Thinking Materially: Cognition as Extended and Enacted

In: Journal of Cognition and Culture
View More View Less
  • 1 Center for Cognitive Archaeology, University of Colorado, Colorado Springs

Login via Institution

Purchase instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):

€25.00$30.00

Abstract

Human cognition is extended and enacted. Drawing the boundaries of cognition to include the resources and attributes of the body and materiality allows an examination of how these components interact with the brain as a system, especially over cultural and evolutionary spans of time. Literacy and numeracy provide examples of multigenerational, incremental change in both psychological functioning and material forms. Though we think materiality, its central role in human cognition is often unappreciated, for reasons that include conceptual distribution over multiple material forms, the unconscious transparency of cognitive activity in general, and the different temporalities of metaplastic change in neurons and cultural forms.

  • Amiet, P. (1972). Mémoires de la délégation archéologique en Iran, Tome XLIII, Mission de Susiane (Vol. I). Paris: Librairie Orientaliste Paul Geuthner.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

  • Beller, S., & Bender, A. (2011). Explicating numerical information: When and how fingers support (or hinder) number comprehension and handling. Frontiers in Cognition, 2, 13.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2011). Fingers as a tool for counting: Naturally fixed or culturally flexible? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 12.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blažek, V. (1999). Numerals: Comparative etymological analyses and their implications: Saharan, Nubian, Egyptian, Berber, Kartvelian, Uralic, Altaic and Indo-European languages. Brno, Czech Republic: Masarykova Univerzita.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bramanti, A. (2015). Rethinking the writing space: Anatomy of some early dynastic signs. In E. Devecchi, G. G. W. Müller, & J. Mynářová (Eds.), Current research in cuneiform palaeography: Proceedings of the workshop organised at the 60th Rencontre Assyriologique Internationale, Warsaw 2014 (pp. 3147). Gladbeck, Germany: PeWe-Verlag.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brannon, E. M. (2006). The representation of numerical magnitude. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 222229.

  • Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 3641.

  • Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273293.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Butterworth, B., Reeve, R., Reynolds, F., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Numerical thought with and without words: Evidence from indigenous Australian children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(35), 1317913184. doi:10.1073/pnas.0806045105

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chrisomalis, S. (2005). Evaluating ancient numeracy: Social versus developmental perspectives on ancient Mesopotamian numeration. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society (Vancouver, British Columbia).

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chrisomalis, S. (2010). Numerical notation: A comparative history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. J. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 719.

  • Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: The case for the visual word form area. NeuroImage, 22, 466476.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coinman, N. R. (1996). Worked bone in the Levantine Upper Paleolithic: Rare examples from the Wadi al-Hasa, West-Central Jordan. Paléorient, 22(2), 113121. doi:10.3406/paleo.1996.4638

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Comrie, B. (1989). Language universals and linguistic typology (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

  • Coolidge,F. L., & Wynn, T. (2009). The rise of Homo sapiens: The evolution of modern thinking. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Cooper, J. S. (1996). Sumerian and Akkadian. In P. T. Daniels & W. Bright (Eds.), The world’s writing systems (pp. 3757). New York: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cooper, J. S. (2004). Babylonian beginnings: The origin of the cuneiform writing system in comparative perspective. In S. D. Houston (Ed.), The first writing: Script invention as history and process (pp. 7199). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Damerow, P. (1996). Prehistory and cognitive development. In M. Killen (Ed.), Invited Lecture at the Twenty-Fifth Annual Symposium of the Jean Piaget Society Berkeley, June 1–June 3, 1995. (pp. 137). Berlin: Max Planck Institute for the History of Science.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Damerow, P. (2007). The material culture of calculation: A theoretical framework for a historical epistemology for the concept of number. In U. Gellert & E. Jablonka (Eds.), Mathematisation and demathematisation: Social, philosophical and educational ramifications (pp. 1956). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Damerow, P. (2010). Abstraction and representation: Essays on the cultural evolution of thinking. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56(2), 384398.

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254262.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 234244.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Spelke, E., & Pica, P. (2008). Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures. Science, 320(5880), 12171220.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Jobert, A., …Cohen, L., (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330(6009), 13591364.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1987). A thousand plateaus: Capitalism and schizophrenia. (B. Massumi, Trans.). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Derrida, J. (1974). Of grammatology. (G. C. Spivak, Trans.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

  • Devlin, K. (2003). Mathematics: The science of patterns: The search for order in life, mind and the universe. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • di Paolo, E. A., Rohde, M., & de Jaegher, H. (2010). Horizons for the enactive mind: Values, social interaction, and play. In J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, & E. A. di Paolo (Eds.), Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science (pp. 3387). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Diakonoff, I. M. (1983). Some reflections on numerals in Sumerian: Towards a history of mathematical speculation. Journal of the American Oriental Society, 103(1), 8492.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Divale, W. (1999). Climatic instability, food storage, and the development of numerical counting: A cross-cultural study. Cross-Cultural Research, 33(4), 341368. doi:10.1177/106939719903300403

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Edzard, D. O. (1980). Sumerisch 1 bis 10 in Ebla. Studi Eblaiti, III(5–8), 121127.

  • Englund, R. K. (1991). Archaic dairy metrology. Iraq, 53, 101104.

  • Englund, R. K. (1998). Texts from the Late Uruk period. In Mesopotamien: Späturuk-Zeit und Frühdynastische Zeit (pp. 15233). Freiburg, Switzerland: Universitätsverlag.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Englund, R. K. (2001). Grain accounting practices in archaic Mesopotamia. Changing views on ancient Near Eastern mathematics. Berliner Beiträge Zum Vorderen Orient, 19 135.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44(4), 386392.

  • Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2008). Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition, 108(3), 819824.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gall, F. J. (1835). On the functions of the brain and of each of its parts: With observations of the possibility of determining the instincts, propensities, and talents, or the moral and intellectual dispositions of men and animals, by the configuration of the brain and head. (W. Lewis Jr, Trans.) (Vol. 1). Boston, MA: Marsh, Capen and Lyon.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 92409245.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: The President and Fellows of Harvard College.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Giovanni, F. B. d’Arcais. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 6(4), 337355.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science, 306(5695), 496499. doi:10.1126/science.1094492

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gowers, T. (Ed.). (2008). The Princeton companion to mathematics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Greenberg, J. H. (1978). Generalizations about numeral systems. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language (Vol. 3, pp. 249295). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Haas, C. (1996). Writing technology: Studies on the materiality of literacy. New York: Routledge.

  • Hodder, I. (2012). Entangled: An archaeology of the relationships between humans and things. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Høyrup, J. (2002). Lengths, widths, surfaces: A portrait of Old Babylonian algebra and its kin. New York: Springer.

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Hutchins, E. (1999). Cognitive artifacts. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 15551577.

  • James, K. H., & Engelhardt, L. (2012). The effects of handwriting experience on functional brain development in pre-literate children. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1(1), 3242.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237(4821), 14451452.

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1989). Cognition, unconscious processes. In G. Adelman (Ed.), Neuroscience year: Supplement 1 to the encyclopedia of neuroscience (pp. 3436). Boston, MA: Birkhauser.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Krispijn, T. J. H. (2012). Writing Semitic with cuneiform script: The interaction of Sumerian and Akkadian orthography in the second half of the third millennium BC. In A. de Voogt & J. F. Quack (Eds.), The idea of writing: Writing across borders (pp. 181218). Leiden, The Netherlands: Koninklijke Brill NV.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M.-T., & Velay, J.-L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: A comparison between handwriting and typing. Acta Psychologica, 119(1), 6779.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Malafouris, L. (2008). Beads for a plastic mind: The “blind man”s stick’ (bms) hypothesis and the active nature of material culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 18(3), 401414.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Malafouris, L. (2010). Metaplasticity and the human becoming: Principles of neuroarchaeology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 88, 4972.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the primacy of material engagement. Time and Mind, 8(4), 351371.

  • Marshack, A. (1991). The roots of civilization: The cognitive beginnings of man’s first art, symbol and notation (Revised an.). New York: Moyer Bell Ltd.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Menninger, K. (1992). Number words and number symbols: A cultural history of numbers. (P. Broneer, Trans.). New York: Dover.

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (2012). Phenomenology of perception. (D. Landes, Trans.). London: Routledge.

  • Moscati, M. F. (1832). History and conversion of an anti-phrenologist. The Lancet, 1(480), 213216.

  • Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard Advantages of longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological Science, 25(6), 11591168.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nakamura, K., Kuo, W.-J., Pegado, F., Cohen, L., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Dehaene, S. (2012). Universal brain systems for recognizing word shapes and handwriting gestures during reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(50), 2076220767.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nayak, S., Wheeler, B. L., Shiflett, S. C., & Agostinelli, S. (2000). Effect of music therapy on mood and social interaction among individuals with acute traumatic brain injury and stroke. Rehabilitation Psychology, 45(3), 274283.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nissen, H. J., Damerow, P., & Englund, R. K. (1993). Archaic bookkeeping: Early writing and techniques of economic administration in the ancient Near East. (P. Larsen, Trans.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Núñez, R. E., Cooperrider, K., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Number concepts without number lines in an indigenous group of Papua New Guinea. PLoS One, 7(4), e35662. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0035662

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Olson, D. R. (2013). Literacy and the languages of rationality. Pragmatics & Cognition, 21(3), 431447.

  • Overmann, K. A. (2013a). Material scaffolds in numbers and time. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23(01), 1939.

  • Overmann, K. A. (2013b). Numbers and time: A cross-cultural investigation of the origin and use of numbers as a cognitive technology. Master’s thesis, Department of Psychology. University of Colorado, Colorado Springs, CO.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Overmann, K. A. (2015). Numerosity structures the expression of quantity in lexical numbers and grammatical number. Current Anthropology, 56(5), 638653.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Overmann, K. A. (2016a). Beyond writing: The development of literacy in the Ancient Near East. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 26(2), 285303.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Overmann, K. A. (2016b). Number concepts are constructed through material engagement: A reply to Sutliff, Read, and Everett. Current Anthropology, 57(3), 352356.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Overmann, K. A. (2016c). The role of materiality in numerical cognition. Quaternary International, 405, 4251.

  • Pegado, F., Nakamura, K., & Hannagan, T. (2014). How does literacy break mirror invariance in the visual system? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 15.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(1), 324.

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L.-H. (2013). Write to read: The brain’s universal reading and writing network. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(2), 5657.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Piazza, M., Pica, P., Izard, V., Spelke, E. S., & Dehaene, S. (2013). Education enhances the acuity of the nonverbal approximate number system. Psychological Science, 24(6), 10371043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, 306(5695), 499503. doi:10.1126/science.1102085

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Postgate, J. N. (2013). Bronze Age bureaucracy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Previtali, P., Rinaldi, L., & Girelli, L. (2011). Nature or nurture in finger counting: A review on the determinants of the direction of number-finger mapping. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reese, D. S. (2002). On the incised cattle scapulae from the East Mediterranean and Near East. Bonner Zoologische Beitrage, 50, 183198.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rips, L. J., Bloomfield, A., & Asmuth, J. (2008). From numerical concepts to concepts of number. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(6), 623642. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08005566

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robson, E. (2008). Mathematics in ancient Iraq: A social history. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Russell, B. (1910). The theory of logical types. In D. Lackey (Ed.), Essays in analysis (pp. 215252). New York: George Braziller.

  • Russell, B. (1920). Introduction to mathematical philosophy (2nd ed.). London: George Allen & Unwin, Ltd.

  • Schmandt-Besserat, D. (1992). Before writing: From counting to cuneiform (2 vols.). Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

  • Senner, W. M. (1989). Theories and myths on the origins of writing: A historical overview. In W. M. Senner (Ed.), The origins of writing (pp. 126). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development, 75(2), 428444. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2004.00684.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Studevent-Hickman, B. (2007). The ninety-degree rotation of the cuneiform script. In J. Cheng & M. H. Feldman (Eds.), Ancient Near Eastern art in context: Studies in honor of Irene J. Winter by her students (pp. 486513). Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sülzenbrück, S., Hegele, M., Rinkenauer, G., & Heuer, H. (2011). The death of handwriting: Secondary effects of frequent computer use on basic motor skills. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43(3), 247251.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Taylor, J. (2015). Wedge order in cuneiform: A preliminary survey. In E. Devecchi, G. G. W. Müller, & J. Mynářová (Eds.), Current research in cuneiform palaeography: Proceedings of the workshop organised at the 60th Rencontre Assyriologique Internationale, Warsaw 2014 (pp. 130). Gladbeck, Germany: PeWe-Verlag.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratnet, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495552.

  • Tovino, S. A. (2007). Imaging body structure and mapping brain function: A historical approach. American Journal of Law and Medicine, 33(2&3), 193228.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., Boom, J., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2012). The development of executive functions and early mathematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 100119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Veldhuis, N. (2011). Levels of literacy. In K. Radner & E. Robson (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cuneiform culture (pp. 6889). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Veldhuis, N. (2014). History of the cuneiform lexical tradition. Münster: Ugarit-Verlag.

  • Vogel, A. C., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2014). The vwfa: It’s not just for words anymore. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 110.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wilcke, C. (2005). ED Lú a und die Sprache(n) der archaischen Texte. In W. H. van Soldt (Ed.), Ethnicity in ancient Mesopotamia: Papers read at the 48th Recontre Assyriologique International, Leiden, 1–4 July 2002 (pp. 430445). Leiden, The Netherlands: Netherlands Institute.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wynn, T. (2002). Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(3), 389402.

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 158 93 10
Full Text Views 215 18 0
PDF Downloads 45 15 0