In a great variety of cultures oaths, ordeals, or lie detectors are used to adjudicate in trials, even though they do not reliably discern liars from truth tellers. I suggest that these practices owe their cultural success to the triggering of cognitive mechanisms that make them more culturally attractive. Informal oaths would trigger mechanisms related to commitment in communication. Oaths used in judicial contexts, by invoking supernatural punishments, would trigger intuitions of immanent justice, linking misfortunes following an oath with perjury. These intuitions would justify the infliction of costs on oath takers in a way that appears morally justified. Ordeals reflect the same logic. Intuitions about immanent justice link a worse outcome following the ordeal with a guilty verdict. This link justifies the application of the ordeal, and the fixed costs involved (burning, poisoning). Lie detectors also rely on the creation of a link between a specified outcome and a guilty verdict. However, they do not rely on intuitions about immanent justice, but on a variety of intuitions ranging from the plausibly universal to the culturally idiosyncratic. As a result, lie detectors involve lower fixed costs than ordeals, and are less cross-culturally successful than oaths or ordeals.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2004). Evidentiality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Allott, A. (1964). Evidence in african customary law. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 59–80.
Al-Simadi, F. A. (2000). Jordanian students’ beliefs about nonverbal behaviors associated with deception in Jordan. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 28(5), 437–441.
Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Optimally interacting minds. Science, 329(5995), 1081–1085.
Balliet, D. (2010). Communication and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 54(1), 39.
Bartlett, R. (1986). Trial by fire and water: The medieval judicial ordeal. New York: Clarendon Press Oxford.
Basden, G. T. (1966). Niger Ibos: A description of the primitive life, customs and animistic beliefs, etc., of the Ibo people of Nigeria. London: Cass.
Baumard, N., André, J. B., & Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(01), 59–78.
Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2013a). Explaining moral religions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(6), 272–280.
Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2013b). Religious beliefs as reflective elaborations on intuitions: A modified dual-process model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(4), 295–300.
Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2012). What goes around comes around: The evolutionary roots of the belief in immanent justice. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12(1–2), 67–80.
Beattie, J. (1960). Bunyoro: An African kingdom. London: Harcourt College Pub.
Bibas, S. (2002). Harmonizing Substantive-Criminal-Law Values and Criminal Procedure: The Case of Alford and Nolo Contendere Pleas. Cornell L. Rev., 88, 1361.
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the Forest: The Evolution of Egalitarian Behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Boehm, Christopher. (1987). Blood revenge: The enactment and management of conflict in Montenegro and other tribal societies. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Bond, C. F., Omar, A., Mahmoud, A., & Bonser, R. N. (1990). Lie detection across cultures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14(3), 189–204.
Bonner, R. J. (1905). Evidence in Athenian courts. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Boyer, G. (1964). La preuve dans les anciens droits du proche-Orient. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 62–87.
Boyer, P. (2020). Why Divination?. Current Anthropology, 61(1).
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion Explained. London: Heinemann.
Brown, P. (1975). Society and the supernatural: A medieval change. Daedalus, 133–151.
Brunschvig, R. (1964). Le système de la preuve en droit musulman. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 169–186.
Callan, M. J., Ferguson, H. J., & Bindemann, M. (2013). Eye movements to audiovisual scenes reveal expectations of a just world. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 34.
Callan, M. J., Sutton, R. M., & Dovale, C. (2010). When deserving translates into causing: The effect of cognitive load on immanent justice reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 1097–1100.
Callan, M. J., Sutton, R. M., Harvey, A. J., & Dawtry, R. J. (2014). Immanent justice reasoning: Theory, research, and current directions. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 49, pp. 105–161). Elsevier.
Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership. Econometrica, 74(6), 1579–1601.
Claidière, N., Smith, K., Kirby, S., & Fagot, J. (2014). Cultural evolution of systematically structured behaviour in a non-human primate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1797), 20141541.
Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7(1–2), 1–2.
Colman, R. V. (1974). Reason and unreason in early medieval law. The Journal of Interdisciplinary History, 4(4), 571–591.
Conner, A. W. (2000). True confessions? Chinese confessions then and now. In K. G. Turner, J. V. Feinerman, & R. K. Guy (Eds.), The Limits of the Rule of Law in China (pp. 132–162). Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Cory, H., & Hartnoll, M. M. (1945). Customary Law of the Haya Tribe, Tanganyika Territory (Vol. 7). London: Frank Cass & Co.
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl 4), S131–S133.
Czekanowski, J. (1917). Investigations in the area between the Nile and the Congo: First volume: Ethnography, the interlacustrine region of Mporo and Ruanda. Leipzig: Klinkhardt & Biermann.
Davies, W., & Fouracre, P. (1992). The settlement of disputes in early medieval Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Diamond, A. S. (2004). Primitive law, past and present (Kindle edition). London: Routledge.
Eriksson, K., & Coultas, J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14(1–2), 1–26.
Erskine, A., Morley, S., & Pearce, S. (1990). Memory for pain: A review. Pain, 41(3), 255–265.
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1956). Nuer religion. Clarendon Press.
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1957). The Zande royal court. Brussels: Éditions Universitares.
Fisher, G. (1997). The jury’s rise as lie detector. The Yale Law Journal, 107(3), 575–713.
Floyd, S., Norcliffe, E., & San Roque, L. (Eds.). (2018). Egophoricity (Vol. 118). London: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., Frith, C., & Tylén, K. (2012). Coming to terms quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science, 0956797612436816.
Gluckman, M. (1967). The judicial process among the Barotse of Northern Rhodesia (Zambia). Manchester: Manchester University Press.
Goubert, L., Vlaeyen, J. W., Crombez, G., & Craig, K. D. (2011). Learning about pain from others: An observational learning account. The Journal of Pain, 12(2), 167–174.
Griffiths, T. L., Kalish, M. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3503–3514.
Hamidullah, M. (1964). La génèse du droit de la preuve en islam. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 187–200.
Harrison, A. R. W. (1971). The law of Athens (Vol. 2). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Hartwig, M., & Bond, C. H. (2011). Why do lie-catchers fail? A lens model meta-analysis of human lie judgments. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 643.
Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1028.
Holleman, J. F. (1975). Some Problems of Evidence in Shona Tribal Law. In M. Fortes & S. Patterson (Eds.), Studies in African Social Anthropology (pp. 75–94). London: Academic Press.
Hyams, P. R. (1981). Trial by ordeal: The key to proof in the early common law. In M. S. Arnold, T. A. Green, S. A. Scully, & S. D. White (Eds.), On the laws and customs of England: Essays in honor of Samuel E. Thorne (pp. 90–126). Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.
Ishii, R. (1964). The history of evidence in japan. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 521–534.
Jacquemet, N., Luchini, S., Shogren, J. F., & Zylbersztejn, A. (2018). Coordination with communication under oath. Experimental Economics, 21(3), 627–649.
Kerr, M. H., Forsyth, R. D., & Plyley, M. J. (1992). Cold water and hot iron: Trial by ordeal in England. The Journal of Interdisciplinary History, 22(4), 573–595.
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 10681–10686.
Klíma, J. (1964). La preuve dans le droit hittite. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 89–102.
Lebzelter, V. (1934). Native cultures in southwest and south Africa, vol. 2. Leipzig: Karl W. Hiersemann.
Leeson, P. T. (2011). Trial by battle. Journal of Legal Analysis, 3(1), 341–375.
Leeson, P. T. (2012). Ordeals. The Journal of Law and Economics, 55(3), 691–714.
Leeson, P. T., & Coyne, C. J. (2012). Sassywood. Journal of Comparative Economics, 40(4), 608–620.
Lévy, J. P. (1964). Le problème de la preuve dans les droits savants du Moyen-Age. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 137–167.
Logue, A. W., Ophir, I., & Strauss, K. E. (1981). The acquisition of taste aversions in humans. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19(4), 319–333.
Mahr, J., & Csibra, G. (2017). Why do we remember? The communicative function of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–93.
Mead, M. (1969). Social organization of Manua. Honolulu: Bishop Museum Press.
Meek, C. K. (1937). Law and authority in a Nigerian tribe: A study in indirect rule. New York: Barnes & Noble.
Mercier, H. (2020). Not Born Yesterday: The Science of Who we Trust and What we Believe. New York: Princeton University Press.
Mercier, H. (2017). How gullible are we? A review of the evidence from psychology and social science. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 103.
Meyer, H. (1916). The Barundi: An Ethnological Study from German East Africa. Leipzig: Ott Spamer.
Middleton, J. (1953). The Central Tribes of the North-eastern Bantu: The Kikuyu, Including Emlu, Meru, Mbere, Chuka, Mwimbi, Thanaka, and the Kamba of Kenya. London: International African Institute.
Miller, W. I. (1988). Ordeal in Iceland. Scandinavian Studies, 189–218.
Miton, H., Claidière, N., & Mercier, H. (2015). Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303–312.
Morin, O. (2013). How portraits turned their eyes upon us: Visual preferences and demographic change in cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 222–229.
Morin, O. (2016). How Traditions Live and Die. New York: Oxford University Press.
Morley, S. (1993). Vivid memory for ‘everyday’pains. Pain, 55(1), 55–62.
Murdock, G. P. (1980). Theories of illness: A world survey. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Nichols, S. (2002). On the genealogy of norms: A case for the role of emotion in cultural evolution. Philosophy of Science, 69(2), 234–255.
Olson, T. (2000). Of enchantment: The passing of the ordeals and the rise of the jury trial. Syracuse Law Review, 50, 109–196.
Opie, I. A., & Opie, P. (2001). The lore and language of schoolchildren. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Gardner, R. (1992). Covenants with and without a sword: Self-governance is possible. The American Political Science Review, 404–417.
Peters, E. (1996). Torture: Expanded Edition. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Posner, R. A. (1998). An economic approach to the law of evidence. Stan. L. Rev., 51, 1477.
Ravenhill, D. W. (1953). Ordeal by Fire: An Eyewitness Account. Man, 53, 185–186.
Reali, F., & Griffiths, T. L. (2009). The evolution of frequency distributions: Relating regularization to inductive biases through iterated learning. Cognition, 111(3), 317–328.
Retel-Laurentin, A. (1969). Oracles et ordalies chez les Nzakara (Vol. 33). ra. Paris and the Hague: Mouton.
Reynolds, S. (1997). Kingdoms and communities in Western Europe, 900–1300. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Roberts, J. M. (1965). Oaths, autonomic ordeals, and power. American Anthropologist, 67(6), 186–212.
Roberts, S. (1979). Order and Dispute: An Introduction to Legal Anthropology. New York: St Martin’s Press.
Rocher, L. (1964). The theory of proof in ancient Hindu law. Recueil de La Société Jean Bodin, 18, 325–371.
Roscoe, J. (1924). The Bagesu and other tribes of the Uganda protectorate. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Routledge, W. S., & Routledge, K. (1910). With a prehistoric people: The Akikuyu of British East Africa (Vol. 63). London: Edward Arnold.
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23–41.
Russell, M. J. (1959). Hired Champions. Am. J. Legal Hist., 3, 242.
Schapera, I. (1955). A handbook of Tswana law and custom. London: Oxford University Press.
Schelling, T. C. (1960). The strategy of conflict. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Segrave, K. (2004). Lie detectors: A social history. Jefferson: McFarland.
Sell, A., Sznycer, D., Al-Shawaf, L., Lim, J., Krauss, A., Feldman, A., … Tooby, J. (2017). The grammar of anger: Mapping the computational architecture of a recalibrational emotion. Cognition, 168, 110–128.
Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(35), 15073–15078.
Shack, W. A. (1979). Collective Oath Compurgation in Anglo-Saxon England and African states. European Journal of Sociology/Archives Européennes de Sociologie, 20(1), 1–18.
Shweder, R., Much, N., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). Divinity and the “big three” explanations of suffering. Morality and Health, 119, 119–169.
Silving, H. (1959). The oath: I. The Yale Law Journal, 68(7), 1329–1390.
Smith, E. W., & Dale, A. M. (1920). The Ila-speaking peoples of northern Rhodesia (Vol. 2). London: MacMillan and Co.
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell.
Sperber, D. (1997). Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language, 12(1), 67–83.
Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind and Language, 25(4), 359–393.
Tenney, E. R., MacCoun, R. J., Spellman, B. A., & Hastie, R. (2007). Calibration trumps confidence as a basis for witness credibility. Psychological Science, 18(1), 46–50.
Thomas, K. (1971). Religion and the Decline of Magic. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.
Trovillo, P. V. (1938). History of lie detection. Am. Inst. Crim. L. & Criminology, 29, 848.
Ullmann, W. (1946). Medieval principles of evidence. Law Quarterly Review, 62, 77.
Underwood, R. H. (1995). Truth Verifiers: From the Hot Iron to the Lie Detector. Ky. >LJ, 84, 597.
Van Caenegem, R. C. (1965). The law of evidence in the twelfth century. In S. Kuttner & J. J. Ryan (Eds.), Proceedings of the Second International Congress of Medieval Canon Law (Vol. 1). Wetteren: Cultural Press.
Van Caenegem, R. C. (1990). Reflexions on rational and irrational modes of proof in medieval Europe. Tijdschrift Voor Rechtsgeschiedenis, 58, 263.
Vullioud, C., Clément, F., Scott-Phillips, T., & Mercier, H. (2017). Confidence as an expression of commitment: Why misplaced expressions of confidence backfire. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 9–17.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1771 | 198 | 16 |
Full Text Views | 89 | 13 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 134 | 17 | 0 |
In a great variety of cultures oaths, ordeals, or lie detectors are used to adjudicate in trials, even though they do not reliably discern liars from truth tellers. I suggest that these practices owe their cultural success to the triggering of cognitive mechanisms that make them more culturally attractive. Informal oaths would trigger mechanisms related to commitment in communication. Oaths used in judicial contexts, by invoking supernatural punishments, would trigger intuitions of immanent justice, linking misfortunes following an oath with perjury. These intuitions would justify the infliction of costs on oath takers in a way that appears morally justified. Ordeals reflect the same logic. Intuitions about immanent justice link a worse outcome following the ordeal with a guilty verdict. This link justifies the application of the ordeal, and the fixed costs involved (burning, poisoning). Lie detectors also rely on the creation of a link between a specified outcome and a guilty verdict. However, they do not rely on intuitions about immanent justice, but on a variety of intuitions ranging from the plausibly universal to the culturally idiosyncratic. As a result, lie detectors involve lower fixed costs than ordeals, and are less cross-culturally successful than oaths or ordeals.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1771 | 198 | 16 |
Full Text Views | 89 | 13 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 134 | 17 | 0 |