The investigation of vocal similarity between individuals has provided evidence of the flexibility of communication signals. This study evaluates the impact of group membership, affiliative bonds, kinship and dominance on acoustic similarity in two primate species with different social styles, intolerant rhesus macaques and tolerant Tonkean macaques. We focused on the fundamental frequencies of the contact calls emitted by adult females. Close kinship promoted vocal similarity between individuals in both species, and also group membership in Tonkean macaques, indicating the involvement of experiential and/or genetic factors. In rhesus macaques more similarities were observed between partners with strong or weak dominance asymmetry than between those with medium asymmetry, which again points to the role of experience. No evidence was found that dominance influences vocal similarity in Tonkean macaques. Our results provide additional evidence to the flexibility of vocal signals produced by macaques, and reveal that it is influenced by social style.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Ackermann, H., Hage, S.R. & Ziegler, W. (2014). Phylogenetic reorganization of the basal ganglia: a necessary, but not the only, bridge over a primate Rubicon of acoustic communication. — Behav. Brain Sci. 37: 577-604.
Anderson, C., Keltner, D. & John, O.P. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. — J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84: 1054-1068.
Arriaga, G., Zhou, E.P. & Jarvis, E.D. (2012). Of mice, birds, and men: the mouse ultrasonic song system has some features similar to humans and song-learning birds. — PLoS ONE 7: e46610.
Balasubramaniam, K.N., Dittmar, K., Berman, C.M., Butovskaya, M., Cooper, M.A., Majolo, B., Ogawa, H., Schino, G., Thierry, B. & de Waal, F.B.M. (2012). Hierarchical steepness and phylogenetic models: phylogenetic signals in Macaca. — Anim. Behav. 83: 1207-1218.
Barton, K. (2016). MuMIn: multi-model inference. — R Package version 1.15.6. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, available online at https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MunMn.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. — J. Stat. Soft. 67: 1-48.
Belyk, M. & Brown, S. (2017). The origins of the vocal brain in humans. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77: 177-193.
Belzung, C. & Anderson, J.R. (1986). Social rank and responses to feeding competition in rhesus monkeys. — Behav. Process. 12: 307-316.
Briefer, E.F. & McElligott, A.G. (2012). Social effects on vocal ontogeny in an ungulate, the goat, Capra hircus. — Anim. Behav. 83: 991-1000.
Burnham, K.P. & Anderson, D.R. (2002). Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. — Springer Science & Business Media, New York, NY.
Candiotti, A., Zuberbühler, K. & Lemasson, A. (2012). Convergence and divergence in Diana monkey vocalizations. — Biol. Lett. 8: 382-385.
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M. (2018). Flexible usage and social function in primate vocalizations. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115: 1974-1979.
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L. & Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? — Ethology 243: 221-243.
de la Torre, S. & Snowdon, C.T. (2009). Dialects in pygmy marmosets? Population variation in call structure. — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 333-342.
De Marco, A., Sanna, A., Cozzolino, R. & Thierry, B. (2014). The function of greetings in male Tonkean macaques. — Am. J. Primatol. 76: 989-998.
de Vries, H., Netto, W.J. & Hanegraaf, P.L.H. (1993). Matman: a program for the analysis of sociometric matrices and behavioural transition matrices. — Behaviour 125: 157-175.
de Waal, F.B.M. (1986). The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. — Q. Rev. Biol. 61: 459-479.
Dobson, S.D. (2012). Coevolution of facial expression and social tolerance in macaques. — Am. J. Primatol. 74: 229-235.
Doxiadis, G.G.M., de Groot, N., Otting, N., de Vos-Rouweler, A.J.M., Bolijn, M.J., Heijmans, C.M.C., de Groot, N.G., van der Wiel, M.K.H., Remarque, E.J., Vangenot, C., Nunes, J.M., Sanchez-Mazas, A. & Bontrop, R.E. (2013). Haplotype diversity generated by ancient recombination-like events in the MHC of Indian rhesus macaques. — Immunogenetics 65: 569-584.
Edwin, C.J. & Chopra, S.R.K. (1984). Group characteristics of Macaca mulatta inhabiting submontane and montane forests of the Indian subcontinent: a comparative review. — In: Current primate researches (Roonwal, M.L., Mohnot, S.M. & Rathore, N.S., eds). Jodhpur University, Jodhpur, p. 307-313.
Ey, E., Pfefferle, D. & Fischer, J. (2007). Do age- and sex-related variations reliably reflect body size in non-human primate vocalizations? A review. — Primates 48: 253-267.
Feekes, F. (1982). Song mimesis within colonies of Cacicus c. cela (Icteridae, Aves). A colonial password? — Z. Tierpsychol. 58: 119-152.
Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K. & Todt, D. (1998). Local variation in Barbary macaque shrill barks. — Anim. Behav. 56: 623-629.
Fox, J. (2003). Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. — J. Stat. Soft. 8: 1-27, available online at https://www.jstatsoft.org/v08/i15/.
Freeberg, T.M., Dunbar, R.I. & Ord, T.J. (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. — Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 367: 1785-1801.
Geissmann, T. (1984). Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analysed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus x H. lar). — Folia Primatol. 42: 216-235.
Giles, H., Coupland, N. & Coupland, J. (1991). Accommodation theory: communication, context, and consequence. — In: Context of accommodation: developments in sociolinguistics (Giles, H., Coupland, N. & Coupland, J., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 1-68.
Green, S. (1975). Dialects in Japanese monkeys: vocal learning and cultural transmission of locale-specific vocal behavior? — Z. Tierpsychol. 38: 304-314.
Gregory, S.W. & Webster, S. (1996). A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status perceptions. — J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70: 1231-1240.
Gruber, T. & Grandjean, D. (2017). A comparative neurological approach to the decoding of emotional expressions in primate vocalizations. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 73: 182-190.
Gruber, T. & Clay, Z. (2016). A comparison between bonobos and chimpanzees: a review and update. — Evol. Anthropol. 25: 239-252.
Gustison, M.L., le Roux, A. & Bergman, T.J. (2012). Derived vocalizations of geladas (Theropithecus gelada) and the evolution of vocal complexity in primates. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 367: 1847-1859.
Hage, S.R. (2018). Dual neural network model of speech and language evolution: new insights on flexibility of vocal production systems and involvement of frontal cortex. — Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 21: 80-87.
Hage, S.R., Gavrilov, N. & Nieder, A. (2013). Cognitive control of distinct vocalizations in rhesus monkeys. — J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25: 1692-1701.
Hammerschmidt, K. & Fischer, J. (2008). Constraints in primate vocal production. — In: The evolution of communicative creativity: from fixed signals to contextual flexibility (Oller, K.D. & Griebel, U., eds). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, p. 93-119.
Herrenschmidt, N. (1977). Semi-free breeding of tropical Celebes macaques (Macaca tonkeana) in continental European climate. — J. Med. Primatol. 6: 58-65.
Hervé, M. (2017). RVAideMemoire: diverse basic statistical and graphical functions. R package version 0.9-65. — R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, available online at https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=RVAideMemoire.
Hodun, A., Snowdon, C.T. & Soini, P. (1981). Subspecific variation in the long calls of the tamarin, Saguinus fuscicollis. — Z. Tierpsychol. 57: 97-110.
Hopkins, W.D., Taglialatela, J. & Leavens, D.A. (2007). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. — Anim. Behav. 11: 281-286.
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F. & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. — Biom. J. 50: 346-363.
Janik, V.M. (2014). Cetacean vocal learning and communication. — Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28: 60-65.
Koda, H. (2004). Flexibility and context-sensitivity during the vocal exchange of coo calls in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). — Behaviour 141: 1279-1296.
Koda, H., Oyakawa, C., Kato, A. & Masataka, N. (2007). Experimental evidence to the volitional control of vocal production in an immature gibbon. — Behaviour 144: 681-692.
Koda, H., Nishimura, T., Tokuda, T.I., Oyakawa, C., Nihonmatsu, T. & Masataka, N. (2012). Soprano singing in gibbons. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 149: 347-355.
Koda, H., Lemasson, A., Oyakawa, C., Rizaldi, Pamungkas, J. & Masataka, N. (2013). Possible role of mother–daughter vocal interactions on the development of species specific song in gibbons. — PLoS ONE 8: e71432.
Kumar, R., Sinha, A. & Radhakrishna, S. (2013). Comparative demography of two commensal macaques in India: implications for population status and conservation. — Folia Primatol. 84: 384-393.
Lameira, A.R. (2017). Bidding evidence for primate vocal learning and the cultural substrates for speech evolution. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83: 429-439.
Lameira, A.R., Hardus, M.E., Kowalsky, B. & De Vries, H. (2013). Orangutan (Pongo spp.) whistling and implications for the emergence of an open-ended call repertoire: a replication and extension. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134: 1-11.
Lameira, A.R., Hardus, M.E., Mielke, A., Wich, S.A. & Shumaker, R.W. (2016). Vocal fold control beyond the species-specific repertoire in an orang-utan. — Sci. Rep. 6: 30315.
Lemasson, A. & Hausberger, M. (2004). Patterns of vocal sharing and social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell’s monkeys. — J. Comp. Psychol. 118: 347-359.
Lemasson, A., Blois-Heulin, C., Jubin, R. & Hausberger, M. (2006). Female social relationships in a captive group of Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). — Am. J. Primatol. 68: 1161-1170.
Lemasson, A., Jubin, R., Masataka, N. & Arlet, M. (2016). Copying hierarchical leaders’ voices? Acoustic plasticity in female Japanese macaques. — Sci. Rep. 6: 21289.
Lemasson, A., Ouattara, K., Petit, E.J. & Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? — BMC Evol. Biol. 11: 362.
Lemasson, A., Ouattara, K. & Zuberbühler, K. (2013). Exploring the gaps between primate calls and human language. — In: The evolutionary emergence of language, evidence and inference (Botha, R. & Everaert, M., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 181-203.
Levréro, F., Carrete-Vega, G., Herbert, A., Lawabi, I., Courtiol, A., Willaume, E., Kappeler, P.M. & Charpentier, M.J.E. (2015). Social shaping of voices does not impair phenotype matching of kinship in mandrills. — Nature Commun. 6: 7609.
Levréro, F., Touitou, S., Frédet, J., Nairaud, B., Guéry, J.P. & Lemasson, A. (2019). Social bonding drives vocal exchanges in bonobos. — Sci. Rep. 9: 711.
Maciej, P., Ndao, I., Hammerschmidt, K. & Fischer, J. (2013). Vocal communication in a complex multi-level society: constrained acoustic structure and flexible call usage in Guinea baboons. — Front. Zool. 10: 58.
Maeda, T. & Masataka, N. (1987). Locale-specific vocal behaviour of the tamarin (Saguinus I. labiatus). — Ethology 75: 25-30.
Mitani, J.C. & Gros-Louis, J. (1998). Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypotheses. — Behaviour 135: 1041-1064.
Mitani, J.C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P. & Byrne, R.W. (1992). Dialects in wild chimpanzees? — Am. J. Primatol. 27: 233-243.
Nakagawa, N., Matsubara, M., Shimooka, Y. & Nishikawa, M. (2015). Embracing in a wild group of Yakushima macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) as an example of social customs. — Curr. Anthropol. 56: 104-120.
Neefe, J.R., Ellis, E.B. & Rogentine, G.N. (1975). Rhesus lymphocyte alloantigens. III. Identification of new antigen. — Tiss. Antig. 6: 195-204.
Newman, J.D. & Symmes, D. (1982). Inheritance and experience in the acquisition of primate acoustic behavior. — In: Primate communication (Snowdon, C.T., Brown, C.H. & Peterson, M.R., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 259-278.
Owren, M.J., Dieter, J.A., Seyfarth, R.M. & Cheney, D.L. (1993). Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. — Dev. Psychobiol. 26: 389-406.
Pardo, J.S., Gibbons, R., Suppes, A. & Krauss, R.M. (2012). Phonetic convergence in college roommates. — J. Phon. 40: 190-197.
Pfefferle, D., Hammerschmidt, K., Mundry, R., Ruiz-Lambides, A.V., Fischer, J. & Widdig, A. (2016). Does the structure of female rhesus macaque coo calls reflect relatedness and/or familiarity? — PLoS ONE 11: e0161133.
Pombo, A.R., Waltert, M., Mansjoer, S.S., Mardiastuti, A. & Mühlenberg, M. (2004). Home range, diet and behaviour of the Tonkean macaque (Macaca tonkeana) in Lore Lindu National Park, Sulawesi. — In: Land use, nature conservation, and the stability of rainforest margins in southeast Asia (Gerold, G., Fremerey, M. & Guhardja, E., eds). Springer, Berlin, p. 313-325.
R Core Team (2017). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. — R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, available online at https://www.R-project.org.
Rebout, N., Desportes, C. & Thierry, B. (2017). Resource partitioning in tolerant and intolerant macaques. — Aggress. Behav. 43: 513-520.
Richard, J.P. (1991). Sound analysis and synthesis using an Amiga micro-computer. — Bioacoustics 3: 45-60.
Riley, E.P. (2007). Flexibility in diet and activity patterns of Macaca tonkeana in response to anthropogenic habitat alteration. — Int. J. Primatol. 28: 107-133.
Silk, J.B., Brosnan, S.F., Henrich, J., Lambeth, S.P. & Shapiro, S. (2013). Chimpanzees share food for many reasons: the role of kinship, reciprocity, social bonds and harassment on food transfers. — Anim. Behav. 85: 941-947.
Snowdon, C.T. (2017). Learning from monkey “talk”. — Science 335: 1120-1122.
Snowdon, C.T. & Elowson, A.M. (1999). Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired. — Ethology 105: 893-908.
Snowdon, C.T. & Hausberger, M. (eds) (1997). Social influences of vocal development. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Sugiura, H. (1998). Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese macaques. — Anim. Behav. 55: 673-687.
Tanaka, T., Sugiura, H. & Masataka, N. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of the development of group differences in acoustic features of coo calls in two groups of Japanese macaques. — Ethology 112: 7-21.
Thierry, B. (1994). Social transmission, tradition and culture in primates: from the epiphenomenon to the phenomenon. — Techn. Cult. 23/24: 91-119.
Thierry, B. (2007). Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. — Evol. Anthropol. 16: 224-238.
Thierry, B. (2010). The macaques: a double-layered social organization. — In: Primates in perspective (Campbell, C.J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K.C., Panger, M. & Bearder, S.K., eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford, p. 229-241.
Thierry, B., Anderson, J.R., Demaria, C., Desportes, C. & Petit, O. (1994). Tonkean macaque behaviour from the perspective of the evolution of Sulawesi macaques. — In: Current primatology, vol. 2: Social development, learning and behaviour (Roeder, J.J., Thierry, B., Anderson, J.R. & Herrenschmidt, N., eds). Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg, p. 103-117.
Tyack, P.L. (2008). Convergence of calls as animals form social bonds, active compensation for noisy communication channels, and the evolution of vocal learning in mammals. — J. Comp. Psychol. 122: 319-340.
Volodin, I.A., Volodina, E.V., Lapshina, E.N., Efremova, K.O. & Soldatova, N.V. (2014). Vocal group signatures in the goitred gazelle Gazella subgutturosa. — Anim. Cogn. 17: 349-357.
Watson, S.K., Townsend, S.W., Schel, A.M., Wilke, C., Wallace, E.K., Cheng, L., West, V. & Slocombe, K.E. (2015). Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. — Curr. Biol. 25: 495-499.
Whitehead, H. (2009). SOCPROG programs: analysing animal social structures. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63: 765-778.
Wilbrecht, L. & Nottebohm, F. (2003). Vocal learning in birds and humans. — Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 9: 135-148.
Wilkinson, G.S. & Boughman, J.W. (1998). Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear-nosed bats. — Anim. Behav. 55: 337-350.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1235 | 235 | 45 |
Full Text Views | 107 | 9 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 104 | 11 | 0 |
The investigation of vocal similarity between individuals has provided evidence of the flexibility of communication signals. This study evaluates the impact of group membership, affiliative bonds, kinship and dominance on acoustic similarity in two primate species with different social styles, intolerant rhesus macaques and tolerant Tonkean macaques. We focused on the fundamental frequencies of the contact calls emitted by adult females. Close kinship promoted vocal similarity between individuals in both species, and also group membership in Tonkean macaques, indicating the involvement of experiential and/or genetic factors. In rhesus macaques more similarities were observed between partners with strong or weak dominance asymmetry than between those with medium asymmetry, which again points to the role of experience. No evidence was found that dominance influences vocal similarity in Tonkean macaques. Our results provide additional evidence to the flexibility of vocal signals produced by macaques, and reveal that it is influenced by social style.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1235 | 235 | 45 |
Full Text Views | 107 | 9 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 104 | 11 | 0 |