Recent neuropaleontological research suggests that the parietal lobe has increased in size as much as the frontal lobes in Homo Sapiens over the past 150,000 years, but has not provided a neuropsychological explanation for the evolution of human socialization or the development of religion. Drawing from several areas of research, (i.e., neurodevelopment, neuropsychology, paleoneurology, cognitive science, archeology, and anthropology), we argue that parietal evolution in Homo sapiens integrated sensations and mental processes into a more integrated subjective “sense of self”. This enhanced self advanced prosocial traits (e.g., increased empathy, greater social bonding, enhanced theory of mind capacities), promoting more effective socialization skills (e.g., parenting, group cooperation). Conversely, when this enhanced sense of self became inhibited, powerful experiences of self-transcendence occurred. We believe these potent selfless experiences became increasingly sought after though ritual means (e.g., music, dance, vision quests, spirit travel), providing the foundations for the development of shamanism and religion.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Alexander, M. P., Stuss, D., & Benson, D. F. (1979). Capgras syndrome: A reduplicative phenomenon. Neurology, 29, 334–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.29.3.334.
Alloway, K. D., & Flaherty, K. (2021). Evolution of the encephalized human brain: How did we become exceptional? In M. Kathleen Pitirri, & Joan T. Richtsmeier (Eds.), Evolutionary Cell Processes in Primates-Volume I: Bones, brains, and muscle, (pp. 133–158). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781003206231.
Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33 (4), 245–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000853.
Balzeau, A., Turq, A., Talamo, S., Daujeard, C., Guérin, G., Welker, F., … & Gómez-Olivencia, A. (2020). Pluridisciplinary evidence for burial for the La Ferrassie 8 Neandertal child. Scientific Reports, 10 (1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-77611-z.
Barceló-Coblijn, L., & Gomila, A. (2012). Evidence of recursion in tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35 (4), 219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001865.
Barresi, J., & Martin, R. (2011). History as prologue: Western theories of the self. In S. Gallagher (Ed.), Oxford handbook of the self, (pp. 33–56). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199548019.003.0002.
Barton, R. A., & Venditti, C. (2013). Human frontal lobes are not relatively large. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 (22), 9001–9006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1215723110.
Blanke, O., & Metzinger, T. (2009). Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (1), 7–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003.
Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self-processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. Journal of Neuroscience, 25 (3), 550–557. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005.
Bruner, E. (2021). Evolving human brains: paleoneurology and the fate of Middle Pleistocene. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 28 (1), 76–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-10020-09500-09508.
Bruner, E. (2018a). Human paleoneurology and the evolution of the parietal cortex. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 91, 136–147. https://doi.org/10.1159/000488889.
Bruner, E. (2018b). The brain, the braincase, and the morphospace. In E. Bruner, N., Ogihara, & H.C., Tanabe (Eds.), Digital endocasts: From skulls to brains (Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series), (pp. 93–114). Tokyo: Springer Japan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-56582-6_7.
Bruner, E., Amano, H., Pereira-Pedro, A.S., & Ogihara, N. (2018). The evolution of the parietal lobes in the genus Homo. In E. Bruner, N, Ogihara, & H.C., Tanabe (Eds.), Digital endocasts: From skulls to brains (Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series), (pp. 219–237). Tokyo: Springer Japan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-56582-6_15.
Bruner, E., De La Cuétara, J. M., & Holloway, R. (2011). A bivariate approach to the variation of the parietal curvature in the genus Homo. The Anatomical Record: Advances in Integrative Anatomy and Evolutionary Biology, 294 (9), 1548–1556. https://doi.org/10.1002/ar.21450.
Bruner, E., & Iriki, A. (2016). Extending mind, visuospatial integration, and the evolution of the parietal lobes in the human genus. Quaternary International, 405, 98–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.05.019.
Bruner, E., Preuss, T. M., Chen, X., & Rilling, J. K. (2017). Evidence for expansion of the precuneus in human evolution. Brain Structure and Function, 222 (2), 1053–1060. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1172-y.
Buckner, R. L., & Krienen, F. M. (2013). The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17 (12), 648–665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.017.
Cantlon, J. F., Brannon, E. M., Carter, E. J., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2006). Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biology, 4 (5), e125. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040125.
Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain, 129 (3), 564–583. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl004.
Changeux, J. P., & Lou, H. C. (2011). Emergent pharmacology of conscious experience: new perspectives in substance addiction. The faseb Journal, 25 (7), 2098–2108. https://doi.org/10.1096/fj.11-0702ufm.
Chiron, C., Jambaque, I., Nabbout, R., Lounes, R., Syrota, A., & Dulac, O. (1997). The right brain hemisphere is dominant in human infants. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 120 (6), 1057–1065. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.6.1057.
Cohen, D. (2019). Sex and the evolution of spirituality. In J.R. Feierman, & L. viedo, (Eds.). The evolution of religion, religiosity and theology: A multi-level and multi-disciplinary approach, (pp. 54–69). New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429285608.
Cohen, D., Bhushan, B. Hanks, R., Yoon, D. P., Johnstone, B., & Hunt, I. (2020). The right parietal lobe, sense of self, and empathy: Cross-cultural, ethnic, and religious considerations. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 23 (5), 375–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2020.1725454.
Coolidge, F. L. (2014). The exaptation of the parietal lobes in Homo sapiens. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 92, 295–298. https://doi10.4436/JASS.92013.
Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. G. (2018). The rise of Homo sapiens: The evolution of modern thinking (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Cozolino, L. (2006). The neuroscience of human relationship. New York: Norton.
Damasio, A. (2003). Mental self: The person within. Nature, 423 (6937), 227–227. https://doi.org/10.1038/423227a.
Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. New York: Vintage Books.
Daprati, E., Sirigu, A., & Nico, D. (2010). Body and movement: consciousness in the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 48 (3), 756–762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.008.
Desmurget, M., Reilly, K. T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese, C., & Sirigu, A. (2009). Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science, 324 (5928), 811–813. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1169896.
Dein, S. (2020). Transcendence, religion and social bonding. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 42 (1), 77–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0084672420905018.
Devinsky, O. (2000). Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy & Behavior, 1 (1), 60–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ebeh.2000.0025.
De Waal, F. B. (2012). The antiquity of empathy. Science, 336 (6083), 874–876. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1220999.
Dunbar, R. I. (2013). The origin of religion as a small-scale phenomenon. In S. Clarke, R. Powell, & J. Savulescu (Eds.), Religion, intolerance, and conflict: A scientific and conceptual investigation, (pp. 48–66). Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Dunbar, R. I. (2016). The social brain hypothesis and human evolution. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.44.
Dunbar, R. I. (2020). Religion, the social brain and the mystical stance. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 42 (1), 46–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0084672419900547.
Ebeling, U., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). Anatomy of the parietal lobe: mapping the individual pattern. Acta neurochirurgica, 136(1), 8–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01411428.
Farrant, B. M., Devine, T. A., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Empathy, perspective taking and prosocial behaviour: The importance of parenting practices. Infant and Child Development, 21 (2), 175–188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.740.
Finlay, B.L., & Workman, A.D. (2013). Human exceptionalism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17 (5), 199–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.001.
Fischer, R., Callander, R., Reddish, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). How do rituals affect cooperation?. Human Nature, 24 (2), 115–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9167-y.
Freton, M., Lemogne, C., Bergouignan, L., Delaveau, P., Lehéricy, S., & Fossati, P. (2014). The eye of the self: precuneus volume and visual perspective during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain Structure and Function, 219 (3), 959–968. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-013-0546-2.
Gabi, M., Neves, K., Masseron, C., Ribeiro, P.F., Ventura-Antunes, L., Torres, L., … & Herculano-Houzel, S. (2016). No relative expansion of the number of prefrontal neurons in primate and human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (34), 9617–9622. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1610178113.
Glasser, M. F., Goyal, M. S., Preuss, T. M., Raichle, M. E., & Van Essen, D. C. (2014). Trends and properties of human cerebral cortex: correlations with cortical myelin content. Neuroimage, 93, 165–175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.060.
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Development, 601–622. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130201.
Grattan, L. M., & Eslinger, P. (1989). Higher cognition and social behavior: Changes in cognitive flexibility and empathy after cerebral lesions. Neuropsychology, 3(3), 175–185. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0091764.
Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., … & Pääbo, S. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328(5979), 710–722. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1188021.
Gunz, P., Neubauer, S., Maureille, B., & Hublin, J. J. (2010). Brain development after birth differs between Neanderthals and modern humans. Current Biology, 20(21), R921-R922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.018.
Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 685–694. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094500.
Guy-Evans, Olivia. (April 13, 2021). Parietal Lobe: Definition, Functions, and Location (with illustration by Elizabeth Miclau). Retrieved Feb. 14, 2023 from: https://www.simplypsychology.org/parietal-lobe.html.
Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C. J., Wedeen, V. J., & Sporns, O. (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biology, 6 (7), e159. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159.
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Vintage.
Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S.T. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Journal of Neuroscience, 26 (4), 1133–1137. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006.
Happe, F. (2003). Theory of mind and the self. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1001, 134–144. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1279.008.
Hobson, N. M., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Xygalatas, D., & Inzlicht, M. (2018). The psychology of rituals: An integrative review and process-based framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22 (3), 260–284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868317734944.
Hollan, D. (2012). Emerging issues in the cross-cultural study of empathy. Emotion Review, 4 (1), 70–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073911421376.
Iriki, A., & Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: a scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367 (1585), 10–23. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0190.
Johnstone, B., Bodling, A., Cohen, D., Christ, S. E., & Wegrzyn, A. (2012). Right parietal lobe “selflessness” as the neuropsychological basis of spiritual transcendence. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 22, 267–284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2012.657524.
Johnstone, B., & Cohen, D. (2019). Neuroscience, selflessness, and spiritual experience: Explaining the science of transcendence. New York: Elsevier.
Johnstone, B., Cohen, D., Bryant, K. R., Glass, B., & Christ, S. E. (2015). Functional and structural indices of empathy: Evidence for self-orientation as a neuropsychological foundation of empathy. Neuropsychology 29 (3), 463–472. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/neu0000155.
Johnstone, B., Cohen, D., & Dennison, A. (2021). The integration of sensations and mental experiences into a unified experience: A neuropsychological model for the “sense of self”. Neuropsychologia, 159, 107939. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107939.
Johnstone, B., Cohen, D., Konopacki, K., & Ghan, C. (2016a). Selflessness as a foundation of spiritual transcendence: Perspectives from the neurosciences and religious studies. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 26 (4), 287–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2015.1118328.
Johnstone, B., & Glass, B. A. (2008). Support for a neuropsychological model of spirituality in persons with traumatic brain injury. Zygon, 43, 861–874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9744.2008.00964.x.
Johnstone, B., Hanks, R., Bhushan, B., Cohen, D., Roseberry, J., & Yoon, D. P. (2016b). Selflessness as a universal neuropsychological foundation of spiritual transcendence: Validation with Christian, Hindu, and Muslim traditions. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 20, 175–187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2017.1333090.
Klein, R. G. (2009). The human career: Human biological and cultural origins (3rd ed.) Chicago & London: University of Chicago Press.
Klein, R. G. (2000). Archeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(1), 17–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:1%3C17::AID-EVAN3%3E3.0.CO;2-A.
Lage, C. A., Wolmarans, D. W., & Mograbi, D. C. (2022). An evolutionary view of self-awareness. Behavioural Processes, 194, 104543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104543.
Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8 (12), 528–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.001.
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self-development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131080.
Li, L., Hu, X., Preuss, T. M., Glasser, M. F., Damen, F. W., Qiu, Y., & Rilling, J. (2013). Mapping putative hubs in human, chimpanzee and rhesus macaque connectomes via diffusion tractography. Neuroimage, 80, 462–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.024.
Lombard, M., & Högberg, A. (2021). Four-field co-evolutionary model for human cognition: variation in the Middle Stone Age/Middle Palaeolithic. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 28 (1), 142–177. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-020-09502-6.
Lou, H. C., Changeux, J. P., & Rosenstand, A. (2017). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of self-awareness. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 765–773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.004.
Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjaer, T. W., … & Lisanby, S. H. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101 (17), 6827–6832. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0400049101.
Margulies, D. S., Vincent, J. L., Kelly, C., Lohmann, G., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., … & Petrides, M. (2009). Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 (47), 20069–20074. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905314106.
Martinón-Torres, M., d’errico, F., Santos, E., Álvaro Gallo, A., Amano, N., Archer, W., … & Petraglia, M. D. (2021). Earliest known human burial in Africa. Nature, 593 (7857), 95–100. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03457-8.
Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Feinberg, T. E., Lee, G. P., & Nichols, M. E. (2000). Anosognosia and asomatognosia during intracarotid amobarbital inactivation. Neurology, 55 (6), 816–820. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.55.6.816.
Naito, E., Morita, T., Saito, D. N., Ban, M., Shimada, K., Okamoto, Y., … & Asada, M. (2017). Development of right-hemispheric dominance of inferior parietal lobule in proprioceptive illusion task. Cerebral Cortex, 27 (11), 5385–5397. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx223.
Nakahashi, W. (2017). The effect of trauma on Neanderthal culture: a mathematical analysis. Homo, 68 (2), 83–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchb.2017.02.001.
Neubauer, S., Gunz, P., & Hublin, J. J. (2009). The pattern of endocranial ontogenetic shape changes in humans. Journal of Anatomy, 215 (3), 240–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01106.x.
Northoff, G., & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8 (3), 102–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004.
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greek, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in the brain: A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage, 31 (1), 440–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002.
Nowell, A. (2010). Defining behavioral modernity in the context of Neandertal and anatomically modern human populations. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39 (1), 437–452. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105113.
Orban, G. A., & Caruana, F. (2014). The neural basis of human tool use. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 310. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00310.
Oxenham, M., & Willis, A. (2017). Towards a bioarchaeology of care of children. In L. Tilley, & A. Schrenk (Eds.), New Developments in the Bioarchaeology of Care: Further Case Studies and Extended Theory, (pp. 219–235). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-39901-0_11.
Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108 (5), 711. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/pspi0000014.
Pearce, E. (2018). Neanderthals and Homo sapiens: cognitively different kind of humans? In L. D. Di Paolo, F. Di Vincenzo, & F. De Petrillo (Eds.), Evolution of primate social cognition, (pp. 181–196). Cham: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-973-319-93776-2_12.
Pearce, E., Stringer, C., & Dunbar, R. I. (2013). New insights into differences in brain organization between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280 (1758), 20130168. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.0168.
Philippi, C. L., Feinstein, J. S., Khalsa, S. S., Damasio, A., Tranel, D., Landini, G., Williford, K., . & Rudrauf, D. (2012). Preserved self-awareness following extensive bilateral brain damage to the insula, anterior cingulate, and medial prefrontal cortices. PLoS one 7 (8): e38413. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038413.
Prüfer, K., Racimo, F., Patterson, N., Jay, F., Sankararaman, S., Sawyer, S., Heinze, A.…. & Pääbo, S. (2014). The complete genome sequence of a Neanderthal from the Altai Mountains. Nature, 505 (7481), 43–49. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12886.
Rochat, P. (2010). The innate sense of the body develops to become a public affair by 2–3 years. Neuropsychologia, 48 (3), 738–745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.021.
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 (9), 677–688. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.677.
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362 (1481), 773–786. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2087.
Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2014). Implicit false-belief processing in the human brain. NeuroImage, 101, 268–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.014.
Seitz, R. J., & Angel, H. F. (2020). Belief formation–A driving force for brain evolution. Brain and Cognition, 140, 105548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105548.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Goldsher, D., Berger, B. D., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2004). Impairment in cognitive and affective empathy in patients with brain lesions: Anatomical and cognitive correlates. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 1113–1127. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490515531.
Sherwood, C. C., Bauernfeind, A. L., Bianchi, S., Raghanti, M. A., & Hof, P. R. (2012). Human brain evolution writ large and small. Progress in Brain Research, 195, 237–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00011-8.
Singh, M. (2018). The cultural evolution of shamanism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001893.
Skowronski, J.J., & Sedikides, C. (2019). On the evolution of the human self: A data-driven review and reconsideration. Self and Identity, 18 (1), 4–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2017.1350601.
Sommer, J. D. (1999). The Shanidar iv ‘flower burial’: a re-evaluation of Neanderthal burial ritual. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 9 (1), 127–129. doi:10.1017/S0959774300015249.
Spikins, P., Rutherford, H., & Needham, A. (2010). From homininity to humanity: Compassion from the earliest archaics to modern humans. Time & Mind: The Journal of Archaeology, Consciousness, and Culture, 303–325. https://doi.org/10.2752/175169610X12754030955977.
Stiner, M. C. (2017). Love and death in the Stone Age: what constitutes first evidence of mortuary treatment of the human body?. Biological Theory, 12 (4), 248–261. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-017-0275-5.
Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. (2014). Music and social bonding: “self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1096. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096.
Thorpe, N. (2017). The palaeolithic compassion debate–alternative projections of modern-day disability into the distant past. In Care in the Past: Archaeological and Interdisciplinary Perspective, L. Powell, W. Southwell-Wright, & R. Gowland (Eds.), (pp. 93–110). Oxford: Oxbow Books. http://digital.casalini.it/9781785703386.
Torrey, E. F. (2017). Evolving brains, emerging gods: early humans and the origins of religion. New York: Columbia University Press. https://doi.org/10.1080/0048721X.2018.1427385.
Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48 (3), 703–712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034.
Tsakiris, M., Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The role of the right temporo-parietal junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one’s body. Neuropsychologia, 46 (12), 3014–3018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.004.
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53 (1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114.
Turner, J. H., Maryanski, A., Petersen, A. K., & Geertz, A. W. (2017). The emergence and evolution of religion: By means of natural selection. New York & London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315111995.
Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35 (4), 203–218. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001452.
VanDerveer’s, B., & Bar-Yosef, O. (2019). The paleolithic burials at Qafzeh cave, Israel. paleo. Revue d’archéologie préhistorique, (30–31), 256–275. https://doi.org/10.4000/paleo.4848.
Villa, P., Soriano, S., Tsanova, T., Degano, I., Higham, T. F., d’Errico, F., … & Beaumont, P.B. (2012). Border cave and the beginning of the later Stone Age in South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 (33), 13208–13213. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1202629109.
Wadley, L. (2005). Putting ochre to the test: replication studies of adhesives that may have been used for hafting tools in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution, 49 (5), 587–601. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.06.007.
Winkelman, M. J. (1990). Shamans and other “magico-religious” healers: a cross-cultural study of their origins, nature, and social transformations. Ethos, 18 (3), 308–352. https://www.jstor.org/stable/640339.
Winkelman, M. (2002). Shamanism and cognitive evolution (with comments). Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 12 (1), 71–101. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774302000045.
Winkelman, M. (2004). Shamanism as the original neurotheology. Zygon®, 39 (1), 193–217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9744.2004.00566.x.
Zhang, S., & Chiang-shan, R.L. (2012). Functional connectivity mapping of the human precuneus by resting state fMRI. Neuroimage, 59 (4), 3548–3562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.023.
Zilhão, J. (2016). Lower and Middle Palaeolithic mortuary behaviours and the origins of ritual burial. In C. Renfrew, M.J. Boyd, & I. Morley (Eds.), Death rituals, social order and the archaeology of immortality in the ancient world, (pp. 27–44). Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316014509.004.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 263 | 263 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 9 | 9 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 25 | 25 | 2 |
Recent neuropaleontological research suggests that the parietal lobe has increased in size as much as the frontal lobes in Homo Sapiens over the past 150,000 years, but has not provided a neuropsychological explanation for the evolution of human socialization or the development of religion. Drawing from several areas of research, (i.e., neurodevelopment, neuropsychology, paleoneurology, cognitive science, archeology, and anthropology), we argue that parietal evolution in Homo sapiens integrated sensations and mental processes into a more integrated subjective “sense of self”. This enhanced self advanced prosocial traits (e.g., increased empathy, greater social bonding, enhanced theory of mind capacities), promoting more effective socialization skills (e.g., parenting, group cooperation). Conversely, when this enhanced sense of self became inhibited, powerful experiences of self-transcendence occurred. We believe these potent selfless experiences became increasingly sought after though ritual means (e.g., music, dance, vision quests, spirit travel), providing the foundations for the development of shamanism and religion.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 263 | 263 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 9 | 9 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 25 | 25 | 2 |