Activity budgets and behavioural synchrony in a wild equid living in a fission-fusion social system

in Behaviour
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Variation in activity budgets among individuals of different age-sex classes and reproductive status may lead to decreases in behavioural synchrony (i.e., individuals performing the same behaviour at the same time in the same group) in social species. Here, we assessed the costs of behavioural synchrony in terms of time allocated to feeding behaviour among individuals of different age-sex classes and reproductive status in the kiang (Equus kiang), a poorly known wild equid that inhabits the Tibetan Plateau. Our study was conducted in Eastern Ladakh (India), during summer and fall. Our results showed that groups were highly synchronized, and that individuals in groups were particularly synchronized when feeding. Despite a slight sexual dimorphism, males and females had similar activity budgets. Males in groups, however, spent less time feeding than solitary males, and females in groups with foals spent less time feeding and more time standing than females in groups without foals. We suggest that group formation in males and the presence of foals for females incur behavioural costs by lowering their time spent feeding. Because these costs occur at a predictable time of the year, it could be profitable for adult kiangs not to form permanent groups year-round. Individuals with divergent needs might benefit from the loose social system observed in kiangs, which could be a key feature of their adaptation to a highly seasonal environment.

Activity budgets and behavioural synchrony in a wild equid living in a fission-fusion social system

in Behaviour

Sections

References

AltmannJ. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. — Behaviour 49: 227-267.

BergerJ. (1983). Induced abortion and social factors in wild horses. — Nature 303: 59-61.

BergerJ. (1986). Wild horses of the Great Basin: social competition and population size. — The University of Chicago PressChicago, IL.

BurnhamK.P.AndersonD.R. (2002). Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. — SpringerNew York, NY.

ConradtL. (1998). Could asynchrony in activity between the sexes cause intersexual social segregation in ruminants?Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 265: 1359-1363.

ConradtL.RoperT.J. (2000). Activity synchrony and social cohesion: a fission-fusion model. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 267: 2213-2218.

CôtéS.D.SchaeferJ.A.MessierF. (1997). Time budget and synchrony of activities in muskoxen: the influence of sex, age, and season. — Can. J. Zool. 75: 1628-1635.

DenzauG.DenzauH. (1999). Wildesel. — ThorbeckeStuttgart.

DuncanP. (1982). Foal killing by stallions. — Appl. Anim. Ethol. 8: 567-570.

DuncanP. (1992). Horses and grasses: the nutritional ecology of equids and their impact on the camargue. — SpringerNew York, NY.

FischhoffI.R.SundaresanS.R.CordingleyJ.LarkinH.M.SellierM.J.RubensteinD.I. (2007). Social relationships and reproductive state influence leadership roles in movements of plains zebra, Equus burchellii. — Anim. Behav. 73: 825-831.

FoxJ.L.NurbuC.ChundawatR.S. (1991). The mountain ungulates of Ladakh, India. — Biol. Conserv. 58: 167-190.

GiraldeauL.-A.CaracoT. (2000). Social foraging theory. — Princeton University PressPrinceton, NJ.

HamelS.CôtéS.D. (2008). Trade-offs in activity budget in an alpine ungulate: contrasting lactating and nonlactating females. — Anim. Behav. 75: 217-227.

HauschildtV.GerkenM. (2015). Temporal stability of social structure and behavioural synchronization in Shetland pony mares (Equus caballus) kept on pasture. — Acta Agr. Scand. A-An. 65: 33-41.

JarmanP.J. (1974). The social organization of antelope in relation to their ecology. — Behaviour 48: 216-267.

KlingelH. (1977). Observations on social organization and behaviour of African and Asiatic wild asses (Equus africanus and Equus hemionus). — Z. Tierpsychol. 44: 323-331.

LittellR.C.MillikenG.A.StroupW.W.WolfingerR.D.SchabenbergerO. (2006). SAS for mixed models. — SAS InstituteCary, NC.

MeldrumG.E.RuckstuhlK.E. (2009). Mixed-sex group formation by bighorn sheep in winter: trading costs of synchrony for benefits of group living. — Anim. Behav. 77: 919-929.

MichelenaP.NoëlS.GautraisJ.GerardJ.-F.DeneubourgJ.-L.BonR. (2006). Sexual dimorphism, activity budget and synchrony in groups of sheep. — Oecologia 148: 170-180.

MoehlmanP.D. (1998). Feral asses (Equus africanus): intraspecific variation in social organization in arid and mesic habitats. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 60: 171-195.

MoorcroftW.TrebeckG. (1841). Travels in the Provinces of Hindustan and the Panjab; in Ladakh and Kashmir; in Peshawar Kabul Kunduz and Bokhara; from 1819 to 1825. — John MurrayLondon.

NeuhausP.RuckstuhlK.E. (2002). The link between sexual dimorphism, activity budgets, and group cohesion: the case of the plain zebra (Equus Burchelli). — Can. J. Zool. 80: 1437-1441.

Noy-MeirI. (1973). Desert ecosystems: environment and producers. — Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 4: 25-51.

Noy-MeirI. (1974). Desert ecosystems: higher trophic levels. — Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5: 195-214.

Pérez-BarberíaF.J.RobertsonE.SoriguerR.AldezabalA.MendizabalM.Pérez-FernándezE. (2007). Why do polygynous ungulates segregate in space? Testing the activity-budget hypothesis in Soay sheep. — Ecol. Monogr. 77: 631-647.

PfisterO. (2004). Birds and mammals of Ladakh. — Oxford University PressNew Delhi.

PutmanR.J. (1986). Grazing in temperate ecosystems: large herbivores and the ecology of the New Forest. — Timber PressPortland, OR.

RandsS.A.CowlishawG.PettiforR.A.RowcliffeJ.M.JohnstoneR.A. (2008). The emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs when the qualities of individuals differ. — BMC Evol. Biol. 8: 51.

RawatG.S.AdhikariB.S. (2005). Floristics and distribution of plant communities across moisture and topographic gradients in Tso Kar basin, Changthang Plateau, Eastern Ladakh. — Arct. Antarct. Alp. Res. 17: 539-544.

RookA.J.PenningP.D. (1991). Synchronisation of eating, ruminating and idling activity by grazing sheep. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 32: 157-166.

RubensteinD.I. (1986). Ecology and sociality in horses and zebras. — In: Ecological aspects of social evolution ( RubensteinD.I.WranghamR.W. eds). Princeton University PressPrinceton, NJ p.  282-302.

RubensteinD.I. (1989). Life history and social organization in arid adapted ungulates. — J. Arid Environ. 17: 145-156.

RubensteinD.I. (1994). The ecology of female social behaviour in horses, zebras, and asses. — In: Animal societies: individuals interactions and organization ( JarmanP.RossiterA. eds). Kyoto University PressKyoto p.  13-28.

RuckstuhlK.E. (1998). Foraging behaviour and sexual segregation in bighorn sheep. — Anim. Behav. 56: 99-106.

SAS Institute (2003). SAS/stat user’s guide. — SAS InstituteCary, NC.

SchallerG.B. (1998). Wildlife of the Tibetan steppe. — The University of Chicago PressChicago, IL.

St-LouisA. (2010). Écologie du kiang au Ladakh: sélection des ressources approvisionnement et comportement social d’un équidé méconnu du Plateau Tibétain. — Ph.D. Thesis Université Laval Laval.

St-LouisA.CôtéS.D. (2009). Equus kiang (Perisodactyla: Equidae). — Mamm. Species 835: 1-11.

St-LouisA.CôtéS.D. (2012). Foraging behaviour at several temporal scales in a wild alpine equid. — Oecologia 169: 167-176.

St-LouisA.CôtéS.D. (2014). Resource selection in a high-altitude rangeland equid, the kiang (Equus kiang): influence of forage abundance and quality at multiple spatial scales. — Can. J. Zool. 92: 239-249.

SundaresanS.R.FischhoffI.R.RubensteinD.I. (2007). Male harassment influences female movements and associations in Grevy’s zebra (Equus grevyi). — Behav. Ecol. 18: 860-865.

van DierendonckM.C.SigurjónsdóttirH.ColenbranderB.ThorhallsdóttirA.G. (2004). Differences in social behaviour between late pregnant, post-partum and barren mares in a herd of Icelandic horses. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 89: 283-297.

WalkerA.B.D.ParkerK.L.GillinghamM.P. (2006). Behaviour, habitat associations, and intrasexual differences of female Stone’s sheep. — Can. J. Zool. 84: 1187-1201.

WardD. (2006). Long-term effects of herbivory on plant diversity and functional types in arid ecosystems. — In: Large herbivore ecology ecosystem dynamics and conservation ( DanellK.BergströmR.DuncanP.PastorJ. eds). Cambridge University PressNew York, NY p.  142-169.

XuR. (2003). Measuring explained variation in linear mixed effects models. — Stat. Med. 22: 3527-3541.

ZarJ.H. (1984). Biostatistical analysis2nd edn.Prentice-HallEnglewood Cliffs.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Synchrony coefficients of kiang groups in relation to (a) habitat types and (b) date in Tso Kar basin, Ladakh, India, 2003–2005. Different letters indicate statistical significance.

  • View in gallery

    Variations in the activity budgets (% of time ± SE) among age-sex classes in kiangs in Tso Kar basin, Ladakh, India, 2003–2005.

  • View in gallery

    Comparison of the activity budgets (% of time ± SE) of (a) females in groups with foals and females in groups without foals and (b) single males and males in groups in kiangs in Tso Kar basin, Ladakh, India, 2003–2005. Asterisks indicate significant differences between adjacent bars.

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 18 18 6
Full Text Views 64 64 46
PDF Downloads 3 3 1
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0