Systematic, broad phylogenetic comparisons of diverse cognitive abilities are essential to understand cognitive evolution. Few studies have examined multiple skills comparatively, using identical tasks across species. Previous research centered on primates, but recent evidence suggests that complex cognition may have evolved in distantly related taxa. We administered the tasks of the primate cognition test battery (PCTB) to 4 parrot species for a first direct comparison with primates. The parrots did not perform significantly worse than the previously tested primates in all but one of the test scales, but remained at chance levels throughout. Chimpanzees outperformed them in the physical but not the social domain. No differences between the domains nor across the parrot species were detected. It remains questionable whether the chance level performance reflects the parrots’ cognitive capacity or results from task constraints, which would limit the suitability of PCTB for phylogenetic comparisons. Possible implications for the field are discussed.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Aïn, S., Giret, N., Grand, M., Kreutzer, M. & Bovet, D. (2009). The discrimination of discrete and continuous amounts in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). — Anim. Cogn. 12: 145-154.
Amici, F., Barney, B., Johnson, V.E., Call, J. & Aureli, F. (2012). A modular mind? A test using individual data from seven primate species. — PLoS ONE 7: e51918.
Auersperg, A.M.I., Gajdon, G.K. & Huber, L. (2009). Kea (Nestor notabilis) consider spatial relationships between objects in the support problem. — Biol. Lett. 5: 455-458.
Auersperg, A.M.I., Huber, L. & Gajdon, G.K. (2011a). Navigating a tool end in a specific direction: stick-tool use in kea (Nestor notabilis). — Biol. Lett. 7: 825-828.
Auersperg, A.M.I., von Bayern, A.M.P., Gajdon, G.K., Huber, L. & Kacelnik, A. (2011b). Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi access box paradigm. — PLoS ONE 6: e20231.
Auersperg, A.M.I., Gajdon, G.K. & von Bayern, A.M.P. (2012a). A new approach to comparing problem solving, flexibility and innovation. — Commun. Integr. Biol. 5: 140-145.
Auersperg, A.M.I., Szabo, B., Von Bayern, A.M.P. & Kacelnik, A. (2012b). Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo. — Curr. Biol. 22: R903-R904.
Auersperg, A.M.I., Szabo, B., von Bayern, A.M.P. & Bugnyar, T. (2014a). Object permanence in the Goffin cockatoo (Cacatua goffini). — J. Comp. Psychol. 128: 88-98.
Auersperg, A.M.I., Bugnyar, T. & Kacelnik, A. (2014b). Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 281: 20140972.
Balsby, T.J.S. & Bradbury, J.W. (2009). Vocal matching by orange-fronted conures (Aratinga canicularis). — Behav. Process. 82: 133-139.
Baños-Villalba, A., Blanco, G., Díaz-luque, J.A., Dénes, F.V., Hiraldo, F. & Tella, J.L. (2017). Seed dispersal by macaws shapes the landscape of an Amazonian ecosystem. — Sci. Rep. 7: 1-12.
Beran, M.J., Parrish, A.E., Perdue, B.M. & Washburn, D.A. (2014). Comparative cognition: past, present, and future. — Intern. J. Comp. Psychol. 27: 3-30.
Berg, K.S., Delgado, S., Cortopassi, K.A., Beissinger, S.R. & Bradbury, J.W. (2012). Vertical transmission of learned signatures in a wild parrot. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: 585-591.
Bond, A.B., Kamil, A. & Balda, R.P. (2003). Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids? — Anim. Behav. 65: 479-487.
Bräuer, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers? — J. Comp. Psychol. 119: 145-154.
Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2006). Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape? — J. Comp. Psychol. 120: 38-47.
Bray, E.E., Maclean, E.L. & Hare, B.A. (2014). Context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs? — Anim. Cogn. 17: 15-31.
Button, K.S., Ioannidis, J.P.A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B.A., Flint, J., Robinson, E.S.J. & Munafò, M.R. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. — Nature Rev. Neurosci. 14: 365-376.
Call, J., Hare, B.A. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee gaze following in an object-choice task? — Anim. Cogn. 1: 89-99.
Call, J., Agnetta, B. & Tomasello, M. (2000). Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. — Anim. Cogn. 3: 23-34.
Chittka, L., Rossiter, S.J., Skorupski, P. & Fernando, C. (2012). What is comparable in comparative cognition? — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 367: 2677-2685.
Dawson, B.V. & Foss, B.M. (1965). Observational learning in budgerigars. — Anim. Behav. 13: 470-474.
de Waal, F.B.M. & Ferrari, P.F. (2010). Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition? — Trends Cogn. Sci. 14: 201-207.
Deaner, R.O., van Schaik, C.P. & Johnson, V. (2006). Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A meta-analysis of nonhuman primate studies. — Evol. Psychol. 4: 149-196.
Emery, N.J. & Clayton, N. (2004). The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes? — Science 306: 1903-1907.
Emery, N.J. (2006). Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence? — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 361: 23-43.
Fraixedas, S., Fernández-llamazares, Á., Rico, A., Bach, A., Borrós, M., Barriocanal, C. & Boada, C. (2014). Suitability of the south Caribbean coast of Costa Rica for reintroduction of the great green macaw Ara ambiguus. — Nature Resourc. 5: 653-665.
Funk, M.S. (2002). Problem solving skills in young yellow-crowned parakeets (Cyanoramphus auriceps). — Anim. Cogn. 5: 167-176.
Galef, B.G., Manzig, L.A. & Field, R.M. (1986). Imitation learning in budgerigars: Dawson & Foss (1965) revisited. — Behav. Process. 13: 191-202.
Giret, N., Miklósi, Á., Kreutzer, M. & Bovet, D. (2009). Use of experimenter-given cues by African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). — Anim. Cogn. 12: 1-10.
Guenther, A., Brust, V., Dersen, M. & Trillmich, F. (2014). Learning and personality types are related in cavies (Cavia aperea). — J. Comp. Psychol. 128: 74-81.
Hare, B. (2001). Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? — Anim Cogn 4: 269-280.
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C. & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. — Science 298: 1634-1636.
Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. — Anim. Behav. 68: 571-581.
Heinsohn, R., Zdenek, C.N., Cunningham, R.B., Endler, J.A. & Langmore, N.E. (2017). Tool-assisted rhythmic drumming in palm cockatoos shares key elements of human instrumental music. — Sci. Adv. 3: e1602399.
Herrmann, E., Melis, A.P. & Tomasello, M. (2006). Apes’ use of iconic cues in the object-choice task. — Anim. Cogn. 9: 118-130.
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M.V., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized cultural intelligence hypothesis. — Science 317: 1360-1366.
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2010). Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. — PLoS ONE 5: 2-5.
Heyes, C. & Saggerson, A. (2002). Testing for imitative and nonimitative social learning in the budgerigar using a two-object / two-action test. — Anim. Behav. Cogn. 64: 851-859.
Hopkins, W.D., Russell, J.L. & Schaeffer, J. (2014). Chimpanzee intelligence is heritable. — Curr. Biol. 24: 1649-1652.
Huber, L., Rechberger, S. & Taborsky, M. (2001). Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. — Anim. Behav. 62: 945-954.
Iwaniuk, A.N., Dean, K.M. & Nelson, J.E. (2005). Interspecific allometry of the brain and brain regions in parrots (Psittaciformes): comparisons with other birds and primates. — Brain Behav. Evol. 65: 40-59.
Jaeggi, A.V., Dunkel, L.P., van Noordwijk, M.A., Wich, S.A., Sure, A.A.L. & van Schaik, C.P. (2010). Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: implications for culture. — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 62-71.
James, A.S., Groman, S.M., Seu, E., Jorgensen, M., Fairbanks, L.A. & Jentsch, J.D. (2007). Dimensions of impulsivity are associated with poor spatial working memory performance in monkeys. — J. Neurosci. 27: 14358-14364.
Jelbert, S.A., Taylor, A.H. & Gray, R.D. (2016). Does absolute brain size really predict self-control ? Hand-tracking training improves performance on the A-not-B task. — Biol. Lett. 12: 20150871.
Krasheninnikova, A., Bräger, S. & Wanker, R. (2013). Means-end comprehension in four parrot species: explained by social complexity. — Anim. Cogn. 16: 755-764.
Krasheninnikova, A., Höner, F., O’Neill, L., Penna, E. & von Bayern, A.M.P. (2018). Economic decision-making in parrots. — Sci. Rep. 8: 12537.
Lacreuse, A., Russell, J.L., Hopkins, W.D. & Herndon, J.G. (2014). Cognitive and motor aging in female chimpanzees. — Neurobiol. Aging 35: 1-22.
Lambert, M.L., Jacobs, I., Osvath, M. & von Bayern, A.M.P. (2019). Birds of a feather? Parrot and corvid cognition compared. — Behaviour 156: 505-594.
MacLean, E.L. (2016). Unraveling the evolution of uniquely human cognition. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113: 6348-6354.
MacLean, E.L., Matthews, L.J., Hare, B.A., Nunn, C.L., Anderson, R.C., Aureli, F., et al. (2012). How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. — Anim. Cogn. 15: 223-238.
MacLean, E.L., Hare, B., Nunn, C.L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R.C., et al. (2014). The evolution of self-control. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111: E2140-E2148.
Moore, B.R. (1992). Avian movement imitation and a new form of mimicry: tracing the evolution of a complex form of learning. — Behaviour 122: 231-263.
Mottley, K. & Heyes, C. (2003). Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) copy virtual demonstrators in a two-action test. — J. Comp. Psychol. 117: 363-370.
O’Hara, M.O., Huber, L. & Kopanny, G. (2015). The advantage of objects over images in discrimination and reversal learning by kea, Nestor notabilis. — Anim. Behav. 101: 51-60.
O’Hara, M.O., Mioduszewska, B., Von Bayern, A., Auersperg, A., Wilkinson, A., Huber, L. & Gajdon, G.K. (2017). The temporal dependence of exploration on neotic style in birds. — Sci. Rep. 7: 1-9.
O’Hara, M.O., Schwing, R., Federspiel, I., Gajdon, G.K. & Huber, L. (2016). Reasoning by exclusion in the kea (Nestor notabilis). — Anim. Cogn. 19: 965-975.
Olkowicz, S., Kocourek, M., Lučan, R.K., Porteš, M., Fitch, W.T., Herculano-Houzel, S. & Němec, P. (2016). Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113: 7255-7260.
Osvath, M., Kabadayi, C. & Jacobs, I. (2014). Independent evolution of similar complex cognitive skills: the importance of embodied degrees of freedom. — Anim. Behav. Cogn. 1: 249-264.
Pepperberg, I.M. (1994). Numerical competence in an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). — J. Comp. Psychol. 108: 36-44.
Pepperberg, I.M. (2006). Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 100: 77-86.
Pepperberg, I.M., Willner, M.R. & Gravitz, L.B. (1997). Development of Piagetian object permanence in grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). — J. Comp. Psychol 111: 63-75.
Péron, F., Rat-fischer, L., Nagle, L. & Bovet, D. (2010). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: do grey parrots understand human intentional actions? — Interaction Studies 11: 428-441.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. — Norton, New York, NY.
Reader, S.M., Hager, Y. & Laland, K.N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 366: 1017-1027.
Russell, J.L., Lyn, H., Schaeffer, J.A. & Hopkins, W.D. (2011). The role of socio-communicative rearing environments on the development of social and physical cognition in apes. — Dev. Sci. 14: 1459-1470.
Scheid, C. & Noë, R. (2014). The performance of rooks in a cooperative task depends on their temperament. — Anim. Cogn. 13: 545-553.
Schloegl, C., Schmidt, J., Boeckle, M., Weiss, B.M. & Kotrschal, K. (2012). Grey parrots use inferential reasoning based on acoustic cues alone. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: 4135-4142.
Schmitt, V. (2018). Implementing new portable touchscreen-setups to enhance cognitive research and enrich zoo-housed animals. — BioRxiv 316042: 1-21.
Schmitt, V., Pankau, B. & Fischer, J. (2012). Old world monkeys compare to apes in the primate cognition test battery. — PLoS ONE 7: e32024.
Schuck-Paim, C., Borsari, A. & Ottoni, E.B. (2009). Means to an end: Neotropical parrots manage to pull strings to meet their goals. — Anim. Cogn. 12: 287-301.
Seed, A., Emery, N.J. & Clayton, N.S. (2009). Intelligence in corvids and apes: a case of convergent evolution? — Ethology 115: 401-420.
Shaw, R.C. & Schmelz, M. (2017). Cognitive test batteries in animal cognition research: evaluating the past, present and future of comparative psychometrics. — Anim. Cogn. 20: 1003-1018.
Shettleworth, S.J. (2009). The evolution of comparative cognition: is the snark still a boojum? — Behav. Proc. 80: 210-217.
Shettleworth, S.J. (2010). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. — Trends Cogn. Sci. 14: 477-481.
Sol, D., Duncan, R.P., Blackburn, T.M., Cassey, P. & Lefebvre, L. (2005). Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102: 5460-5465.
Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. (2001). Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). — J. Comp. Psychol. 115: 122-126.
Stevens, J.R., Hallinan, E.V. & Hauser, M.D. (2005). The ecology and evolution of patience in two New World monkeys. — Biol. Lett. 1: 223-226.
Tamungan, S.A. & Ajayib, S.S. (2003). Diversity of food of the Grey Parrot Psittacus erithacus in Korup National Park, Cameroon. — Bull. ABC 10: 33-36.
Taylor, A.H., Hunt, G.R., Holzhaider, J.C. & Gray, R.D. (2007). Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. — Curr. Biol. 17: 1504-1507.
Tella, J.L., Baños-Villalba, A., Hernández-Brito, D., Rojas, A., Pacífico, E., Díaz-Luque, J.A., et al. (2015). Parrots as overlooked seed dispersers. — Front. Ecol. Environ. 13: 338-339.
Tobias, J.A. & Brightsmith, D.J. (2007). Distribution, ecology and conservation status of the Blue-headed Macaw Primolius couloni. — Biol. Cons. 139: 126-138.
Valenchon, M., Lévy, F. & Fortin, M. (2013). Stress and temperament affect working memory performance for disappearing food in horses, Equus caballus. — Anim. Behav. 86: 1233-1240.
van Horik, J. & Emery, N.J. (2011). Evolution of cognition. — Wiley Interdiscipl. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2: 621-633.
Villegas-Retana, S.A. & Araya, D. (2017). Consumo de almendro de playa (Terminalia catappa) y uso de hojas como herramienta por parte del ave Ara ambiguus (Psittaciformes: Psittacidae) en Costa Rica. — UNED Res. J. 9: 199-201.
von Bayern, A.M.P., Heathcote, R.J.P., Rutz, C. & Kacelnik, A. (2009). The role of experience in problem solving and innovative tool use in crows. — Curr. Biol. 19: 1965-1968.
Wellman, H.M., Lane, J.D., Labounty, J. & Olson, L. (2011). Observant, nonaggressive temperament predicts theory-of-mind development. — Dev. Sci. 14: 319-326.
Werdenich, D. & Huber, L. (2006). A case of quick problem solving in birds: string pulling in keas, Nestor notabilis. — Anim. Behav. 71: 855-863.
Yamashita, C. & Machado de Barros, Y. (1997). The Blue-throated Maxaw Ara glaucogularis: characterization of its distinctive habitats in savannahs of the Beni, Bolivia. — Ararajuba 5: 141-150.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1766 | 282 | 38 |
Full Text Views | 255 | 70 | 7 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 277 | 76 | 9 |
Systematic, broad phylogenetic comparisons of diverse cognitive abilities are essential to understand cognitive evolution. Few studies have examined multiple skills comparatively, using identical tasks across species. Previous research centered on primates, but recent evidence suggests that complex cognition may have evolved in distantly related taxa. We administered the tasks of the primate cognition test battery (PCTB) to 4 parrot species for a first direct comparison with primates. The parrots did not perform significantly worse than the previously tested primates in all but one of the test scales, but remained at chance levels throughout. Chimpanzees outperformed them in the physical but not the social domain. No differences between the domains nor across the parrot species were detected. It remains questionable whether the chance level performance reflects the parrots’ cognitive capacity or results from task constraints, which would limit the suitability of PCTB for phylogenetic comparisons. Possible implications for the field are discussed.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1766 | 282 | 38 |
Full Text Views | 255 | 70 | 7 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 277 | 76 | 9 |