The Mnemonic of Intuitive Ontology Violation is Not the Distinctiveness Effect: Evidence From a Broad Age Spectrum of Persons in the uk and China During a Free-Recall Task

in Journal of Cognition and Culture
No Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

Abstract

The typical formulation of Pascal Boyer’s counterintuitiveness theory asserts that concepts violating intuitive ontological-category structures are more memorable. However, Boyer’s (2001) original claim centered on the transmission advantages of counter-ontological representations that were cultural. Nevertheless, subsequent studies focused on the recall of novel counterintuitive representations, and an “alternative account” of the memorability of counterintuitive concepts has emerged resembling the distinctiveness effect (Upal, 2010). Yet, experimental evidence shows that familiar concepts have memorability advantages over novel ones (Anaki & Bentin, 2009; Ingram, Mickes, & Wixted, 2011). This investigation of these pan-cultural transmission biases used a large age-representative sample (13–86 years; N = 365) in the uk and China. Results were analyzed by hlm, with familiarity, counterintuitiveness, and delay as 2-level fixed factors, and age as a covariate. No support was revealed for the typical formulation of the hypothesis — however, a significant age effect and interaction of familiarity x counterintuitiveness were found.

The Mnemonic of Intuitive Ontology Violation is Not the Distinctiveness Effect: Evidence From a Broad Age Spectrum of Persons in the uk and China During a Free-Recall Task

in Journal of Cognition and Culture

Sections

References

AnakiD. & BentinS. (2009) Familiarity effects on categorization levels of faces and objects. Cognition111144149.

BaddeleyA.EysenckM. W. & AndersonM. C. (2009). Memory. New York: Psychology Press.

BanerjeeK.HaqueO. S. & SpelkeE. S. (2013). Melting lizards and crying mailboxes: Children’s preferential recall of minimally counterintuitive concepts. Cognitive Science37139.

BarrettJ. L. (2008). Coding and quantifying counterintuitiveness: Theoretical and methodological reflections. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion20308338.

BarrettJ. L. & NyhofM. A. (2001). Spreading non-natural Concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition & Culture169100.

BlochM. (2005). Essays on cultural transmission. New York: Berg.

BoyerP. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley: University of California Press.

BoyerP. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.

BoyerP. & RambleC. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science25535564.

BrimerR. W. & MuellerJ. H. (1979). Immediate and final recall of pictures and words with written or oral tests. The American Journal of Psychology93437447.

GonceL. O.UpalM. A.SloneD. J. & TweneyR. D. (2006). Role of context in memory recall of counterintuitive concepts. Journal of Cognition & Culture6521547.

GreenD. M. & SwetsJ. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

GregoryJ. P. & GreenwayT.S. (2017). Is there a window of opportunity for religiosity? Children and adolescents preferentially recall religious-type cultural representations, but older adults do not. Religion Brain & Behavior7(2) 98116. doi:10.1080/2153599X.2016.1196234

GregoryJ. P. & BarrettJ. L. (2009). Epistemology and counterintuitiveness: Role and relationship in epidemiology of cultural representations. Journal of Cognition and Culture9289314.

HallJ. F. (1954). Learning as a function of word frequency. The American Journal of Psychology67138140.

HildyardA. & OlsonD. R. (1982). On the comprehension and memory of oral vs. written discourse. In D. Tannen (Ed.) Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy (pp. 1933). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

HirshmanE.WhelleyM. N. & PalijM. (1989). An investigation of paradoxical memory effects. Journal of Memory and Language28594609.

HuntR. (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von Restorff really did. Psychonomic Bulleting and Review2105112.

IngramK. M.MickesL. & WixtedJ. T. (2011). Recollection can be weak and familiarity can be strong. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition38325339.

JohnsonC. V. M.KellyS. W. & BishopP. (2010) Measuring the mnemonic advantage of counter intuitive and counter schematic concepts. Journal of Cognition and Culture10111123.

KeilF. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

LegareC. H.ZhuL. & WellmanH. M. (2013). Examining biological explanations in Chinese preschool children: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Cognition and Culture136793.

NairneJ. S.VanArrsdallJ. E.PanderiadaJ. N. S.CogdillM. & LeBreton J. M. (2013). Adaptive memory: The mnemonic value of animacy. Psychological Science2420992105.

NisbettR. E. (2003). The geography of thought. New York: Free Press.

NisbettR. E.PengK.ChoiC. & NorenzayanA. (2001). Culture and thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review108291310.

Norenzayan. A.AtranS.FaulknerJ. & SchallerM. (2006). Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science30531553.

SchmidtS. R. (1985). Encoding and retrieval processes in the memory of conceptually distinctive events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition11565578.

SpelkeE. S. (1991). Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Piaget’s theory. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.) Epigenesis of mind: Studies in biology and cognition (pp. 133170. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

SperberD. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

SumbyW. H. (1963). Word frequency and serial position effects. Journal of Oral Learning and Oral Behavior1443450.

SurprenantA. M. & NeathI. (2009). Principles of memory. New York: Psychology Press.

UpalM. A. (2010). An alternative account of the minimal counterintuitiveness effect. Cognitive Systems Research11194203.

WaddillP. J. & McDanielM. A. (1998). Distinctiveness effects in recall: Differential processing or privileged retrieval? Memory and Cognition26108120.

> WangQ. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-construct: Autobiographical memory and self-description in American and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology40315.

WellmanH. M.CrossD. & WatsonJ. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development72655684.

Figures

  • View in gallery
  • View in gallery
  • View in gallery
    An example of a familiar intuitive-congruent (fINT) stimulus used for the writtenmaterials presentation. This accompanying image did not directly depict the meaning of the modified concept with reference to the subject-predicate statement component of the stimulus; in all cases the image displayed lacked any modifiers, even though the actual stimulus included a modifier (e.g. the image for “a frog that is jumping” would show not a jumping frog, but a stationary frog that persons would typically entertain upon encountering FROG).
  • View in gallery
    Bars representing mean percentage of intuitive-congruent stimuli (int) and counterintuitive stimuli (mci) recalled immediately following a distractor task and again after a one-week period of delay [error bars +/− 2SE].
  • View in gallery
    Bars represent mean percentage of recall rates of familiar stimuli (fMCI and fINT) and unfamiliar stimuli (uMCI and uINT) immediately following a distractor task and again after a one-week period of delay [error bars +/− 2SE].
  • View in gallery
    Bars representing mean percentage of recall rate of familiar counterintuitive stimuli (fMCI) and unfamiliar counterintuitive stimuli (uMCI) immediately following a distractor task and again after a one-week period of delay [error bars +/− 2SE].

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 28 28 4
Full Text Views 154 154 65
PDF Downloads 7 7 3
EPUB Downloads 2 2 2