Inter-Individual Differences in Vicarious Tactile Perception: a View Across the Lifespan in Typical and Atypical Populations

in Multisensory Research
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?

Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.

Help

 

Have Institutional Access?

Login with your institution. Any other coaching guidance?

Connect

Touch is our most interpersonal sense, and so it stands to reason that we represent not only our own bodily experiences, but also those felt by others. This review will summarise brain and behavioural research on vicarious tactile perception (mirror touch). Specifically, we will focus on vicarious touch across the lifespan in typical and atypical groups, and will identify the knowledge gaps that are in urgent need of filling by examining what is known about how individuals differ within and between typical and atypical groups.

Sections
References
  • AddabboM.LonghiE.BologniniN.SennaI.TagliabueP.CassiaV. M.TuratiC. (2015). Seeing touches early in lifePloS One 10e0134549. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0134549. Erratum in: PLoS One11 e0151294. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0151294.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AdlerJ.SchabingerN.MichalM.BeutelM. E.GillmeisterH. (2016). Is that me in the mirror? Depersonalisation modulates tactile mirroring mechanismsNeuropsychologia 85148158.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AdolphsR.DamasioH.TranelD.CooperG.DamasioA. R. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mappingJ. Neurosci. 2026832690.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aimola DaviesA. M.WhiteR. C. (2013). A sensational illusion: vision-touch synaesthesia and the rubber hand paradigmCortex 49806818.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AllisonT.McCarthyG.WoodC. C.WilliamsonP. D.SpencerD. D. (1989). Human cortical potentials evoked by stimulation of the median nerve. Cytoarchitectonic areas generating long-latency activityJ. Neurophysiol. 62711722.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AllisonT.McCarthyG.WoodC. C. (1992). The relationship between human long-latency somatosensory evoked potentials recorded from the cortical surface and from the scalpElectroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 84301314.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AspellJ. E.PalluelE.BlankeO. (2012). Early and late activity in somatosensory cortex reflects changes in bodily self-consciousness: an evoked potential studyNeuroscience 216110122.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AtkinsonJ.BraddickO. (1982). Sensory and perceptual capacities of the neonate in: Psychobiology of the Human NewbornStrattonP. M. (Ed.) pp.  191220. WileyLondon, UK.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AvenantiA.BuetiD.GalatiG.AgliotiS. M. (2005). Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for painNat. Neurosci. 8955960.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AvikainenS.ForssN.HariR. (2002). Modulated activation of the human SI and SII cortices during observation of hand actionsNeuroimage 15640646.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BachP.Fenton-AdamsW.TipperS. P. (2014). Can’t touch this: the first-person perspective provides privileged access to predictions of sensory action outcomesJ. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40(2) 457464.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BagbyR. M.ParkerJ. D. A.TaylorG. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structureJ. Psychosom. Res. 382332.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BaileyP. E.HenryJ. D.Von HippelW. (2008). Empathy and social functioning in late adulthoodAging Ment. Health 12499503.

  • BanissyM. J.WardJ. (2007). Mirror-touch synaesthesia is linked with empathyNat. Neurosci. 10815816.

  • BanissyM. J.WardJ. (2013). Mechanisms of self–other representations and vicarious experiences of touch in mirror-touch synaesthesiaFront. Hum. Neurosci. 7112. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00112.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BanissyM. J.Cohen KadoshR.MausG. W.WalshV.WardJ. (2009). Prevalence, characteristics, and a neurocognitive model of mirror-touch synaesthesiaExp. Brain Res. 198261272.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baron-CohenS.WheelwrightS. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger’s syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differencesJ. Autism Dev. Disord. 34163175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BernhardtJ. (1987). Sensory capabilities of the fetusMCN Am. J. Matern. Child Nurs. 124447.

  • BirdG.VidingE. (2014). The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymiaNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47520532.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BirdG.SilaniG.BrindleyR.WhiteS.FrithU.SingerT. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autismBrain 13315151525.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BjörnsdotterM.OlaussonH. (2011). Vicarious responses to social touch in posterior insular cortex are tuned to pleasant caressing speedsJ. Neurosci. 3195549562.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BlakemoreS. J.BristowD.BirdG.FrithC.WardJ. (2005). Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision–touch synaesthesiaBrain 12815711583.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BlankeO.SlaterM.SerinoA. (2015). Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousnessNeuron 88145166.

  • BocciaM. L. (1986). Grooming site preferences as a form of tactile communication and their role in the social relations of rhesus monkeys in: Current Perspectives in Primate Social DynamicsTaubD. M.KingF. A. (Eds) pp.  505518. Van Nostrand ReinholdNew York, NY, USA.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BologniniN.MaravitaA. (2007). Proprioceptive alignment of visual and somatosensory maps in the posterior parietal cortexCurr. Biol. 1718901895.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BologniniN.RossettiA.MaravitaA.MiniussiC. (2011). Seeing touch in the somatosensory cortex: a TMS study of the visual perception of touchHum. Brain Mapp. 3221042114.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BologniniN.MiniussiC.GalloS.VallarG. (2013a). Induction of mirror-touch synaesthesia by increasing somatosensory cortical excitabilityCurr. Biol. 23436437.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BologniniN.RossettiA.ConventoS.VallarG. (2013b). Understanding others’ feelings: the role of the right primary somatosensory cortex in encoding the affective valence of others’ touchJ. Neurosci. 3342014205.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BologniniN.RossettiA.FusaroM.VallarG.MiniussiC. (2014). Sharing social touch in the primary somatosensory cortexCurr. Biol. 2415131517.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BotvinickM.CohenJ. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes seeNature 391(6669) 756.

  • BrassM.RubyP.SpenglerS. (2009). Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognitionPhil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364(1528) 23592367.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BufalariI.IontaS. (2013). The social and personality neuroscience of empathy for pain and touchFront. Hum. Neurosci. 7393. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00393.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BufalariI.AprileT.AvenantiA.Di RussoF.AgliotiS. M. (2007). Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortexCereb. Cortex 1725532561.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CardiniF.CostantiniM.GalatiG.RomaniG. L.LàdavasE.SerinoA. (2011). Viewing one’s own face being touched modulates tactile perception: an fMRI studyJ. Cogn. Neurosci. 23503513.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CardiniF.Tajadura-JiménezA.SerinoA.TsakirisM. (2013). It feels like it’s me: interpersonal multisensory stimulation enhances visual remapping of touch from other to selfJ. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39630637.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ChanA. W. Y.BakerC. I. (2015). Seeing is not feeling: posterior parietal but not somatosensory cortex engagement during touch observationJ. Neurosci. 3514681480.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CioffiM. C.BanissyM. J.MooreJ. W. (2016). ‘Am I moving?’ An illusion of agency and ownership in mirror-touch synaesthesiaCognition 146426430.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CoelhoA. M.Jr.TurnerS. A.BramblettC. A. (1983). Allogrooming and social status: an assessment of the contributions of female behavior to the social organization of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas)Primates 24184197.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CollM. P.BirdG.CatmurC.PressC. (2015). Cross-modal repetition effects in the mu rhythm indicate tactile mirroring during action observationCortex 63121131.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CritchleyH. D.WiensS.RotshteinP.ÖhmanA.DolanR. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awarenessNat. Neurosci. 7189195.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DavisM. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approachJ. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44113126.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • De GuzmanM.BirdG.BanissyM. J.CatmurC. (2016). Self–other control processes in social cognition: from imitation to empathyPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371(1686) 20150079. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2015.0079.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DeschrijverE.WiersemaJ. R.BrassM. (2015). The interaction between felt touch and tactile consequences of observed actions: an action-based somatosensory congruency paradigmSoc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1111621172.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DohertyR. W. (1997). The emotional contagion scale: a measure of individual differencesJ. Nonverb. Behav. 21131154.

  • EbischS. J.PerrucciM. G.FerrettiA.Del GrattaC.RomaniG. L.GalleseV. (2008). The sense of touch: embodied simulation in a visuotactile mirroring mechanism for observed animate or inanimate touchJ. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016111623.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • EbischS. J.FerriF.SaloneA.PerrucciM. G.D’AmicoL.FerroF. M.RomaniG. L.GalleseV. (2011). Differential involvement of somatosensory and interoceptive cortices during the observation of affective touchJ. Cogn. Neurosci. 2318081822.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ElbertT.PantevC.WienbruchC.RockstrohB.TaubE. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string playersScience 270(5234) 305307.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ErnstJ.NorthoffG.BökerH.SeifritzE.GrimmS. (2013). Interoceptive awareness enhances neural activity during empathyHum. Brain Mapp. 3416151624.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • EshkevariE.RiegerE.LongoM. R.HaggardP.TreasureJ. (2012). Increased plasticity of the bodily self in eating disordersPsychol. Med. 42819828.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FilippettiM. L.JohnsonM. H.Lloyd-FoxS.DragovicD.FarroniT. (2013). Body perception in newbornsCurr. Biol. 2324132416.

  • FilippettiM. L.Lloyd-FoxS.LongoM. R.FarroniT.JohnsonM. H. (2014). Neural mechanisms of body awareness in infantsCereb. Cortex 2537793787.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FilippettiM. L.OrioliG.JohnsonM. H.FarroniT. (2015). Newborn body perception: sensitivity to spatial congruencInfancy 20455465.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FiniC.CardiniF.Tajadura-JiménezA.SerinoA.TsakirisM. (2013). Embodying an outgroup: the role of racial bias and the effect of multisensory processing in somatosensory remappingFront. Behav. Neurosci. 7165. DOI:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00165.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GallaceA.SpenceC. (2010). The science of interpersonal touch: an overviewNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34246259.

  • GalleseV.SinigagliaC. (2010). The bodily self as power for actionNeuropsychologia 48746755.

  • GazzolaV.Aziz-ZadehL.KeysersC. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humansCurr. Biol. 1618241829.

  • GillmeisterH. (2014). A new perceptual paradigm to investigate the visual remapping of others’ tactile sensations onto one’s own body shows “mirror touch” for the handsFr. Psychol. 595. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00095.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GoddeB.StauffenbergB.SpenglerF.DinseH. R. (2000). Tactile coactivation-induced changes in spatial discrimination performanceJ. Neurosci. 2015971604.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HeydrichL.BlankeO. (2013). Distinct illusory own-body perceptions caused by damage to posterior insula and extrastriate cortexBrain 136790803.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HeyesC. (2014). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mindPerspect. Psychol. Sci. 9131143.

  • HogeveenJ.ObhiS. S.BanissyM. J.SantiestebanI.PressC.CatmurC.BirdG. (2015). Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior frontal cortex in the control of imitationSoc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1010031009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HolleH.BanissyM. J.WrightT.BowlingN.WardJ. (2011). “That’s not a real body”: identifying stimulus qualities that modulate synaesthetic experiences of touchConsc. Cogn. 20720726.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HolleH.BanissyM. J.WardJ. (2013). Functional and structural brain differences associated with mirror-touch synaesthesiaNeuroimage 8310411050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HrbekA.KarlbergP.OlssonT. (1973). Development of visual and somatosensory evoked responses in pre-term newborn infantsElectroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 34225232.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HunterE. C.SierraM.DavidA. S. (2004). The epidemiology of depersonalisation and derealisationSoc. Psychiat. Psychiat. Epidemiol. 39918.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • IshidaH.NakajimaK.InaseM.MurataA. (2010). Shared mapping of own and others’ bodies in visuotactile bimodal area of monkey parietal cortexJ. Cogn. Neurosci. 228396.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KalischT.RagertP.SchwenkreisP.DinseH. R.TegenthoffM. (2009). Impaired tactile acuity in old age is accompanied by enlarged hand representations in somatosensory cortexCereb. Cortex 1915301538.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KanayamaN.SatoA.OhiraH. (2009). The role of gamma band oscillations and synchrony on rubber hand illusion and crossmodal integrationBrain Cogn. 691929.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KeysersC.GazzolaV. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensationsCurr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19666671.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KeysersC.WickerB.GazzolaV.AntonJ. L.FogassiL.GalleseV. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touchNeuron 42335346.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KeysersC.KaasJ. H.GazzolaV. (2010). Somatosensation in social perceptionNat. Rev. Neurosci. 11417428.

  • KressI. U.MinatiL.FerraroS.CritchleyH. D. (2011). Direct skin-to-skin vs. indirect touch modulates neural responses to stroking vs. tappingNeuroreport 22646651.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KuehnE.TrampelR.MuellerK.TurnerR.Schütz-BosbachS. (2013). Judging roughness by sight — a 7-tesla fMRI study on responsivity of the primary somatosensory cortex during observed touch of self and othersHum. Brain Mapp. 3418821895.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KuehnE.MuellerK.TurnerR.Schütz-BosbachS. (2014). The functional architecture of S1 during touch observation described with 7 T fMRIBrain Struct. Funct. 219119140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KuehnE.MuellerK.LohmannG.Schuetz-BosbachS. (2016). Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profileBrain Struct. Funct. 22115551571.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LammC.DecetyJ.SingerT. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 5424922502.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LammC.SilaniG.SingerT. (2015). Distinct neural networks underlying empathy for pleasant and unpleasant touchCortex 707989.

  • LammC.BukowskiH.SilaniG. (2016). From shared to distinct self–other representations in empathy: evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disordersPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371(1686) 20150083. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2015.0083.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LaneR. D.SechrestL.RiedelR. (1998). Sociodemographic correlates of alexithymiaCompr. Psychiat. 39377385.

  • LankinenK.SmedsE.TikkaP.PihkoE.HariR.KoskinenM. (2016). Haptic contents of a movie dynamically engage the spectator’s sensorimotor cortexHum. Brain Mapp. 3740614068.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LegrandD. (2006). The bodily self: the sensori-motor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousnessPhenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 589118.

  • LightK. C.GrewenK. M.AmicoJ. A. (2005). More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal womenBiol. Psychol. 69521.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LockwoodP. L. (2016). The anatomy of empathy: vicarious experience and disorders of social cognitionBehav. Brain Res. 311255266.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MahayanaI. T.BanissyM. J.ChenC. Y.WalshV.JuanC. H.MuggletonN. G. (2014). Motor empathy is a consequence of misattribution of sensory information in observersFront. Hum. Neurosci. 847. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00047.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MaisterL.BanissyM. J.TsakirisM. (2013). Mirror-touch synaesthesia changes representations of self-identityNeuropsychologia 51802808.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MarshallP. J.MeltzoffA. N. (2014). Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infantsPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369(1644) 20130620. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2013.0620.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MarshallP. J.SabyJ. N.MeltzoffA. N. (2013). Imitation and the developing social brain: infants somatotopic EEG patterns for acts of self and otherInt. J. Psychol. Res. (Medellin) 62229.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Martinez-JauandM.Gonzalez-RoldanA. M.MunozM. A.SitgesC.CifreI.MontoyaP. (2012). Somatosensory activity modulation during observation of other’s pain and touchBrain Res. 14674855.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MattilaA. K.SalminenJ. K.NummiT.JoukamaaM. (2006). Age is strongly associated with alexithymia in the general populationJ. Psychosom. Res. 61629635.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MehlingW. E.PriceC.DaubenmierJ. J.AcreeM.BartmessE.StewartA. (2012). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA)PLoS One 7e48230. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0048230.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MeyerK.KaplanJ. T.EssexR.DamasioH.DamasioA. (2011). Seeing touch is correlated with content-specific activity in primary somatosensory cortexCereb. Cortex 2121132121. DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhq289.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MolenberghsP.CunningtonR.MattingleyJ. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studiesNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36341349.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Molnar-SzakacsI.UddinL. Q. (2013). Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron systemFront. Hum. Neurosci. 7571. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MooradianT. A.DavisM.MatzlerK. (2011). Dispositional empathy and the hierarchical structure of personalityAm. J. Psychol. 12499109.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MussapA. J.SaltonN. (2006). A ‘rubber-hand’ illusion reveals a relationship between perceptual body image and unhealthy body changeJ. Health Psychol. 11627639.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NummenmaaL.SmirnovD.LahnakoskiJ. M.GlereanE.JääskeläinenI. P.SamsM.HariR. (2014). Mental action simulation synchronizes action–observation circuits across individualsJ. Neurosci. 34748757.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • OtsuruN.HashizumeA.NakamuraD.EndoY.InuiK.KakigiR.YugeL. (2014). Sensory incongruence leading to hand disownership modulates somatosensory cortical processingCortex 5818.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ParadisoS.VaidyaJ. G.McCormickL. M.JonesA.RobinsonR. G. (2008). Aging and alexithymia: association with reduced right rostral cingulate volumeAm. J. Ger. Psychiat. 16760769.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pascual-LeoneA.TorresF. (1993). Plasticity of the sensorimotor cortex representation of the reading finger in Braille readersBrain 1163952.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PihkoE.NanginiC.JousmäkiV.HariR. (2010). Observing touch activates human primary somatosensory cortexEur. J. Neurosci. 3118361843.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PinedaJ. A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing”Brain Res. Rev. 505768.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RagertP.SchmidtA.AltenmüllerE.DinseH. R. (2004). Superior tactile performance and learning in professional pianists: evidence for meta-plasticity in musiciansEur. J. Neurosci. 19473478.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RemijnG. B.KikuchiM.ShitamichiK.UenoS.YoshimuraY.NagaoK.TsubokawaT.KojimaH.HigashidaH.MinabeY. (2014). Somatosensory evoked field in response to visuotactile stimulation in 3-to 4-year-old childrenFront. Hum. Neurosci. 8170. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00170.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RigatoS.AliJ. B.Van VelzenJ.BremnerA. J. (2014). The neural basis of somatosensory remapping develops in human infancyCurr. Biol. 2412221226.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RizzolattiG.CraigheroL. (2004). The mirror-neuron systemAnnu. Rev. Neurosci. 27169192.

  • RochatP.StrianoT. (2000). Perceived self in infancyInfant Behav. Dev. 23513530.

  • RossettiA.MiniussiC.MaravitaA.BologniniN. (2012). Visual perception of bodily interactions in the primary somatosensory cortexEur. J. Neurosci. 3623172323.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RubyP.DecetyJ. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agencyNat. Neurosci. 4546550.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RuffmanT.HenryJ. D.LivingstoneV.PhillipsL. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: implications for neuropsychological models of agingNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32863881.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RutherfordM. D. (2004). The effect of social role on theory of mind reasoningBr. J. Psychol. 9591103.

  • SabyJ. N.MeltzoffA. N.MarshallP. J. (2013). Infants’ somatotopic neural responses to seeing human actions: I’ve got you under my skinPLoS One 8e77905. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0077905.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SabyJ. N.MeltzoffA. N.MarshallP. J. (2015). Neural body maps in human infants: somatotopic responses to tactile stimulation in 7-month-oldsNeuroimage 1187478.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SantiestebanI.BanissyM. J.CatmurC.BirdG. (2012). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junctionCurr. Biol. 2222742277.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SantiestebanI.BirdG.TewO.CioffiM. C.BanissyM. J. (2015a). Mirror-touch synaesthesia: difficulties inhibiting the otherCortex 71116121.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SantiestebanI.ShahP.WhiteS.BirdG.HeyesC. (2015b). Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera controlPsychon. Bull. Rev. 22844849.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SchaeferM.FlorH.HeinzeH. J.RotteM. (2006). Dynamic modulation of the primary somatosensory cortex during seeing and feeling a touched handNeuroimage 29587592.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SchaeferM.XuB.FlorH.CohenL. G. (2009). Effects of different viewing perspectives on somatosensory activations during observation of touchHum. Brain Mapp. 3027222730.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SchaeferM.HeinzeH. J.RotteM. (2012). Embodied empathy for tactile events: interindividual differences and vicarious somatosensory responses during touch observationNeuroimage 60952957.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SchaeferM.RotteM.HeinzeH. J.DenkeC. (2013). Mirror-like brain responses to observed touch and personality dimensionsFront. Hum. Neurosci. 7227. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00227.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SchubertR.RitterP.WustenbergT.PreuschhofC.CurioG.SommerW. (2008). Spatial attention related SEP amplitude modulations covary with BOLD signal in S1 — a simultaneous EEG–fMRI studyCereb. Cortex 1826862700.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SerinoA.PizzoferratoF.LàdavasE. (2008). Viewing a face especially one’s own face being touched enhances tactile perception on the facePsychol. Sci. 19434438.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SerinoA.GiovagnoliG.LàdavasE. (2009). I feel what you feel if you are similar to mePlos One 4(3) e4930. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0004930.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SierraM.LoperaF.LambertM. V.PhillipsM. L.DavidA. S. (2002). Separating depersonalisation and derealisation: the relevance of the “lesion method”J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 72530532.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SimeonD. (2004). Depersonalisation disorder: a contemporary overviewCNS Drugs 18343354.

  • SimeonD.GiesbrechtT.KnutelskaM.SmithR. J.SmithL. M. (2009). Alexithymia, absorption, and cognitive failures in depersonalization disorder: a comparison to posttraumatic stress disorder and healthy volunteersJ. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 197492498.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SingerT.SeymourB.O’DohertyJ.KaubeH.DolanR. J.FrithC. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of painScience 30311571162.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SpenglerF.GoddeB.DinseH. R. (1995). Effects of ageing on topographic organization of somatosensory cortexNeuroreport 6469473.

  • StreltsovaA.McCleeryJ. P. (2014). Neural time-course of the observation of human and non-human object touchSoc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9333341.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TamirD. I.MitchellJ. P. (2010). Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizingProc. Natl Acad. Sci. 1071082710832.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TaylorM. J.BoorR.EkertP. G. (1996). Preterm maturation of the somatosensory evoked potentialElectroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 100448452.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ThakkarK. N.NicholsH. S.McIntoshL. G.ParkS. (2011). Disturbances in body ownership in schizophrenia: evidence from the rubber hand illusion and case study of a spontaneous out-of-body experiencePloS One 6e27089. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0027089.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TsakirisM. (2008). Looking for myself: current multisensory input alters self-face recognitionPloS One 3e4040. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0004040.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TsakirisM.Tajadura-JiménezA.CostantiniM. (2011). Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representationsProc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 278(1717) 24702476.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • VandenbrouckeS.CrombezG.LoeysT.GoubertL. (2015). Vicarious experiences and detection accuracy while observing pain and touch: the effect of perspective takingAtten. Percept. Psychophys. 7717811793.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WardJ.BanissyM. J. (2015). Explaining mirror-touch synaesthesiaCogn. Neurosci. 6118147.

  • WhiteR. C.DaviesA. M. A. (2012). Specular vision–touch synaesthesia: two reference framesPerception 41871874.

  • WoodA.RychlowskaM.KorbS.NiedenthalP. (2016). Fashioning the face: sensorimotor simulation contributes to facial expression recognitionTrends Cogn. Sci. 20227240.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WoodR.GalleseV.CattaneoL. (2010). Visuotactile empathy within the primary somatosensory cortex revealed by short-latency afferent inhibitionNeuroscience Lett. 4732831.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • YangT.BanissyM. J. (2016). Empathy and aging: mechanisms of empathy throughout adulthood in: Psychology and Neurobiology of EmpathyWattD. F.PankseppJ. (Eds). Nova Science PublishersNew York.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ZmyjN.JankJ.Schutz-BosbachS.DaumM. M. (2011). Detection of visual-tactile contingency in the first year after birthCognition 1208289.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
Index Card
Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 155 138 8
Full Text Views 185 176 1
PDF Downloads 16 15 0
EPUB Downloads 1 1 0