Understanding the Correspondences: Introduction to the Special Issue on Crossmodal Correspondences

In: Multisensory Research
View More View Less
  • 1 Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Biology, and Cognitive Interaction Technology-Center of Excellence (CITEC), University of Bielefeld, Germany
  • 2 Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Oxford University, UK
  • 3 Centre for the Study of the Senses, School of Advanced Study, University of London, UK

If the inline PDF is not rendering correctly, you can download the PDF file here.

  • Bien N., ten Oever S., Goebel R., Sack A. T. (2012). The sound of size: crossmodal binding in pitch-size synesthesia: a combined TMS, EEG, and psychophysics study, NeuroImage 59, 663672.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bremner A., Caparos S., Davidoff J., de Fockert J., Linnell K., Spence C. (2013). Bouba and Kiki in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste matches to Westerners, Cognition 126, 165172.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carnevale M. J., Harris L. R. (2016). Which direction is up for a high pitch? Multisens. Res. 29, 113132.

  • Cytowic R. E. (1993). The Man Who Tasted Shapes. G. P. Putnam’s Sons, New York, NY, USA.

  • Deroy O., Spence C. (2013a). Why we are not all synesthetes (not even weakly so), Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 643664.

  • Deroy O., Spence C. (2013b). Are we all born synaesthetic? Examining the neonatal synaesthesia hypothesis, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 12401253.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deroy O. & Spence C. (in press). Lessons of synaesthesia for consciousness: learning from the exception, rather than the general, Neuropsychologia. DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deroy O., Spence C. (2016). Crossmodal correspondences: four challenges, Multisens. Res. 29, 2848.

  • Deroy O., Crisinel A.-S., Spence C. (2013). Crossmodal correspondences between odors and contingent features: odors, musical notes, and geometrical shapes, Psychon. Bull Rev. 20, 878896.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deroy O., Fasiello I., Hayward V. & Auvray M. (in press). Differentiated audio-tactile correspondences in sighted and blind individuals, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dingemanse M. (2012). Advances in the cross-linguistic study of ideophones, Lang. Linguist. Compass 6, 654672.

  • Etzi R., Spence C., Zampini M., Gallace A. (2016). When sandpaper is ‘kiki’ and satin is ‘bouba’: an exploration of the associations between words, emotional states, and the tactile attributes of everyday materials, Multisens. Res. 29, 133155.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Evans K. K., Treisman A. (2010). Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features, J. Vis. 10, 6. DOI:10.1167/10.1.6.

  • Jraissati Y., Slobodenyuk N., Kanso A., Ghanem L., Elhajj I. (2016). Haptic and tactile adjectives are consistently mapped onto color space, Multisens. Res. 29, 253278.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Knöferle K. M., Spence C. (2012). Crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes, Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 9921006.

  • Knöferle K. M., Woods A., Käppler F., Spence C. (2015). That sounds sweet: using crossmodal correspondences to communicate gustatory attributes, Psychol. Marketing 32, 107120.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Köhler W. (1929). Gestalt Psychology. Liveright Publication, New York, NY, USA.

  • Köhler W. (1947). Gestalt Psychology: an Introduction to New Concepts in Modern Psychology. Liveright Publication, New York, NY, USA.

  • Lewkowicz D. J., Turkewitz G. (1980). Cross-modal equivalence in early infancy: auditory–visual intensity matching, Dev. Psychol. 16, 597607.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ludwig V. U., Adachi I., Matzuzawa T. (2011). Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans, Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 2066120665.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marks L. (1978). The Unity of the Senses: Interrelations Among the Modalities. Academic Press, New York, NY, USA.

  • Marks L. E. (1996). On perceptual metaphors, Metaphor. Symb. Act. 11, 3966.

  • Marks L. E. (2004). Cross-modal interactions in speeded classification, in: Handbook of Multisensory Processes, Calvert G. A., Spence C., Stein B. E. (Eds), pp.  85105. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, USA.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marks L. E., Hammeal R. J., Bornstein M. H. (1987). Perceiving similarity and comprehending metaphor, Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 52(215), 1102.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nava E., Grassi M., Turati C. (2016). Audio-visual, visuo-tactile and audio-tactile correspondences in preschoolers, Multisens. Res. 29, 93111.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Osgood C. E., Suci G. J., Tannenbaum P. H. (1957). The Measurement of Meaning. University of Illinois Press, Urbana, IL, USA.

  • Palmer S. E., Schloss K. B., Xu Z., Prado-León L. R. (2013). Music-color associations are mediated by emotion, Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 88368841.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Palmer S. E., Langlois T. S., Schloss K. B. (2016). Music-to-color associations of single-line piano melodies in non-synesthetes, Multisens. Res. 29, 157193.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parise C. V. (2016). Crossmodal correspondences: standing issues and experimental guidelines, Multisens. Res. 29, 728.

  • Parise C. V., Spence C. (2013). Audiovisual cross-modal correspondences in the general population, in: Oxford Handbook of Synaesthesia, Simner J., Hubbard E. M. (Eds), pp.  790815. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parise C. V., Knorre K., Ernst M. O. (2014). Natural auditory scene statistics shapes human spatial hearing, Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 61046108.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pratt C. C. (1930). The spatial character of high and low tones, J. Exp. Psychol. 13, 278285.

  • Ratcliffe V. F., Taylor A. M., Reby D. (2016). Cross-modal correspondences in non-human mammal communication, Multisens. Res. 29, 4991.

  • Sadaghiani S., Maier J. X., Noppeney U. (2009). Natural, metaphoric, and linguistic auditory direction signals have distinct influences on visual motion processing, J. Neurosci. 29, 64906499.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sapir E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism, J. Exp. Psychol. 12, 225239.

  • Simner J., Hubbard E. M. (Eds) (2013). The Oxford Handbook of Synesthesia. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

  • Snider J. G., Osgood C. E. (1969). Semantic Differential Technique: a Sourcebook. Aldine Publishing, Chicago, IL, USA.

  • Spence C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review, Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 125.

  • Spence C. (2012). Managing sensory expectations concerning products and brands: capitalizing on the potential of sound and shape symbolism, J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 3754.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spence C. (2014). Assessing the influence of shape and sound symbolism on the consumer’s response to chocolate, New Food 17, 5962.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spence C., Parise C. V. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of crossmodal correspondences, i-Perception 3, 410412.

  • Stekelenburg J. J. & Keetels M. (in press). The effect of synesthetic associations between the visual and auditory modalities on the Colavita effect, Exp. Brain Res. DOI:10.1007/s00221-015-4363-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stumpf K. (1883). Tonpsychologie [Psychology of Tones]. S. Hirzel, Leipzig, Germany.

  • Van Leeuwen T. M., Dingemanse M., Todil N., Agameya A., Majid A. (2016). Non-random associations of graphemes with colors in Arabic, Multisens. Res. 29, 223252.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Velasco C., Woods A. T., Liu J., Spence C. (2016). Assessing the role of taste intensity and hedonics in taste-shape correspondences, Multisens. Res. 29, 209221.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagner K., Dobkins K. R. (2011). Synaesthetic associations decrease during infancy, Psychol. Sci. 22, 10671072.

  • Wagner S., Winner E., Cicchetti D., Gardner H. (1981). “Metaphorical” mapping in human infants, Child Dev. 52, 728731.

  • Walker P., Bremner J. G., Mason U., Spring J., Mattock K., Slater A., Johnson S. P. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synesthetic cross-modality correspondences, Psychol. Sci. 21, 2125.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Walker P., Bremner J. G., Mason U., Spring J., Mattock K., Slater A., Johnson S. P. (2014). Preverbal infants are sensitive to cross-sensory correspondences: much ado about the null results of Lewkowicz and Minar (2014), Psychol. Sci. 25, 835836.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang Q. (J.), Spence C. (2016). ‘Striking a sour note’: assessing the influence of consonant and dissonant music on taste perception, Multisens. Res. 29, 195208.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Woods A. T., Spence C., Butcher N., Deroy O. (2013). Fast lemons and sour boulders: testing the semantic hypothesis of crossmodal correspondences using an internet-based testing methodology, i-Perception 4, 365369.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 260 150 10
Full Text Views 190 16 2
PDF Downloads 17 5 1