Do you want to stay informed about this journal? Click the buttons to subscribe to our alerts.
Play bows represent a common, highly stereotyped behaviour across the genus Canis. However, much of what we know is limited to the wolf and its domestic derivative, the domestic dog. Here we continue to look at the function of play bows among subspecies/variants of Canis lupus by including the dingo. Comparing dingoes to wolves and dogs may provide further insight into the impact of domestication on play behaviour. We analysed play bows in three-to-six month old dingo puppies and compared the results to previous studies of wolves and dogs. The function of play bows in dingoes appears consistent with those observed in dogs and wolf puppies. However, subtle intraspecific differences (such as the frequency and duration of play bows, and vocalizations during play) were apparent, and warrant further investigation in the genus Canis, as well as the Family Canidae more broadly.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Bauer, E., Ward, C. & Smuts, B. (2009). Play like a puppy, play like a dog. — J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 4: 68-69.
Bekoff, M. (1974). Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. — Am. Zool. 14: 323-340.
Bekoff, M. (1995). Play signals as punctuation: the structure of social play in canids. — Behaviour 132: 419-429.
Byosiere, S.-E., Espinosa, J., Marshall-Pescini, S., Smuts, B. & Range, F. (2016a). Investigating the function of play bows in dog and wolf puppies (Canis lupus familiaris, Canis lupus occidentalis). — PLoS One 11: e0168570.
Byosiere, S.-E., Espinosa, J. & Smuts, B. (2016b). Investigating the function of play bows in adult pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). — Behav. Proc. 125: 106-113.
Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R. & Natoli, E. (2010). Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. — Behav. Ecol. 21: 443-455.
Cairns, K.M., Brown, S.K., Sacks, B.N. & Ballard, J.W.O. (2017). Conservation implications for dingoes from the maternal and paternal genome: multiple populations, dog introgression, and demography. — Ecol. Evol. 7: 9787-9807.
Cairns, K.M. & Wilton, A.N. (2016). New insights on the history of canids in Oceania based on mitochondrial and nuclear data. — Genetica 144: 553-565.
Coppinger, R., Glendinning, J., Torop, E., Matthay, C., Sutherland, M. & Smith, C. (1987). Degree of behavioral neoteny differentiates canid polymorphs. — Ethology 75: 89-108.
Corbett, L. (2001). The dingo in Australia and Asia, 2nd edn. — JB Books, Adelaide, SA.
Cordoni, G., Nicotra, V. & Palagi, E. (2016). Unveiling the “secret” of play in dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): asymmetry and signals. — J. Comp. Psychol. 130: 278-287.
Crowther, M.S., Fillios, M., Colman, N. & Letnic, M. (2014). An updated description of the Australian dingo (Canis dingo Meyer: 1793). — J. Zool. 293: 192-203.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. — University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
Déaux, É.C. & Clarke, J.A. (2013). Dingo (Canis lupus dingo) acoustic repertoire: form and contexts. — Behaviour 150: 75-101.
Dolhinow, P. (1999). Play: a critical process in the developmental system. — In: The nonhuman primates (Dolhinow, P. & Fuentes, A., eds). Mayfield, Mountain View, CA, p. 231-237.
Fagen, R. (1981). Animal play behavior. — Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
Feddersen-Petersen, D. (2000). Vocalization of European wolves (Canis lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canis lupus f. fam.). — Arch. Anim. Breed. 43: 387-398.
Fox, M.W. (1978). The dog: its domestication and behavior. — Garland, New York, NY.
Frank, H. (1980). Evolution of canine information processing under conditions of natural and artificial selection. — Z. Tierpsychol. 53: 389-399.
Freedman, A.H., Gronau, I., Schweizer, R.M., Ortega-Del Vecchyo, D., Han, E., Silva, P.M., Galaverni, M., Fan, Z., Marx, P., Lorente-Galdos, B., Beale, H., Ramirez, O., Hormozdiari, F., Alkan, C., Vilà, C., Squire, K., Geffen, E., Kusak, J., Boyko, A.R., Parker, H.G., Lee, C., Tadigotla, V., Siepel, A., Bustamante, C.D., Harkins, T.T., Nelson, S.F., Ostrander, E.A., Marques-Bonet, T., Wayne, R.K. & Novembre, J. (2014). Genome sequencing highlights the dynamic early history of dogs. — PLOS Genet. 10: e1004016.
Fuller, J. & Fox, M. (eds) (1969). The behaviour of dogs, 2nd edn. — Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, MD.
Goodwin, D., Bradshaw, J.W.S. & Wickens, S.M. (1997). Paedomorphosis affects agonistic visual signals of domestic dogs. — Anim. Behav 53: 297-304.
Harrington, F.H. & Mech, L.D. (1978). 6-Wolf Vocalization1 A2-Hall, Roberta L. — In: Wolf and man (Sharp, H.S., ed.). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, p. 109-132.
Horowitz, A. (2009). Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. — Anim. Cogn. 12: 107-118.
Horowitz, A. & Hecht, J. (2016). Examining dog–human play: the characteristics, affect, and vocalizations of a unique interspecific interaction. — Anim. Cogn. 19: 779-788.
Jackson, S.M., Groves, C.P., Fleming, P.J., Aplin, K.P., Eldridge, M.D., Gonzalez, A. & Helgen, K.M. (2017). The wayward dog: is the Australian native dog or dingo a distinct species?. — Zootaxa 4317: 201-224.
Kleiman, D.G. (1967). Some aspects of social behavior in the Canidae. — Am. Zool. 7: 365-372.
Lord, K., Feinstein, M., Smith, B. & Coppinger, R. (2013). Variation in reproductive traits of members of the genus Canis with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). — Behav. Proc. 92: 131-142.
Lorenz, K. (1953). Man meets dog. — Viking Penguin, New York, NY.
Lund, J.D. & Vestergaard, K.S. (1998). Development of social behaviour in four litters of dogs (Canis familiaris). — Acta Vet. Scand. 39: 183-193.
Molenberghs, G. & Verbeke, G. (2005). Models for discrete longitudinal data. — Springer, New York, NY.
Palagi, E. (2011). Playing at every age: modalities and potential functions in non-human primates. — In: Oxford handbook of the development of play (Pellegrini, A.D., ed.). Oxford University Press, Oxford, England, p. 70-82.
Palagi, E., Nicotra, V. & Cordoni, G. (2015). Rapid mimicry and emotional contagion in domestic dogs. — Roy. Soc. Open Sci. 2: 150505.
Parker, H.G., Dreger, D.L., Rimbault, M., Davis, B.W., Mullen, A.B., Carpintero-Ramirez, G. & Ostrander, E.A. (2017). Genomic analyses reveal the influence of geographic origin, migration, and hybridization on modern dog breed development. — Cell Rep. 19: 697-708.
Pellis, S.M. & Iwaniuk, A.N. (2000). Adult–adult play in primates: comparative analyses of its origin, distribution and evolution. — Ethology 106: 1083-1104.
Pellis, S.M. & Pellis, V.C. (1996). On knowing it’s only play: the role of play signals in play fighting. — Aggr. Viol. Behav. 1: 249-268.
Rooney, N.J., Bradshaw, J.W. & Robinson, I.H. (2000). A comparison of dog–dog and dog–human play behaviour. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 66: 235-248.
Sacks, B.N., Brown, S.K., Stephens, D., Pedersen, N.C., Wu, J.-T. & Berry, O. (2013). Y chromosome analysis of dingoes and southeast Asian village dogs suggests a Neolithic continental expansion from southeast Asia followed by multiple Austronesian dispersals. — Mol. Biol. Evol. 30: 1103-1118.
Savolainen, P., Leitner, T., Wilton, A.N., Matisoo-Smith, E. & Lundeberg, J. (2004). A detailed picture of the origin of the Australian dingo, obtained from the study of mitochondrial DNA. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101: 12387-12390.
Smith, B.P. (2015). The dingo debate: origins, behaviour and conservation. — CSIRO Publishing, Melbourne, VIC.
Smith, B.P. & Litchfield, C.A. (2009). A review of the relationship between indigenous Australians, dingoes (Canis dingo) and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). — Anthrozoös 22: 111-128.
Smith, B.P. & Watson, L. (2015). How can private organisations aid dingo conservation? — In: The Dingo Debate. Origins, Behaviour and Conservation 277.
Tembrock, G. (1976). Canid vocalizations. — Behav. Proc. 1: 57-75.
Thomson, P.C. (1992a). The behavioural ecology of dingoes in north-western Australia. II. Activity patterns, breeding season and pup rearing. — Wildl. Res. 19: 519-529.
Thomson, P.C. (1992b). The behavioural ecology of dingoes in north-western Australia. III. Hunting and feeding behaviour, and diet. — Wildl. Res. 19: 531-541.
von Holdt, B.M., Pollinger, J.P., Lohmueller, K.E., Han, E., Parker, H.G., Quignon, P., Degenhardt, J.D., Boyko, A.R., Earl, D.A., Auton, A., Reynolds, A., Bryc, K., Brisbin, A., Knowles, J.C., Mosher, D.S., Spady, T.C., Elkahloun, A., Geffen, E., Pilot, M., Jedrzejewski, W., Greco, C., Randi, E., Bannasch, D., Wilton, A., Shearman, J., Musiani, M., Cargill, M., Jones, P.G., Qian, Z., Huang, W., Ding, Z.L., Zhang, Y.P., Bustamante, C.D., Ostrander, E.A., Novembre, J. & Wayne, R.K. (2010). Genome-wide SNP and haplotype analyses reveal a rich history underlying dog domestication. — Nature 464: 898-902.
Waller, B.M., Peirce, K., Caeiro, C.C., Scheider, L., Burrows, A.M., McCune, S. & Kaminski, J. (2013). Paedomorphic facial expressions give dogs a selective advantage. — PLoS One 8: e82686.
Wang, G.-D., Zhai, W., Yang, H.-C., Wang, L., Zhong, L., Liu, Y.-H., Fan, R.-X., Yin, T.-T., Zhu, C.-L., Poyarkov, A.D., Irwin, D.M., Hytönen, M.K., Lohi, H., Wu, C.-I., Savolainen, P. & Zhang, Y.-P. (2015). Out of southern east Asia: the natural history of domestic dogs across the world. — Cell Res. 26: 21-33.
Ward, C., Bauer, E.B. & Smuts, B.B. (2008). Partner preferences and asymmetries in social play among domestic dog, Canis lupus familiaris, littermates. — Anim. Behav. 76: 1187-1199.
Yin, S. & McCowan, B. (2004). Barking in domestic dogs: context specificity and individual identification. — Anim. Behav. 68: 343-355.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 2170 | 177 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 333 | 16 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 135 | 28 | 2 |
Play bows represent a common, highly stereotyped behaviour across the genus Canis. However, much of what we know is limited to the wolf and its domestic derivative, the domestic dog. Here we continue to look at the function of play bows among subspecies/variants of Canis lupus by including the dingo. Comparing dingoes to wolves and dogs may provide further insight into the impact of domestication on play behaviour. We analysed play bows in three-to-six month old dingo puppies and compared the results to previous studies of wolves and dogs. The function of play bows in dingoes appears consistent with those observed in dogs and wolf puppies. However, subtle intraspecific differences (such as the frequency and duration of play bows, and vocalizations during play) were apparent, and warrant further investigation in the genus Canis, as well as the Family Canidae more broadly.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 2170 | 177 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 333 | 16 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 135 | 28 | 2 |