Finger Counting Habits and Spatial-Numerical Association in Horizontal and Vertical Orientations

in Journal of Cognition and Culture
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Abstract

The SNARC effect reflects an association between space and number representations, suggesting a mental number line. Horizontally, the number line is oriented from left-to-right while vertically, the number line is oriented from bottom-to-top. The horizontal SNARC effect seems to be related to finger counting habits, especially in left-hand starters. The present study aimed to investigate how finger counting habits affected the horizontal and vertical SNARC effect in parity and magnitude comparing tasks carried out by Italian adults. After a finger counting questionnaire, left- and right-starters were selected. Participants performed parity and magnitude comparing tasks with horizontal and vertical response key assignments. In both tasks, the SNARC effect was found for both spatial orientations. However, right-starters exhibited significant regression slopes compared to left-starters in all conditions. The results are discussed taking into account the relationship between finger counting habits and SNARC effect in horizontal and vertical orientations.

Finger Counting Habits and Spatial-Numerical Association in Horizontal and Vertical Orientations

in Journal of Cognition and Culture

Sections

References

AndresM.DavareM.PesentiM.OlivierE.SeronX., 'Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction' (2004) 15 NeuroReport: 2773-2777.

AndresM.OstryD. J.NicolF.PausT., 'Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping' (2008) 44 Cortex: 414-419.

BentonA. L., 'The fiction of the “Gerstmann syndrome”' (1961) 24 Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry: 176-181.

ButterworthB., The Mathematical Brain, (Macmillan, London 1999).

CappellettiM.FreemanE. D.CipolottiL., 'The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect' (2007) 45 Neuropsychologia: 2989-3000.

ConantL. L., The Number Concept, (MacMillan, New York, NY 1896).

DaarM.PrattJ., 'Digits affect actions: The SNARC effect and response selection' (2008) 44 Cortex: 400-405.

DehaeneS., The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics, (Oxford University Press, Oxford 1997).

DehaeneS.DupouxE.MehlerJ., 'Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison' (1990) 16 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance: 626-641.

DehaeneS.BossiniS.GirauxP., 'The mental representation of parity and number magnitude' (1993) 122 Journal of Experimental Psychology: General: 371-396.

de HeviaM. D.GirelliL.VallarG., 'Numbers and space: a cognitive illusion?' (2006) 158 Experimental Brain Research: 254-264.

de HeviaM. D.VallarG.GirelliL., 'Visualizing numbers in the mind’s eye: The role of visuo-spatial processes in numerical abilities' (2008) 32 Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews: 1361-1372.

Di LucaS.PesentiM., 'Masked priming effect with canonical finger numeral configurations' (2008) 185 Experimental Brain Research: 27-39.

Di LucaS.GranaA.SemenzaC.SeronX.PesentiM., 'Finger-digit compatibility in Arabic numerical processing' (2006) 59 The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: 1648-1663.

DomahsF.KrinzingerH.WillmesK., ' Mind the gap between both hands: evidence for internal finger-based number representations in children’s mental calculation' (2008) 44 Cortex: 359-367.

FiasW.BrysbaertM.GeypensF.d’YewalleG., 'The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect' (1996) 2 Mathematical Cognition: 95-110.

FiasW.FischerM. H., 'Spatial representation of numbers', in J. I. D. Campbell (ed), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, (Psychology Press, New York, NY 2005) 43-54.

FischerM. H., 'The future for SNARC could be stark' (2006) 42 Cortex: 1066-1068.

FischerM. H., 'Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations' (2008) 44 Cortex: 386-392.

FusonK. C., Children’s Counting and Concepts of Number, (Springer, New York, NY 1988).

Garcia-BafalluyM.NoëlM. P., 'Does finger training increase numerical performance?' (2008) 44 Cortex: 368-375.

GerstmannJ., 'Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for left and right, agraphia and acalculia' (1940) 44 Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry: 398-408.

GeversW.LammertynJ., 'The hunt for the SNARC' (2005) 47 Psychology Science: 10-21.

GeversW.LammertynJ.NotebaertW.VergutsT.FiasW., 'Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect' (2006) 122 Acta Psychologica: 221-233.

HeimburgerR.DemyerW.ReitanR., 'Implications of Gerstmann’s syndrome' (1964) 27 Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry: 52-57.

HubbardE. M.PiazzaM.PinelP.DehaeneS., 'Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex' (2005) 6 Nature Reviews Neuroscience: 435-448.

HungY. H.HungD. L.TzengO. J.-L.WuD. H., 'Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers' (2008) 106 Cognition: 1441-1450.

ItoY.HattaT., 'Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect' (2004) 32 Memory and Cognition: 662-673.

KaanE., 'Direction effects in number word comparison: An event-related potential study' (2005) 16 NeuroReport: 1853-1856.

KinsbourneM.WarringtonE. K., 'The developmental Gerstmann Syndrome' (1963) 8 Annals of Neurology: 490-501.

LindermannO.AbolafiaJ. M.GirardiG.BekkeringH., 'Getting a grip on numbers. Numerical magnitude priming in object grasping' (2007) 33 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance: 1400-1409.

LorchR. F.JrMyersJ. L., 'Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research' (1990) 16 Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning Memory and Cognition: 149-157.

MoyerR. S.LandauerT. K., 'Time required for judgements of numerical inequality' (1967) 215 Nature: 1519-1520.

MüllerD.SchwarzW., 'Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect' (2007) 35 Memory and Cognition: 1151-1161.

NoëlM. P., 'Finger gnosia: A predictor of numerical abilities in children?' (2005) 11 Child Neuropsychology: 413-430.

OldfieldR. C., 'The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory' (1971) 9 Neuropsychologia: 97-113.

RestleF., 'Speed of adding and comparing numbers' (1970) 83 Journal of Experimental Psychology: 274-278.

RossettiY.Jacquin-CourtoisS.RodeG.OtaH.MichelC.BoissonD., 'Does action make the link between number and space representation?' (2004) 15 Psychological Science: 426-430.

RourkeB. P., 'Arithmetic disabilities, specific and otherwise: A neuropsychological perspective' (1993) 26 Jurnal of Learning Disabilities: 26-241.

RouxF. E.BoettoS.SackoO.CholletF.TremouletM., 'Wriing, calculating, and finger recognition in the region of the angular gyrus. A cortical stimulation study of Gerstmann syndrome' (2003) 99 Journal of Neurosurgery: 716-727.

RusconiE.WalshV.ButterworthB., 'Dexterity with numbers: rTMS over left angular gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing' (2005) 43 Neuropsychologia: 1609-1624.

SandriniM.RusconiE., 'A brain for numbers' (2009) 45 Cortex: 796-803.

SankohA. J.HuqueM. F.DubeyS. D., 'Some comments on frequently used multiple endpoint adjustment methods in clinical trials' (1997) 16 Statistics in Medicine: 2529-2542.

SatoM.LalainM., 'On the relationship between handedness and hand-digit mapping in finger counting' (2008) 44 Cortex: 393-399.

SatoM.CattaneoL.RizzolattiG.GalleseV., 'Numbers within our hands: modulation of corticospinal excitabiliy of hand muscles during numerical judgment' (2007) 19 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience: 684-693.

SchneiderW.EschmanA.ZuccolottoA., E-Prime user’s guide, (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA 2002).

SchwarzW.KeusI. M., 'Moving the eyes along the mental number line: comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses' (2004) 66 Perception and Psychophysics: 651-664.

SimonO.ManginJ.-F.CohenL.Le BihanD.DehaeneS., 'Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe' (2002) 33 Neuron: 475-487.

UmiltàC.PriftisK.ZorziM., 'The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect' (2009) 192 Experimental Brain Research: 561-569.

VallarG., 'Spatial neglect, Balint-Holmes’ and Gerstmann’s syndromes and other spatial disorders' (2007) 12 CNS Spectrums: 527-536.

VuK.-P. L.ProctorR. W., 'The prevalence effect in two-dimensional stimulus-response compatibility is a function of the relative salience of the dimensions' (2002) 64 Perception and Psychophysics: 815-828.

VuilleumierP.OrtigueS.BruggerP., 'The number space and neglect' (2004) 40 Cortex: 399-410.

WoodG.FischerM. H., 'Numbers, space, and action – From finger counting to the mental number line and beyond' (2008) 44 Cortex: 353-358.

WoodG.WillmesK.NuerkH.-C.FischerM. H., 'On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect' (2008) 50 Psychology Science Quarterly: 489-525.

ZebianS., 'Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers' (2005) 5 Journal of Cognition and Culture: 166-190.

ZorziM.PriftisK.UmiltàC., 'Neglect disrupts the mental number line' (2002) 417 Nature: 138-139.

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 45 45 14
Full Text Views 12 12 5
PDF Downloads 10 10 8
EPUB Downloads 2 2 2