Tunes and Tones: Music, Language, and Inhibitory Control

In: Journal of Cognition and Culture
View More View Less
  • 1 Southern Illinois University Carbondale, IL, USA
  • 2 Southern Illinois University Carbondale, IL, USA

Login via Institution

Purchase instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):

€25.00$30.00

Abstract

A debate is underway regarding the perceptual and cognitive benefits of bilingualism and musical experience. This study contributes to the debate by investigating auditory inhibitory control in English-speaking monolingual musicians, non-musicians, tone language bilinguals, and non-tone language bilinguals. We predicted that musicians and tone language bilinguals would demonstrate enhanced processing relative to monolinguals and other bilinguals. Groups of monolinguals (N = 22), monolingual musicians (N = 19), non-tone language bilinguals (N = 20) and tone language bilinguals (N = 18) were compared on auditory Stroop tasks to assess domain-transferable processing benefits (e.g. auditory inhibitory control) resulting from potentially shared underlying cognitive mechanisms (Patel, 2003; Bialystok & DePape, 2009). In one task, participants heard the words “high” and “low” presented in high or low pitches, and responded regarding the pitch of the stimuli as quickly as possible. In another task, participants heard the words “rise” or “fall” presented in rising or falling pitch contours, and responded regarding the contour of the stimuli as quickly as possible. Results suggest transferable auditory inhibitory control benefits for musicians across pitch and contour processing, but any possible enhanced processing for speakers of tone languages may be task-dependent, as lexical tone activation may interfere with pitch contour processing.

  • Alexander, J. A., Bradlow, A. R., Ashley, R. D., & Wong, P. C. (2011). Music-melody perception in tone-language and non-tone-language speakers. Psycholinguistic representation of tone. Hong Kong.http://www.sfu. ca/~ jaa37/index files/Alexander et al.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bedny, M., Richardson, H., & Saxe, R. (2015). “Visual” cortex responds to spoken language in blind children. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(33), 1167411681.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bialystok, E., Martin, M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bialystok, E., & DePape, A. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 565574.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T., & Krishnan, A. (2011). Musicians and tone-language speakers share enhanced brainstem encoding but not perceptual benefits for musical pitch. Brain and cognition, 77(1), 110.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blumenfeld, H., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(5), 633660.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Burnham, D., & Mattock, K. (2007). The perception of tones and phones. In O-S. Bohn & M. J. Munro (Eds.) Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (259279). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Co.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chandrasekaran, B., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T. (2007). Mismatch negativity to pitch contours is influenced by language experience. Brain research, 1128(1), 148156.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., & Misura, N. M. (2013). Music Training, Cognition, and Personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 222.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Dolson, M. (2004). Absolute pitch, speech, and tone language: some experiments and a proposed framework. Music Perception, 21, 339356.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Duñabeitia, J., Hernández, J., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234251.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 39(2), 175191.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grosjean, F. (2010). Bilingual: Life and reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Hernandez, E., Li, P. (2007). Age of acquisition: Its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 638650.

  • Katz, J., Pesetsky, D. (2011). The identity thesis for language and music. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 78777881.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kolinsky, R., Cuvelier, H., Goetry, V., Peretz, I., & Morais, J. (2009). Music training facilitates lexical stress processing. Music Perception, 26, 235246.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Krajbich, I., Bartling, B., Hare, T., & Fehr, E. (2015). Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference. Nature Communications, 6. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8455.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kroll, J., De Groot, A. (1997). Lexical and conceptual memory in the bilingual: Mapping form to meaning in two languages. In A. De Groot & J. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 169199). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lakshmanan, U. (2004). Language History Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Illinois.

  • Li, P., Sepanski, S., & Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: A Web-based interface for bilingual research. Behavior research methods, 38(2), 202210.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 11891207.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Magne, C., Schön, D., & Besson, M. (2006). Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician children: Behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 199211.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marian, V., & Spivey, M. J. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97115.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J. & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22, 14251433.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Paap, K., Greenberg, Z. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musical experience limits the degradative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1410014107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Patel, A. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 674681.

  • Pfordresher, P. Q., & Brown, S. (2009). Enhanced production and perception of musical pitch in tone language speakers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 13851398.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schellenberg, E. G. (2005). Music and cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 14, 317320.

  • Schön, D., Magne, C., & Besson, M. (2004). The music of speech: music training facilitates pitch processing in both music and language. Psychophysiology, 41, 341349.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Soares, C., & Grosjean, F. (1984). Bilinguals in a monolingual and a bilingual speech mode: The effect on lexical access. Memory & cognition, 12(4), 380386.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J. C., & Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 681.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 324.

  • Welch, B. L. (1951). On the comparison of several mean values: an alternative approach. Biometrika, 38(3–4), 330336.

  • Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. The Psychological Record, 58, 475482.

  • Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 255 120 3
Full Text Views 188 17 0
PDF Downloads 18 6 0