Metacognitive abilities are considered as a hallmark of advanced human cognition. Existing empirical studies have exclusively focused on populations from Western and industrialized societies. Little is known about young children’s metacognitive abilities in other societal and cultural contexts. Here we tested 4-year-old Yucatec Mayan (a rural native population from Mexico) by adopting a metacognitive task in which children’s explicit assessment of their own knowledge states about the hidden content of a container and their informing judgments (whether or not to inform an ignorant person about the hidden contents of a container) were assessed. Similar to previous studies, we found that Yucatec Mayan children overestimated their knowledge states in the explicit metacognitive task. However, in contrast with studies on Western children, we did not find the facilitating effect of the implicit informing task over the explicit task. These findings suggest that the early development of metacognition combines universal and culture-sensitive features.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Balcomb, F. K., & Gerken, L. (2008). Three-year-old children can access their own memory to guide responses on a visual matching task. Developmental Science, 11, 750–760. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00725.x.
Carpendale, J. I., & Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the distinction between false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Development, 67(4), 1686–1706. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131725.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (4), 148–153. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009. 01.005.
Flavell, J. H. (2000). Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 15–23.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.
Gaskins, S. (2000). Children’s Daily Activities in a Mayan Village: A Culturally Grounded Description. Cross-Cultural Research, 34(4), 375–389.
Gaskins, S., & Lucy, J. (1987). Passing the buck: Responsability and blame in the Yucatec Mayan household. Annual Meetings of the American Anthropological Association.
Gaskins, S., & Paradise, R. (2010). Learning through observation in daily life. In D. F. Lancy, J. C. Bock, & S. Gaskins (Eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood (pp. 85–117). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Goupil, L., Romand-Monnier, M., & Kouider, S. (2016). Infants ask for help when they know they don’t know. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 3492–3496. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515129113.
Groark, K. P. (2013). Toward a cultural phenomenology of intersubjectivity: The extended relational field of the Tzotzil Maya of highland Chiapas, Mexico. Language and Communication, 33, 278–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2011.10.003.
Hanks., W. F. (1993). The five gourds of memory. In A. Monod-Becquelin & M. Fioravanti (Eds.), Mémoire de La Tradition (pp. 330–319). Paris: Société d’Ethnologie.
Harris, P., Bartz, D. T., & Rowe, M. L. (2017). Young children communicate their ignorance and ask questions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7884–7891.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.
Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344, DOI: 10.1126/science.1243091.
Heyes, C., & Bang, D., Shea, N., Frith, C. D. & Fleming, S. M. (2020). Knowing ourselves together: The cultural origins of metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24, 349–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.007.
INEGI (2020). Retrieved on May, 28, 2020 from https://inegi.org.mx/app/areasgeograficas/?ag=23#tabMCcollapse-Indicadores.
Jordan, B. (1989). Cosmopolitical Obstetrics: Some Insights from the Training of Traditional Midwives. (Featured article with comments by C. H. Browner, Robert A. Hahn, R. and P. M. Jeffery, Jean Lave, Carol P. MacCormack, and Lorna Amarasingham Rhodes.). Social Science and Medicine, 28(9), 925–944.
Kramer, K. L. (2005). Maya children: Helpers at the farm. Harvard University Press.
Kim, S., Paulus, M., Sodian, B., & Proust, J. (2016). Children’s sensitivity to their own ignorance in informing. PloS one. 11(3): e0152595. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152595.
Kloo, D., Rohwer, M., & Perner, J. (2017). Direct and indirect admission of ignorance by children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 279–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.014.
Koriat, A. & Levy-Sadot, R. (1999). Processes underlying metacognitive judgments: Information-based and experience-based monitoring of one’s own knowledge. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds), Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology (pp. 483–502). New York: Guilford Press.
Kuhn D. (2000). Metacognitive Development, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9 178–181.
Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., O’Loughlin, M., Schauble, L., Leadbeater, B., & Yotive, W. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated Learning, Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge University Press.
Lecce, S., & Hughes, C. (2010). The Italian job?: Comparing theory of mind performance in British and Italian children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 747–766.
Le Guen, O. (2018). Managing epistemicity among the Yucatec Mayas (Mexico). In J. Proust & M. Frotier (Eds.), Metacognitive diversity: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 193–222). Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press.
Le Guen, O. (2012). The place of supernatural entities in Yucatec Maya daily life and socialization. In P. Nandedeo & A. Breton (Eds.), Maya daily life. Proceedings of the 13th European Maya Conference (Paris, december 2008) (Verlag Anton Saurwein).
Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., Hartwig, M. K., Rawson, K. A., Swan, K.,& Cook, D. (2009). Using standards to improve middle school students’ accuracy at evaluating the quality of their recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(4), 307–318. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017599.
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: a meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44, 523–531. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523.
Loussouarn, A., Gabriel, D. & Proust, J. (2011). Exploring the informational sources of metaperception: The case of Change Blindness Blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1489–1501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.07.001.
Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: Elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 643–655.
Mitchell, P., & Lacohée, H. (1991). Children’s early understanding of false belief. Cognition, 39 (2), 107–127.
Neldner, K., Collier-Baker, E., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) know when they are ignorant about the location of food. Animal Cognition, 18, 683–699. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0836-6.
Paulus, M., Proust, J., & Sodian, B. (2013). Examining implicit metacognition in 3.5-year-old children: An eye tracking and pupilometric study. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00145.
Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61, 973–982. doi: 10.2307/1130869.
Proust, J. (2013). The Philosophy of Metacognition. Mental Agency and Self-Awareness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2007). Improving students’ self-evaluation of learning for key concepts in textbook materials. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 559–579. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440701326022.
Robbins, J., & Rumsey, A. (2008). Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropological Quarterly 81 (2), 407–420. 10.1353/anq.0.0005.
Rohwer, M., Kloo, D., & Perner, J. (2012). Escape from metaignorance: How children develop an understanding of their own lack of knowledge. Child Development, 83, 1869–1883. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01830.x.
Sauter, D., A., LeGuen, O., & Haun, D. B. M. (2011). Categorical perception of emotional facial expressions does not require lexical categories. Emotion, 11 (6), 1479–1483. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025336.
Schneider, W. (2008). The development of metacognitive knowledge in children and adolescents: Major trends and implications for education. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2, 114–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2008.00041.x.
Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology. 47, 1239–1247.
Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., & Frith, C. D. (2014). Supra-personal cognitive control and metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.006.
Shneidman, L. A., Buresh, J. S., Shimpi, P. M., Knight-Schwarz, J., & Woodward, A. L. (2009). Social experience, social attention and word learning in an overhearing paradigm. Language Learning and Development, 5 (4), 266–281. doi:10.1080/ 15475440903001115.
Shneidman, L., Gaskins, S., & Woodward, A. (2016). Child-directed teaching and social learning at 18 months of age: Evidence from Yucatec Mayan and US infants. Developmental Science, 19, 372–381. doi: 10.1111/desc.12318.
Smith, J. D. (2009). The study of animal metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 389–396.
Smith, J. D., Beran, M. J., Couchman, J. J., & Coutinho, M. V. (2008). The comparative study of metacognition: sharper paradigms, safer inferences. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 679–691.
Van Loon, M. H., & Roebers, C. M. (2017). Effects of feedback on self‐evaluations and self‐regulation in elementary school. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31(5), 508–519. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3347.
Wimmer, H., Hogrefe, G. J., & Perner, J. (1988). Children’s understanding of informational access as a source of knowledge. Child Development, 59, 386–396. doi: 10.2307/1130318.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 620 | 163 | 18 |
Full Text Views | 38 | 3 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 79 | 8 | 0 |
Metacognitive abilities are considered as a hallmark of advanced human cognition. Existing empirical studies have exclusively focused on populations from Western and industrialized societies. Little is known about young children’s metacognitive abilities in other societal and cultural contexts. Here we tested 4-year-old Yucatec Mayan (a rural native population from Mexico) by adopting a metacognitive task in which children’s explicit assessment of their own knowledge states about the hidden content of a container and their informing judgments (whether or not to inform an ignorant person about the hidden contents of a container) were assessed. Similar to previous studies, we found that Yucatec Mayan children overestimated their knowledge states in the explicit metacognitive task. However, in contrast with studies on Western children, we did not find the facilitating effect of the implicit informing task over the explicit task. These findings suggest that the early development of metacognition combines universal and culture-sensitive features.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 620 | 163 | 18 |
Full Text Views | 38 | 3 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 79 | 8 | 0 |