Many species of parrots live in fission–fusion social systems, characterised by frequent changes in flock composition. In these systems, the ability to selectively choose flock members is essential in order to maximise individual fitness. As a result, most species of parrots have individual distinctive contact calls that mediate the formation of groups during fission and fusion events. However, in vocal interactions during fission and fusion events, individuals will modify the fine-scale structure of their contact calls in a manner that sometimes will result in imitation of the contact calls of another individual, potentially altering or weakening the individual distinctiveness of contact calls. This presents parrots with an interesting dilemma. Here we present a study investigating the effect of vocal modification during interactions, including vocal imitation, on the individual distinctiveness and sex-specific differences of contact calls from ten captive bred peach-fronted conures (Eupsittula aurea). In order to determine if vocal individual- and sex distinctiveness persists in contact calls that are modified to that of another individual, we compared nine acoustic parameters from spontaneous (baseline) contact calls and contact calls emitted as response to a playback stimulus. Although modified, all acoustic parameters remained individually distinctive when the focal individuals interacted with the playback stimulus. These results provide a strong basis for discriminating between calls from different individuals across several social contexts, which could play an important role in mediating selective associations between individual peach-fronted conures during fission and fusion events.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Amici, F., Aureli, F. & Call, J. (2008). Fission–fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. — Curr. Biol. 18: 1415-1419.
Archie, E.A., Moss, C.J. & Alberts, S.C. (2006). The ties that bind: genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 273: 513-522.
Aubin, T. & Jouventin, P. (2006). How to vocally identify kin in a crowd: the penguin model. — Adv. Stud. Behav. 31: 243-277.
Aureli, F. & Schaffner, C.M. (2007). Aggression and conflict management at fusion in spider monkeys. — Biol. Lett. 3: 147-149.
Balsby, T.J. & Bradbury, J.W. (2009). Vocal matching by orange-fronted conures (Aratinga canicularis). — Behav. Proc. 82: 133-139.
Balsby, T.J., Momberg, J.V. & Dabelsteen, T. (2012). Vocal imitation in parrots allows addressing of specific individuals in a dynamic communication network. — PLoS ONE 7: e49747.
Balsby, T.J. & Scarl, J.C. (2008). Sex-specific responses to vocal convergence and divergence of contact calls in orange-fronted conures (Aratinga canicularis). — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 275: 2147-2154.
Balsby, T.J.S. & Adams, D.M. (2011). Vocal similarity and familiarity determine response to potential flockmates in orange-fronted conures (Psittacidae). — Anim. Behav. 81: 983-991.
Boughman, J.W. & Wilkinson, G.S. (1998). Greater spear-nosed bats discriminate group mates by vocalizations. — Anim. Behav. 55: 1717-1732.
Bradbury, J.W. & Balsby, T.J.S. (2016). The functions of vocal learning in parrots. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70: 293-312.
Bradbury, J.W., Cortopassi, K.A. & Clemmons, J.R. (2001). Geographical variation in the contact calls of orange-fronted parakeets. — Auk 118: 958-972.
Buhrman-Deever, S.C., Hobson, E.A. & Hobson, A.D. (2008). Individual recognition and selective response to contact calls in foraging brown-throated conures, Aratinga pertinax. — Anim. Behav. 76: 1715-1725.
Catchpole, C.K. & Slater, P.J.B. (2008). Bird song: biological themes and variations. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Chapman, C.A. & Lefebvre, L. (1990). Manipulating foraging group size: spider monkey food calls at fruiting trees. — Anim. Behav. 39: 891-896.
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M. (1982). Recognition of individuals within and between groups of free-ranging vervet monkeys. — Amer. Zool. 22: 519-529.
Connor, R.C., Wells, R., Mann, J. & Read, A. (2000). The bottlenose dolphin: social relationships in a fission–fusion society. — In: Cetacean societies: field studies of whales and dolphins (Mann, J., Connor, R.C., Tyack, P. & Whitehead, H., eds). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, p. 89-127.
Cortopassi, K.A. & Bradbury, J.W. (2000). The comparison of harmonically rich sounds using spectrographic cross-correlation and principal coordinates analysis. — Bioacoustics 11: 89-127.
Cortopassi, K.A. & Bradbury, J.W. (2006). Contact call diversity in wild orange-fronted parakeet pairs, Aratinga canicularis. — Anim. Behav. 71: 1141-1154.
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L. & Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? — Ethology 110: 221-243.
Croft, D.P., James, R. & Krause, J. (2008). Exploring animal social networks. — Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Dabelsteen, T. & Pedersen, S.B. (1985). Correspondence between messages in the full song of the blackbird Turdus merula and meanings to territorial males, as inferred from responses to computerized modifications of natural song. — Ethology 69: 149-165.
Dooling, R.J. (1982). Auditory perception in birds. — In: Acoustic communication in birds: production, perception, and design features of sound (Kroodsma, D.E. & Miller, E.H., eds). Academic Press, New York, NY, p. 95-131.
Dyrcz, A., Sauer-Gürth, H., Tkadlec, E. & Wink, M. (2004). Offspring sex ratio variation in relation to brood size and mortality in a promiscuous species: the aquatic warbler Acrocephalus paludicola. — Ibis 146: 269-280.
Falls, J.B., Krebs, J.R. & Mcgregor, P.K. (1982). Song matching in the great tit (Parus major): the effect of similarity and familiarity. — Anim. Behav. 30: 997-1009.
Griffiths, R., Double, M.C., Orr, K. & Dawson, R.J.G. (1998). A DNA test to sex most birds. — Mol. Ecol. 7: 1071-1075.
Hall, M.L., Illes, A. & Vehrencamp, S.L. (2006). Overlapping signals in banded wrens: long-term effects of prior experience on males and females. — Behav. Ecol. 17: 260-269.
Hardouin, L.A., Tabel, P. & Bretagnolle, V. (2006). Neighbour–stranger discrimination in the little owl, Athene noctua. — Anim. Behav. 72: 105-112.
Hile, A.G. & Striedter, G.F. (2000). Call convergence within groups of female budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). — Ethology 106: 1105-1114.
Janik, V.M. (2000). Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). — Science 289: 1355-1357.
Janik, V.M. & Slater, P.J. (2000). The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. — Anim. Behav. 60: 1-11.
Janik, V.M. & Slater, P.J.B. (1997). Vocal learning in mammals. — Adv. Stud. Behav. 26: 59-99.
Juniper, T. & Parr, M. (1998). Parrots: a guide to the parrots of the world. — Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.
Kelley, J.L., Morrell, L.J., Inskip, C., Krause, J. & Croft, D.P. (2011). Predation risk shapes social networks in fission–fusion populations. — PLoS ONE 6: e24280.
King, S.L., Harley, H.E. & Janik, V.M. (2014). The role of signature whistle matching in bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. — Anim. Behav. 96: 79-86.
King, S.L., Friedman, W.R., Allen, S.J., Gerber, L., Jensen, F.H., Wittwer, S., Connor, R.C. & Krützen, M. (2018). Bottlenose dolphins retain individual vocal labels in multi-level alliances. — Curr. Biol. 28: 1993-1999.e3.
Komdeur, J. & Hatchwell, B.J. (1999). Kin recognition: function and mechanism in avian societies. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 14: 237-241.
Krause, J. & Ruxton, G. (2002). Living in groups. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Krebs, J.R., Ashcroft, R. & Van Orsdol, K. (1981). Song matching in the great tit Parus major L. — Anim. Behav. 29: 918-923.
Lind, H., Dabelsteen, T. & Mcgregor, P.K. (1996). Female great tits can identify mates by song. — Anim. Behav. 52: 667-671.
Mammen, D.L. & Nowicki, S. (1981). Individual differences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 9: 179-186.
Marler, P. (2004). Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. — Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1016: 31-44.
Mcgregor, P.K. & Dabelsteen, T. (1996). Communication networks. — In: Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds (Kroodsma, D.E. & Miller, E.H., eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p. 409-425.
Mcgregor, P.K. & Peake, T.M. (2000). Communication networks: social environments for receiving and signalling behaviour. — Acta Ethol. 2: 71-81.
Mcgregor, P.K., Dabelsteen, T., Shepherd, M. & Pedersen, S.B. (1992). The signal value of matched singing in great tits: evidence from interactive playback experiments. — Anim. Behav. 43: 987-998.
Mennill, D.J. & Ratcliffe, L.M. (2004). Do male black-capped chickadees eavesdrop on song contests? A multi-speaker playback experiment. — Behaviour 141: 125-139.
Mennill, D.J., Ratcliffe, L.M. & Boag, P.T. (2002). Female eavesdropping on male song contests in songbirds. — Science 296: 873.
Mundinger, P.C. (1970). Vocal imitation and individual recognition of finch calls. — Science 168: 480-482.
Naguib, M. & Todt, D. (1997). Effects of dyadic vocal interactions on other conspecific receivers in nightingales. — Anim. Behav. 54: 1535-1543.
Naguib, M., Fichtel, C. & Todt, D. (1999). Nightingales respond more strongly to vocal leaders of simulated dyadic interactions. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 266: 537-542.
Nielsen, B.M.B. & Vehrencamp, S.L. (1995). Responses of song sparrows to song-type matching via interactive playback. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 37: 109-117.
Otter, K., Mcgregor, P.K., Terry, A.M.R., Burford, F.R.L., Peake, T.M. & Dabelsteen, T. (1999). Do female great tits (Parus major) assess males by eavesdropping? A field study using interactive song playback. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 266: 1305-1309.
Peake, T.M., Mcgregor, P.K. & Dabelsteen, T. (2001). Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male-to-male vocal interactions. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 268: 1183-1187.
Peake, T.M., Terry, A.M., Mcgregor, P.K. & Dabelsteen, T. (2002). Do great tits assess rivals by combining direct experience with information gathered by eavesdropping? — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 269: 1925-1929.
Podos, J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). — Evolution 51: 537-551.
Podos, J. & Nowicki, S. (2004). Performance limits on birdsong. — In: Nature’s music: the science of bird song (Marler, P. & Slabbekoorn, H., eds). Academic Press, New York, NY, p. 318-341.
Podos, J., Huber, S.K. & Taft, B. (2004). Bird song: the interface of evolution and mechanism. — Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35: 55-87.
Ramos-Fernández, G. (2005). Vocal communication in a fission–fusion society: do spider monkeys stay in touch with close associates? — Int. J. Primatol. 26: 1077-1092.
Rendall, D., Rodman, P.S. & Emond, R.E. (1996). Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. — Anim. Behav. 51: 1007-1015.
Scarl, J.C. & Bradbury, J.W. (2009). Rapid vocal convergence in an Australian cockatoo, the galah Eolophus roseicapillus. — Anim. Behav. 77: 1019-1026.
Schmidt, R., Amrhein, V., Kunc, H.P. & Naguib, M. (2007). The day after: effects of vocal interactions on territory defence in nightingales. — J. Anim. Ecol. 76: 168-173.
Smith, J.E., Kolowski, J.M., Graham, K.E., Dawes, S.E. & Holekamp, K.E. (2008). Social and ecological determinants of fission–fusion dynamics in the spotted hyaena. — Anim. Behav. 76: 619-636.
Smolker, R. & Pepper, J.W. (1999). Whistle convergence among allied male bottlenose dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.). — Ethology 105: 595-617.
Stoddard, P.K. (1996). Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. — In: Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds (Kroodsma, D.E. & Miller, E.H., eds). Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, p. 356-374.
Sugiura, H. (1998). Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese macaques. — Anim. Behav. 55: 673-687.
Thomsen, H., Balsby, T.J.S. & Dabelsteen, T. (2013). Individual variation in the contact calls of the monomorphic peach-fronted conure, Aratinga aurea, and its potential role in communication. — Bioacoustics 22: 215-227.
Tyack, P. (1986). Whistle repertoires oftwo bottlenosed dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: mimicry of signature whistles? — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18: 251-257.
Van Schaik, C.P. (1999). The socioecology of fission–fusion sociality in orangutans. — Primates 40: 69-86.
Vehrencamp, S.L. (2001). Is song-type matching a conventional signal of aggressive intentions? — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 268: 1637-1642.
Vehrencamp, S.L., Ritter, A.F., Keever, M. & Bradbury, J.W. (2003). Responses to playback of local vs. distant contact calls in the orange-fronted conure, Aratinga canicularis. — Ethology 109: 37-54.
Walløe, S., Thomsen, H., Balsby, T.J. & Dabelsteen, T. (2015). Differences in short-term vocal learning in parrots, a comparative study. — Behaviour 152: 1433-1461.
Wanker, R., Sugama, Y. & Prinage, S. (2005). Vocal labelling of family members in spectacled parrotlets, Forpus conspicillatus. — Anim. Behav. 70: 111-118.
White, S.J. (1971). Selective responsiveness by the gannet (Sula bassana) to played-back calls. — Anim. Behav. 19: 125-131.
Willis, C.K.R. & Brigham, R.M. (2004). Roost switching, roost sharing and social cohesion: forest-dwelling big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, conform to the fission–fusion model. — Anim. Behav. 68: 495-505.
Wright, T.F. (1996). Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 263: 867-872.
Wright, T.F., Cortopassi, K.A., Bradbury, J.W. & Dooling, R.J. (2003). Hearing and vocalizations in the orange-fronted conure (Aratinga canicularis). — J. Comp. Psychol. 117: 87-95.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 735 | 105 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 98 | 16 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 118 | 25 | 0 |
Many species of parrots live in fission–fusion social systems, characterised by frequent changes in flock composition. In these systems, the ability to selectively choose flock members is essential in order to maximise individual fitness. As a result, most species of parrots have individual distinctive contact calls that mediate the formation of groups during fission and fusion events. However, in vocal interactions during fission and fusion events, individuals will modify the fine-scale structure of their contact calls in a manner that sometimes will result in imitation of the contact calls of another individual, potentially altering or weakening the individual distinctiveness of contact calls. This presents parrots with an interesting dilemma. Here we present a study investigating the effect of vocal modification during interactions, including vocal imitation, on the individual distinctiveness and sex-specific differences of contact calls from ten captive bred peach-fronted conures (Eupsittula aurea). In order to determine if vocal individual- and sex distinctiveness persists in contact calls that are modified to that of another individual, we compared nine acoustic parameters from spontaneous (baseline) contact calls and contact calls emitted as response to a playback stimulus. Although modified, all acoustic parameters remained individually distinctive when the focal individuals interacted with the playback stimulus. These results provide a strong basis for discriminating between calls from different individuals across several social contexts, which could play an important role in mediating selective associations between individual peach-fronted conures during fission and fusion events.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 735 | 105 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 98 | 16 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 118 | 25 | 0 |