2010). Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition. — Trends Cogn. Sci. 14: 201-207.
de Waal F.B.M., van Roosmalen A. (1979). Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 5: 55-66.
Demuru E., Ferrari P.F., Palagi E. (2015). Emotionality and intentionality in bonobo playful communication. — Anim. Cogn. 18: 333-344.
Dimberg U., Thunberg M. (2012). Empathy, emotional contagion, and rapid facial reactions to angry and happy facial expressions. — PsyCh J. 1: 118-127.
Doyle G.A. (1974). The behaviour of the lesser bushbaby. — In: Prosimian biology ( Martin R.D., Doyle G.A., Walker A.C., eds). University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, PA, p. 213-231.
Drea C.M., Hawk J.E., Glickman S.E. (1996). Aggression decreases as play emerges in infant spotted hyaenas: preparation for joining the clan. — Anim. Behav. 51: 1323-1336.
Dugatkin L.A., Bekoff M. (2003). Play and the evolution of fairness: a game theory model. — Behav. Process. 60: 209-214.
Dunbar R.I.M., Dunbar E.P. (1975). Social dynamics of gelada baboons. Contributions to primatology, Vol. 6. — Karger, Basel.
Fagen R. (1981). Animal play behavior. — Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
Feldman R. (2007). Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. — J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 49: 329-354.
Feldman R. (2010). The relational basis of adolescent adjustment: trajectories of mother–child interactive behaviors from infancy to adolescence shape adolescents’ adaptation. — Attach. Hum. Dev. 12: 173-192.
Ferrari P.F., Bonini L., Fogassi L. (2009). From monkey mirror neurons to primate behaviours: possible ‘direct’ and ‘indirect’ pathways. — Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 364: 2311-2323.
Flack J.C., de Waal F.B.M. (2000). Any animal whatever. Darwinian building blocks of morality in monkeys and apes. — J. Consciousness Stud. 7: 1-29.
Flack J.C., de Waal F.B.M. (2004). Dominance style, social power and conflict management: a conceptual framework. — In: Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization ( Thierry B., Singh M., Kaumanns W., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 157-181.
Fleagle J.G. (2013). Primate adaptation and evolution, 3rd edn. — Academic Press, San Diego, CA.
Fouts H.N., Hallam R.A., Purandare S. (2013). Gender segregation in early-childhood social play among the Bofi foragers and Bofi farmers in Central Africa. — Am. J. Play 5: 333-356.
Fraser O.N., Stahl D., Aureli A. (2008). Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105: 8557-8562.
Fry D.P. (1992). “Respect for the rights of others is peace”: learning aggression versus nonaggression among the Zapotec. — Am. Anthropol. 94: 621-639.
Gallese V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 358: 517-528.
Gallese V., Keysers C., Rizzolatti G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. — Trends Cogn. Sci. 8: 396-403.
Galtung J. (1964). A structural theory of aggression. — J. Peace Res. 1: 95-119.
Hare B., Melis A.P., Woods V., Hastings S., Wrangham R.W. (2007). Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. — Curr. Biol. 17: 619-623.
Hare B., Wobber V., Wrangham R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. — Anim. Behav. 83: 573-585.
Hatfield E., Rapson R.L., Le Y.-C.L. (2009). Emotional contagion and empathy. — In: The social neuroscience of empathy ( Decety J., Ickes W., eds). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, p. 19-30.
Iacoboni M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. — Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60: 653-670.
Judge P.G. (1991). Dyadic and triadic reconciliation in pigtailmacaques (Macaca nemestrina). — Am. J. Primatol. 23: 225-237.
Lee P.C., Moss C.J. (2014). African elephant play, competence and social complexity. — Anim. Behav. Cogn. 1: 144-156.
Lewis K.P., Barton R.A. (2006). Amygdala size and hypothalamus size predict social play frequency in nonhuman primates: a comparative analysis using independent contrasts. — J. Comp. Psychol. 120: 31-37.
Lorenz K. (1966). On aggression. — Bantam, New York, NY.
Maestripieri D. (2004). Maternal behaviour, infant handling and socialization. — In: Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization ( Thierry B., Singh M., Kaumanns W., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 231-234.
Maestripieri D. (2005). Gestural communication in three species of macaques (Macaca mulatta, M. nemestrina, M. arctoides): use of signals in relation to dominance and social context. — Gesture 5: 57-73.
Mancini G., Palagi E. (2009). Play and social dynamics in a captive herd of gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada). — Behav. Proc. 82: 286-292.
Mancini G., Ferrari P.F., Palagi E. (2013a). Rapid facial mimicry in geladas. — Sci. Rep. 3: 1527.
Mancini G., Ferrari P.F., Palagi E. (2013b). In play we trust. Rapid facial mimicry predicts the duration of playful interactions in geladas. — PLoS One 8: e66481.
Martin L.J., Hathaway G., Isbester K., Mirali S., Acland E.L., Niederstrasser N., Slepian P.M., Trost Z., Bartz J.A., Sapolsky R.M., Sternberg W.F., Levitin D.J., Mogil J.S. (2015). Reducing social stress elicits emotional contagion of pain in mouse and human strangers. — Curr. Biol. 25: 326-332.
Martin P., Caro T.M. (1985). On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. — Adv. Stud. Behav. 15: 59-103.
Montgomery S.H. (2014). The relationship between play, brain growth and behavioural flexibility in primates. — Anim. Behav. 90: 281-286.
Naber F., van IJzendoorn M.H., Deschamps P., van Engeland H., Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin increases fathers’ observed responsiveness during play with their children: a double-blind within-subject experiment. — Psychoneuroendocrinology 35: 1583-1586.
Norman K., Pellis S., Barrett L., Henzi S.P. (2015). Down but not out: supine postures as facilitators of play in domestic dogs. — Behav. Process. 110: 88-95.
Norscia I., Palagi E. (2011). When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. — Primates 52: 101-104.
Norscia I., Palagi E. (2015). The socio-matrix reloaded: from hierarchy to dominance profile in wild lemurs. — PeerJ 3: e729.
Norscia I., Antonacci D., Palagi E. (2009). Mating first, mating more: biological market fluctuation in a wild prosimian. — PLoS One 4: e4679.
Pagés E. (1978). Home range, behaviour, and tactile communication in a nocturnal Malagasy lemur Mirza coquereli. — In: Recent advances in primatology, Vol. 3: Evolution ( Chivers D., ed.). Academic Press, New York, NY, p. 171-177.
Palagi E. (2006). Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for natural social systems and inter-individual relationships. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 129: 418-426.
Palagi E., Cordoni G. (2012). The right time to happen: play developmental divergence in the two Pan species. — PLoS One 7: e52767.
Palagi E., Norscia I. (2013). Bonobos protect and console friends and kin. — PLoS One 8: e79290.
Palagi E., Paoli T., Tarli S.B. (2004). Reconciliation and consolation in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Am. J. Primatol. 62: 15-30.
Palagi E., Cordoni G., Tarli S.B. (2006). Possible roles of consolation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 129: 105-111.
Palagi E., Leone A., Mancini G., Ferrari P.F. (2009). Contagious yawning in gelada baboons as a possible expression of empathy. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106: 19262-19267.
Palagi E., Dall’Olio S., Demuru E., Stanyon R. (2014). Exploring the evolutionary foundations of empathy: consolation in monkeys. — Evol. Hum. Behav. 35: 341-349.
Palagi E., Burghardt G.M., Smuts B., Cordoni G., Dall’Olio S., Fouts H.N., Řeháková-Petrů M., Siviy S.M., Pellis S.M. (2015). Rough-and-tumble play as a window on animal communication. — Biol. Rev. DOI:10.1111/brv.12172.
Palagi E., Nicotra V., Cordoni G. (2015). Rapid mimicry and emotional contagion in domestic dogs. — R. Soc. Open Sci. 2: 150505.
Panksepp J., Panksepp J.B. (2013). Toward a cross-species understanding of empathy. — Trends Neurosci. 36: 489-496.
Panksepp J. (2005). Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and humans. — Conscious Cogn. 14: 30-80.
Paquette D. (1994). Fighting and playfighting in captive adolescent chimpanzees. — Aggress. Behav. 20: 49-65.
Paukner A., Suomi S.J., Visalberghi E., Ferrari P.F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. — Science 325: 880-883.
Pellegrini A.D. (1988). Elementary school children’s rough-and-tumble play and social competence. — Dev. Psychol. 24: 802-806.
Pellegrini A.D. (1995). A longitudinal study of boy’s rough-and-tumble play and dominance during early adolescence. — J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 16: 77-93.
Pellis S.M., Iwaniuk A.N. (2002). Brain system size and adult-adult play in primates: a comparative analysis of the roles of the non-visual neocortex and the amygdala. — Behav. Brain Res. 134: 31-39.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C. (1996). On knowing it’s only play: the role of play signals in play fighting. — Aggress. Violent Behav. 1: 249-268.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C. (2009). The playful brain: venturing to the limits of neuroscience. — Oneworld Publications, London.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C. (2010). Social play, social grooming, and the regulation of social relationships. — In: Neurobiology of grooming behavior ( Kalueff A.V., LaPorte J.L., Bergner C.L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 66-87.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C., Bell H.C. (2010). The function of play in the development of the social brain. — Am. J. Play 2: 278-296.
Pellis S.M., Pellis V.C., Himmler B.T. (2014). How play makes for a more adaptable brain: a comparative and neural perspective. — Am. J. Play 7: 73-98.
Pereira M.E. (1993). Agonistic interaction, dominance relation, and ontogenetic trajectories in ring-tailed lemurs. — In: Juvenile primates ( Pereira M.E., Fairbanks L.A., eds). Oxford University Press, New York, NY, p. 285-305.
Pfeifer J.H., Iacoboni M., Mazziotta J.C., Dapretto M. (2008). Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. — NeuroImage 39: 2076-2085.
Pierce J., Bekoff M. (2012). Wild justice redux: what we know about social justice in animals and why it matters. — Soc. Justice Res. 25: 122-139.
Preston S.D., de Waal F.B.M. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. — Behav. Brain Sci. 25: 1-20.
Provine R.R. (1992). Contagious laughter: laughter is a sufficient stimulus for laughs and smiles. — Bull. Psychonomic Soc. 30: 1-4.
Pruitt J.N., Burghardt G.M., Riechert S.E. (2012). Non-conceptive sexual behavior in spiders: a form of play associated with body condition, personality type, and male intrasexual selection. — Ethology 118: 33-40.
Reinhart C.J., Pellis V.C., Thierry B., Gauthier C.A., VanderLaan D.P., Vasey P.L., Pellis S.M. (2010). Targets and tactics of play fighting: competitive versus cooperative styles of play in Japanese and Tonkean macaques. — Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 23: 166-200.
Romero T., Colmenares F., Aureli F. (2009). Testing the function of reconciliation and third-party affiliation for aggressors in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 60-69.
Romero T., Castellanos M.A., de Waal F.B.M. (2011). Post-conflict affiliation by chimpanzees with aggressors: other-oriented versus selfish political strategy. — PLoS One 6: e22173.
Romero T., Nagasawa M., Mogi K., Hasegawa T., Kikusui T. (2014). Oxytocin promotes social bonding in dogs. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111: 9085-9090.
Schino G. (1998). Reconciliation in domestic goats. — Behaviour 135: 343-356.
Singer T. (2006). The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of literature and implications for future research. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30: 855-863.
Smuts B. (2014). Social behaviour among companion dogs with an emphasis on play. — In: The social dog: behavior and cognition ( Kaminski J., Marshall-Pescini S., eds). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p. 105-130.
Thierry B. (1985). Patterns of agonistic interactions in three species of macaque (Macaca mulatta, M. fascicularis, M. tonkeana). — Aggress. Behav. 11: 223-233.
Thierry B., Singh M., Kaumanns W. (eds) (2004). Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization, Vol. 41. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
van Hooff J.A.R.A.M. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. — In: Nonverbal communication ( Hinde R.A., ed.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 209-238.
Verbeek P. (2008). Peace ethology. — Behaviour 145: 1497-1524.
von Borell E. (2000). Coping strategies during stress. — Arch. Tierzucht 43: 441-450.
Wobber V., Wrangham R., Hare B. (2010). Application of the heterochrony framework to the study of behavior and cognition. — Curr. Biol. 20: 226-230.
Wrangham R.W., Wilson M.L., Muller M.N. (2006). Comparative rates of violence in chimpanzees and humans. — Primates 47: 14-26.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 2031 | 370 | 38 |
Full Text Views | 390 | 14 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 276 | 19 | 2 |
The concept of peace, with its corollary of behaviours, strategies and social implications, is commonly believed as a uniquely human feature. Through a comparative approach, we show how social play in animals may have paved the way for the emergence of peace. By playing fairly, human and nonhuman animals learn to manage their social dynamics in a more relaxed and tolerant way that results in a more effective management of conflicts. We show that play promotes tolerance, cooperation, fairness and reciprocity, which are essential elements of the so-called positive peace. This kind of peace is reached through an evolving process in which individuals continually modify social relationships to attain peaceful coexistence. In conclusion, we assume that the concept of peace has deep biological roots that constitute the basis for more sophisticated cultural constructions.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 2031 | 370 | 38 |
Full Text Views | 390 | 14 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 276 | 19 | 2 |