Vocal complexity can be expressed through variations in repertoire size, structure, and individual manatee repertoires. Here we aimed to assess the complexity of the vocal behaviour of Antillean manatees living in captivity (i.e., artificial pools) and in reintroduction enclosures (i.e., natural enclosures placed in an estuarine area). Specifically, we evaluated: (i) the structure of vocalisations to assess whether they had variants; (ii) the variation in call production (rate and pattern) between groups with different configurations; (iii) whether individuality occurred in vocalisation structure. We found four categories of vocalisations, of which two had different variants. Not all study groups produced all call categories and variants. Older and younger males in the reintroduction enclosures had the highest call rates compared to captive females and captive males. The vocal and behavioural patterns differed between groups. Squeak call structure differed between individuals. Such vocal complexity may aid manatees in adapting to their dynamic social and structural environment, facilitating communication.
A complexidade vocal pode ser expressa através de variações no tamanho do repertório vocal, na sua estrutura e individualidade. Aqui objetivamos avaliar a complexidade no comportamento vocal de peixes-boi-marinhos vivendo em cativeiro (i.e., piscinas artificiais) e em recintos de reintrodução (i.e., recintos naturais em uma área estuarina). Especificamente, investigamos: (i) a estrutura das vocalizações para avaliar se apresentavam variantes; (ii) a variação na produção de vocalizações (taxa e padrão) entre grupos com diferentes configurações; (iii) se a individualidade ocorre na estrutura de vocalização. Encontramos quatro categorias de vocalizações, das quais duas possuíam variantes. Nem todos os grupos de estudo produziram todas as categorias de vocalizações e suas variantes. Os machos mais velhos e os mais jovens nos recintos de reintrodução tiveram as maiores taxas de vocalizações em comparação com as fêmeas e machos cativos. Os padrões vocais e comportamentais diferiram entre os grupos. A estrutura física da vocalização Squeak diferiu entre os indivíduos. Essa complexidade vocal pode ajudar os peixes-bois-marinhos a se adaptarem ao seu ambiente social e estrutural dinâmico, facilitando a comunicação.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. — Behaviour 49: 227-267.
Alves, M.D.O., Schwamborn, R., Borges, J.C.G., Marmontel, M., Costa, A.F., Schettini, C.A.F. & Araujo, M.E. (2013). Aerial survey of manatees, dolphins and sea turtles off northeastern Brazil: correlations with coastal features and human activities. — Biol. Conserv. 161: 91-100.
Ames, A.E., Blackwell, S.B., Tevo, O.M. & Heide-Jørgensen, M.P. (2021). Evidence of stereotyped contact call used in narwhale (Monodon monoceros) mother-calf communication. — PLoS ONE 16: e0254929.
Attademo, F.L.N., Normande, I.C., Sousa, G.P., Costa, A.F., Borges, J.C.G., de Alencar, A.E.B., Foppel, E.F.C. & Luna, F.O. (2022). Reproductive success of Antillean manatees released in Brazil: implications for conservation. — J. Mar. Biol. Ass. UK 102: 252-259.
Berger-Tal, O., Wong, B.B.M., Candolin, U. & Barber, J. (2019). What evidence exists on the effects of anthropogenic noise on acoustic communication in animals? A systematic map protocol. — Environ. Evidence 8(1): 18.
Boness, D.J. & Bowen, W.D. (1996). The evolution of maternal care in pinnipeds: new findings raise questions about the evoultion of maternal feeding strategies. — BioScience 46: 645-654.
Bouchet, H., Blois-Heulin, C. & Lemasson, A. (2013). Social complexity parallels vocal complexity: a comparison of three non-human primates. — Psychology 4: 390.
Brady, B., Hedwig, D., Trygonis, V. & Gerstein, E. (2020). Classification of Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris) vocalizations. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 147: 1597-1606.
Brady, B., Moore, J. & Love, K. (2022). Behavior related vocalizations of the Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris). — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 38: 975-989.
Brown, J.H., Gillooly, J.F., Allen, A.P., Savage, V.M. & West, G.B. (2004). Toward a metabolic theory of ecology. — Ecology 85: 1771-1789.
Bucher, T.L., Ryan, M.J. & Bartholomew, G.A. (1982). Oxygen consumption during resting, calling and nest building in the frog Physalaemus pustulosus. — Physiol. Zool. 55: 10-22.
Buder, E.H., Chorna, L.B., Oller, D.K. & Robinson, R.B. (2008). Vibratory regime classification of infant phonation. — J. Voice 22: 553-564.
Caldwell, M.C. & Caldwell, D.K. (1965). Individualised whistle contours in bottlenosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). — Nature 207: 434-435.
Caldwell, M.C. & Caldwell, D.K. (1979). The whistle of the Atlantic bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) — ontogeny. — In: Behaviour of marine animals (Winn, H.E., ed.). Plenum Press, New York, NY, pp. 369-401.
Caorsi, V.Z., Both, C., Cechin, S., Antunes, R. & Borges-Martins, M. (2017). Effects of traffic noise on the calling behaviour of two Neotropical hylid frogs. — PLoS ONE 13: e0197632.
Charlton, B.D., Martin-Wintle, M.S., Owen, M.A., Zhang, H. & Swaisgood, R.R. (2018). Vocal behaviour predicts mating success in giant pandas. — Roy. Soc. Open Sci. 5: 181323.
Charrier, I., Aubin, T. & Mathevon, N. (2010). Mother-calf vocal communication in Atlantic walrus: a first field experimental study. — Anim. Cogn. 13: 471-482.
Chavarría, M.R., Castro, J. & Camacho, A. (2015). The relationship between acoustic habitat, hearing and tonal vocalizations in the Antillean manatee (Trichechus manatus manatus, Linnaeus, 1758). — Biol. Open 4: 1237-1242.
Chaverri, G., Sandoval-Herrera, N.I., Iturralde-Pólit, P., Romero-Vásquez, A., Chaves-Ramírez, S. & Sagot, H. (2021). The energetics of social signalling during roost location in Spix’s disc-winged bats. — J. Exp. Biol. 224: jeb238279.
Cheney, D.L. & Seyfarth, R.M. (2018). Flexible usage and social function in primate vocalisations. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115: 1974-1979.
Costa, B., Umeed, R., Attademo, F., Normande, I.C. & Bezerra, B. (2018). Comportamento vocal noturno e diurno do peixe-boi marinho (Trichechus mantus manatus) — IV Simpósio Pernambucano de Ecologia. — Universidade Rural Federal de Pernambuco, Recife, Brazil.
Crockford, C. & Boesch, C. (2003). Context specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. — Anim. Behav. 66: 115-125.
De Souto et al., (2012). AU: PLEASE COMPLETE REFERENCE.
Dechmann, D.K.N., Wikelski, M., van Noordwijk, H.J., Voigt, C.C. & Voigt-Heucke, S. (2013). Metabolic costs of bat echolocation in a non-foraging context support a role in communication. — Front. Physiol. 4: 66.
Dunn, C., Claridge, D., Durban, J., Shaffer, J., Moretti, D., Tyack, P.L. & Rendell, L.E. (2017). Insights into Blainville’s beaked whale (Mesoplodon densirostris) echolocation ontogeny from recordings of mother-calf pairs. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 33: 1-18.
Favero, I.T., Favero, G.E., Choi-Lima, K.F., dos Santos, H.F., Souza-Alves, J.P., Silva, J.S. & Feitosa, J. (2020). Effects of freshwater limitation on distribution patterns and habitat use of the West Indian manatee, Trichechus manatus, in the northern Brazilian coast. — Aquat. Conserv. 30: 1665-1673.
Fearey, J., Elwen, S.H., James, B.S. & Gridley, T. (2019). Identification of potential signature Trills from free-ranging common dolphins (Delphinus delphis) in South Africa. — Anim. Cogn. 22: 777-789.
Fedurek, P., Zuberbühler, K. & Semple, S. (2017). Trade-offs in the production of animal vocal sequences: insights from the structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots. — Front. Zool. 14: 50.
Freeberg, T.M. (2006). Social complexity can drive vocal complexity: group size influences vocal information in California chickadees. — Psychol. Sci. 17: 557-561.
Freeberg, T.M., Dunbar, R.I.M. & Ord, T.J. (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 367: 1785-1801.
Fripp, D. & Tyack, P.L. (2008). Postpartum whistle production in bottlenose dolphins. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 24: 249-505.
Fuentes, M.M.P.B., Delean, S., Grayson, J., Lavender, S., Logan, M. & Marsh, H. (2016). Spatial and temporal variation in the effects of climactic variables on dugong calf production. — PloS ONE 11: e0155675.
Gazioglu, C., Müftüoglu, A.E., Demir, V., Aksu, A. & Okutan, V. (2015). Connection betweenocean acidification and sound propagation. — Int. J. Environ. Geoinform. 2: 16-26.
Gill, L.F., Goymann, W., Maat, A.T. & Gahr, M. (2015). Patterns of call communication between group-housed zebra finches change during the breeding cycle. — eLife 4: e07770.
Gómez-Carrasco, G., Lesher-Gordillo, J.M., Olivera-Gómez, L.D., Bonde, R.K., Arriaga-Weiss, S., Hernández-Martínez, R., Castañón-Nájera, G., Jiminéz-Domínguez, D., Romo-López, A. & Delgado-Estrella, A. (2020). Genetic diversity and structure from Antillean manatee (Trichechus manatus manatus) in the Southern Gulf of México: comparison between connected and isolated populations. — Trop. Conserv. Sci. 11: 2018.
Green, S.M. (1981). Sex differences and age gradations in vocalisations of Japanese and lion-tailed monkeys (Macaca fuscata and Macaca runcat). — Anim. Zool. 21: 165-183.
Gridley, T., Cockcroft, V.G., Hawkins, E.R., Blewitt, M.L., Morisaka, T. & Janik, V.M. (2014). Signature Trills in free-ranging populations of Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops aduncus. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 30: 1-16.
Gustison, M.L., le Roux, A. & Bergman, T.J. (2012). Derived vocalisations of geladas (Theropithecus gelada) and the evolution of vocal complexity in primates. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 367: 1847-1859.
Gustison, M.L. & Bergman, T.J. (2016). Vocal complexity influences female responses to gelada male calls. — Sci. Reports. 6: 19680.
Hanna, D.E.L., Wilson, D.R., Blouin-Demers, G. & Mennill, D.J. (2014). Spring peepers Pseudacris crucifer modify their call structure in response to noise. — Curr. Zool. 60: 438-448.
Hedwig, D., Poole, J. & Granil, P. (2021). Does social complexity drive vocal complexity? Insights from the two African elephant species. — Animals 11: 3071.
Hénaut, Y., Lopez, S.P.B., Machkour-M’Rabet, S., Morales-Vela, B., Winterton, P. & Delfour, F. (2010). Activities and social interactions in captive Antillean manatees in Mexico. — Mammalia 74: 141-146.
Holt, M.M., Dunkin, R.C., Noren, D. & Williams, T.M. (2013). Are there metabolic costs of vocal responses to noise in marine mammals? — Proc. Meet. Acoust. 19: 010060.
Holt, M.M., Noren, D., Dunkin, R.C. & Williams, T.M. (2015). Vocal performance affects metabolic rate in dolphins: implications for animals communicating in noisy environments. — J. Exp. Biol. 218: 1647-1654.
Holt, M.M., Noren, D.P., Dunkin, R.C. & Williams, T.M. (2016). Comparing the metabolic costs of different sound types in bottlenose dolphins. — Proc. Meet. Acoust. 27: 010019.
Hostetler, J.A., Martin, J., Kosempa, M., Edwards, H.E., Rood, K.A., Barton, S.L. & Runge, M.C. (2021). Reconstructing population dynamics of a threatened marine mammal using multiple data sets. — Sci. Rep. 11: 2702.
Ilyina, T., Zeebe, R.E. & Brewer, P.G. (2009). Future ocean increasingly transparent to low-frequency sound owing to carbon dioxide emissions. — Nature Geosci. 3: 18-22.
Indeck, K.L. (2020). Acoustic communication of female-calf humpback whales during migration. Undergraduate thesis, Unviersity of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD.
Insley, S.J., Philips, A.V. & Charrier, I. (2003). A review of social recognition in pinnipeds. — Aquat. Mamm. 29: 181-201.
Kloepper, L.N. & Simmons, A.M. (2014). Bioacoustic monitoring contributed to an understanding of climate change. — Acoust. Today 2014: 8-15.
Krams, I., Krama, T., Freeberg, T.M., Kullberg, C. & Lucas, J.R. (2012). Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 367: 1879-1891.
Landrau-Giovanetti, N., Mignucci-Giannoni, A.A. & Reidenberg, J.S. (2014). Acoustical and Anatomical determination of sound production and transmission in West Indian (Trichechus manatus) and Amazonian manatees. — Anatom. Rec. 297: 1896-1907.
Lingle, S., Wyman, M.T., Kotrba, R., Teichroeb, L.J. & Romanow, C.A. (2012). What makes a cry a cry? A review of infant distress calls. — Curr. Zool. 58: 698-726.
Lucchini, K., Umeed, R., Guimarães, L., Santos, P., Sommer, I. & Bezerra, B. (2021). The role of touch in captive and semi-captive Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus). — Behaviour 158: 291-313.
Marcoux, M., Whitehead, H. & Rendell, L. (2006). Coda vocalisations recorded in breeding areas almost entirely produced by mature female sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus). — Can. J. Zool. 84: 609-614.
Matthews, J.N., Gill, A., Gordon, J. & Macdonald, D. (1999). Quantitative analysis of tonal calls from five odontocete species, examining interspecific and intraspecific variation. — J. Zool. 249: 403-410.
May-Collado, L., Agnarsson, I. & Wartzok, D. (2007). Re-examining the relationship between body size and tonal signals frequency in whales: a comparative approach using a novel phylogeny. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 23: 524-552.
Medeiros, I.S., Rebelo, V.A., Santos, S.S., Menezes, R., Almeida, N.V., Messias, L.T., Nascimento, J.L.X., Luna, F.O., Marmontel, M. & Borge, J.C.G. (2021). Spatiotemporal dynamics of mangrove forest and association with strandings of Antillean manatee (Trichechus manatus) calves in Paraíba, Brazil. — J. Mar. Biol. Ass. UK 101: 503-510.
Merchan, F., Echevers, G., Poveda, H., Sanchez-Galan, J.E. & Guzman, H.M. (2019). Detection and identification of manatee individual vocalisations in Panamanian wetlands using spectrogram clustering. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 146: 1745-1757.
Miksis-Olds, J.L. & Tyack, P.L. (2009). Manatee (Trichechus manatus) vocalisation usage in relation to environmental noise levels. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1125: 1806-1815.
Mitani, J.C. & Brandt, K.L. (1994). Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long- distance calls of male chimpanzees. — Etholog. 96: 233-252.
Morisaka, T., Yoshida, Y., Akune, Y., Mishima, H. & Nishimoto, S. (2013). Exchange of “signature” calls in captive belugas (Delphinapterus leucas). — J. Ethol. 31: 141-149.
Murie, J. (1872). On the form and structure of the manatee (Manatus americanus). — Trans. Zool. Soc. Lond. 8: 127-202.
Newman, J. (2004). The primate isolation call: a comparison with precocial birds and non-primate mammals. — In: Comparative vertebrate cognition: are primates superior to non-primates? (Rogers, L.J. & Kaplan, G., eds). Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York, NY, pp. 171-187.
Newman, J.D. (2007). Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals. — Behav. Brain Res. 182: 155-165.
Noren, D.P., Holt, M.M., Dunkin, R.C. & Williams, T.M. (2013). The metabolic cost of communicative sound production in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). — J. Exp. Biol. 261(9): 1624-1629.
Nousek, A.E., Slater, P.J.B., Wang, C. & Miller, P.J.O. (2006). The influence of social affiliation on individual vocal signatures of northern resident killer whales. — Biol. Lett. 2: 481-484.
Nuñez, C.M.V. & Rubenstein, D.I. (2020). Communication is key: mother-offspring signalling can affect behavioural responses and offspring survival in feral horses (Equus caballus). — PLoS ONE 15: e0231343.
Oberwerger, K. & Goller, F. (2001). The metabolic cost of birdsong production. — J. Exp. Biol. 204: 3378-3388.
Okabe, S., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K. & Kikusui, T. (2012). The importance of mother–infant communication for social bond formation in mammals. — Anim. Sci. J. 83: 446-452.
Ophir, A.G., Schrader, S.B. & Gillooly, J.F. (2010). Energetic cost of calling: general constraints and species specific differences. — J. Evol. Biol. 23(7): 1564-1569.
O’Shea, T.J. & Poché, L.B. (2006). Aspects of underwater sound communication in Florida manatees (Trichechus manatus latirostris). — J. Mammol. 87: 1061-1071.
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A. & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalisations into context-specific call sequences. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106: 22026-22031.
Peckre, L., Kappeler, P.M. & Fichtel, C. (2019). Clarifying and expanding the social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73: 11.
Ramírez-Jiménez, H.H., Olivera-Gómez, L.D. & Cueva, H. (2017). Habitat use by the Antillean manatee (Trichechus manatus) during an extreme dry season in an urban lake in Tabasco, Mexico. — Therya 8: 3-18.
Ramos, E.A., Maust-Mohl, M., Collom, K.A., Brady, B., Gerstein, E.R., Magnasco, M.O. & Reis, D. (2020). The Antillean manatee produces broadband vocalizations with ultrasonic frequencies. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 147: EL80-EL86.
Rathbun, G.B., Reid, J.P., Bonde, R.K. & Powell, J.A. (1995). Reproduction in free-ranging Florida manatees. U.S. fish and wildlife service information and technology report 1. — U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Washington, DC.
Rekdahl, M.L., Dunlop, R.A. & Noad, M.J. (2013). Temporal stability and change in the social call repertoire of migrating humpback whales. — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133: 1785-1795.
Rendell, L., Cantor, M., Gero, S., Whitehead, H. & Mann, J. (2019). Causes and consequences of female centrality in cetacean societies. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 374: 20180066.
Santos, S.S., Medeiros, S.I., Rebelo, V.A., Carvalho, A.O.B., Debut, J.P., Mantovani, J.E., Círiaco, R.D., Santos, R.E.G., Marmontel, M., Noramande, I.C., Velôsos, T.M.G. & Borges, J.C.G. (2022). Home ranges of released West Indian manatees Trichechus manatus. — Oryx 56: 1-8.
Sayigh, L.S., Tyack, P., Wells, R., Scott, M. & Irvine, A.B. (1995). Sex difference in signature whistle production of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncates. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36: 171-177.
Shapiro, A.D. (2006). Preliminary evidence for signature vocalisations among free-ranging narwhals (Monodon monoceros). — J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120: 1695-1705.
Slocombe, K.E., Townsend, S.W. & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) distinguish between different scream types: evidence from a playback study. — Anim. Cogn. 12: 441-449.
Sousa-Lima, R.S., Paglia, A.P. & Da Fonseca, G.A.B. (2002). Signature information and individual recognition in the isolation calls of Amazonian manatees, Trichechus inunguis (Mammalia: Sirenia). — Anim. Behav. 63: 301-310.
Sousa-Lima, R.S., Paglia, A.P. & da Fonseca, G.A.B. (2008). Gender, age, and identity in the isolation calls of Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus). — Aquat. Mamm. 34(1): 109-122.
Tibetts, E. & Dale, J. (2007). Individual recognition: it is good to be different. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 22: 529-537.
Tyack, P.L. (1986). Population biology, social behavior and communication in whales and dolphins. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 1: 144-150.
Umeed, R. (2016). Comportamento vocal em Trichechus manatus mantus. Masters dissertation. — Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, Recife.
Umeed, R., Attademo, F.L.N. & Bezerra, B. (2018). The influence of age and sex on the vocal repertoire of the Antillean manatee (Trichechus manatus manatus) and their responses to call playback. — Mar. Mamm. Sci. 34: 577-594.
Vasconcelos, R.O., Carriço, R., Ramos, A., Modesto, T., Fonesca, P.J. & Amorim, M.P.C. (2012). Vocal behaviour predicts reproductive success in a teleost fish. — Behav. Ecol. 23: 375-383.
von Fersen, L. & Walb, R. (2018). EAZA Best Practice Guidelines for the Antillean manatee. — EAZA Executive Office, Amsterdam.
Weilgart, L.S. & Whitehead, H. (1988). Distinctive vocalisations from mature male sperm whales (Physeter microcephalus). — Can. J. Zool. 66: 1931-1937.
Zeifman, D.M. (2001). An ethological analysis of human infant crying: answering Tinbergen’s four questions. — Dev. Psychobiol. 39: 265-285.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 384 | 132 | 11 |
Full Text Views | 211 | 9 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 350 | 20 | 0 |
Vocal complexity can be expressed through variations in repertoire size, structure, and individual manatee repertoires. Here we aimed to assess the complexity of the vocal behaviour of Antillean manatees living in captivity (i.e., artificial pools) and in reintroduction enclosures (i.e., natural enclosures placed in an estuarine area). Specifically, we evaluated: (i) the structure of vocalisations to assess whether they had variants; (ii) the variation in call production (rate and pattern) between groups with different configurations; (iii) whether individuality occurred in vocalisation structure. We found four categories of vocalisations, of which two had different variants. Not all study groups produced all call categories and variants. Older and younger males in the reintroduction enclosures had the highest call rates compared to captive females and captive males. The vocal and behavioural patterns differed between groups. Squeak call structure differed between individuals. Such vocal complexity may aid manatees in adapting to their dynamic social and structural environment, facilitating communication.
A complexidade vocal pode ser expressa através de variações no tamanho do repertório vocal, na sua estrutura e individualidade. Aqui objetivamos avaliar a complexidade no comportamento vocal de peixes-boi-marinhos vivendo em cativeiro (i.e., piscinas artificiais) e em recintos de reintrodução (i.e., recintos naturais em uma área estuarina). Especificamente, investigamos: (i) a estrutura das vocalizações para avaliar se apresentavam variantes; (ii) a variação na produção de vocalizações (taxa e padrão) entre grupos com diferentes configurações; (iii) se a individualidade ocorre na estrutura de vocalização. Encontramos quatro categorias de vocalizações, das quais duas possuíam variantes. Nem todos os grupos de estudo produziram todas as categorias de vocalizações e suas variantes. Os machos mais velhos e os mais jovens nos recintos de reintrodução tiveram as maiores taxas de vocalizações em comparação com as fêmeas e machos cativos. Os padrões vocais e comportamentais diferiram entre os grupos. A estrutura física da vocalização Squeak diferiu entre os indivíduos. Essa complexidade vocal pode ajudar os peixes-bois-marinhos a se adaptarem ao seu ambiente social e estrutural dinâmico, facilitando a comunicação.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 384 | 132 | 11 |
Full Text Views | 211 | 9 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 350 | 20 | 0 |