Touch is our most interpersonal sense, and so it stands to reason that we represent not only our own bodily experiences, but also those felt by others. This review will summarise brain and behavioural research on vicarious tactile perception (mirror touch). Specifically, we will focus on vicarious touch across the lifespan in typical and atypical groups, and will identify the knowledge gaps that are in urgent need of filling by examining what is known about how individuals differ within and between typical and atypical groups.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Addabbo M., Longhi E., Bolognini N., Senna I., Tagliabue P., Cassia V. M., Turati C. (2015). Seeing touches early in life, PloS One 10, e0134549. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0134549. Erratum in: PLoS One11, e0151294. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0151294.
Adler J., Schabinger N., Michal M., Beutel M. E., Gillmeister H. (2016). Is that me in the mirror? Depersonalisation modulates tactile mirroring mechanisms, Neuropsychologia 85, 148–158.
Adolphs R., Damasio H., Tranel D., Cooper G., Damasio A. R. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping, J. Neurosci. 20, 2683–2690.
Aimola Davies A. M., White R. C. (2013). A sensational illusion: vision-touch synaesthesia and the rubber hand paradigm, Cortex 49, 806–818.
Allison T., McCarthy G., Wood C. C., Williamson P. D., Spencer D. D. (1989). Human cortical potentials evoked by stimulation of the median nerve. Cytoarchitectonic areas generating long-latency activity, J. Neurophysiol. 62, 711–722.
Allison T., McCarthy G., Wood C. C. (1992). The relationship between human long-latency somatosensory evoked potentials recorded from the cortical surface and from the scalp, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 84, 301–314.
Aspell J. E., Palluel E., Blanke O. (2012). Early and late activity in somatosensory cortex reflects changes in bodily self-consciousness: an evoked potential study, Neuroscience 216, 110–122.
Atkinson J., Braddick O. (1982). Sensory and perceptual capacities of the neonate, in: Psychobiology of the Human Newborn, Stratton P. M. (Ed.), pp. 191–220. Wiley, London, UK.
Avenanti A., Bueti D., Galati G., Aglioti S. M. (2005). Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain, Nat. Neurosci. 8, 955–960.
Avikainen S., Forss N., Hari R. (2002). Modulated activation of the human SI and SII cortices during observation of hand actions, Neuroimage 15, 640–646.
Bach P., Fenton-Adams W., Tipper S. P. (2014). Can’t touch this: the first-person perspective provides privileged access to predictions of sensory action outcomes, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40(2), 457–464.
Bagby R. M., Parker J. D. A., Taylor G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure, J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 23–32.
Bailey P. E., Henry J. D., Von Hippel W. (2008). Empathy and social functioning in late adulthood, Aging Ment. Health 12, 499–503.
Banissy M. J., Ward J. (2007). Mirror-touch synaesthesia is linked with empathy, Nat. Neurosci. 10, 815–816.
Banissy M. J., Ward J. (2013). Mechanisms of self–other representations and vicarious experiences of touch in mirror-touch synaesthesia, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 112. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00112.
Banissy M. J., Cohen Kadosh R., Maus G. W., Walsh V., Ward J. (2009). Prevalence, characteristics, and a neurocognitive model of mirror-touch synaesthesia, Exp. Brain Res. 198, 261–272.
Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger’s syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences, J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175.
Bernhardt J. (1987). Sensory capabilities of the fetus, MCN Am. J. Matern. Child Nurs. 12, 44–47.
Bird G., Viding E. (2014). The self to other model of empathy: providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 520–532.
Bird G., Silani G., Brindley R., White S., Frith U., Singer T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism, Brain 133, 1515–1525.
Björnsdotter M., Olausson H. (2011). Vicarious responses to social touch in posterior insular cortex are tuned to pleasant caressing speeds, J. Neurosci. 31, 9554–9562.
Blakemore S. J., Bristow D., Bird G., Frith C., Ward J. (2005). Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision–touch synaesthesia, Brain 128, 1571–1583.
Blanke O., Slater M., Serino A. (2015). Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness, Neuron 88, 145–166.
Boccia M. L. (1986). Grooming site preferences as a form of tactile communication and their role in the social relations of rhesus monkeys, in: Current Perspectives in Primate Social Dynamics, Taub D. M., King F. A. (Eds), pp. 505–518. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, NY, USA.
Bolognini N., Maravita A. (2007). Proprioceptive alignment of visual and somatosensory maps in the posterior parietal cortex, Curr. Biol. 17, 1890–1895.
Bolognini N., Rossetti A., Maravita A., Miniussi C. (2011). Seeing touch in the somatosensory cortex: a TMS study of the visual perception of touch, Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 2104–2114.
Bolognini N., Miniussi C., Gallo S., Vallar G. (2013a). Induction of mirror-touch synaesthesia by increasing somatosensory cortical excitability, Curr. Biol. 23, 436–437.
Bolognini N., Rossetti A., Convento S., Vallar G. (2013b). Understanding others’ feelings: the role of the right primary somatosensory cortex in encoding the affective valence of others’ touch, J. Neurosci. 33, 4201–4205.
Bolognini N., Rossetti A., Fusaro M., Vallar G., Miniussi C. (2014). Sharing social touch in the primary somatosensory cortex, Curr. Biol. 24, 1513–1517.
Botvinick M., Cohen J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see, Nature 391(6669), 756.
Brass M., Ruby P., Spengler S. (2009). Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364(1528), 2359–2367.
Bufalari I., Ionta S. (2013). The social and personality neuroscience of empathy for pain and touch, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 393. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00393.
Bufalari I., Aprile T., Avenanti A., Di Russo F., Aglioti S. M. (2007). Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex, Cereb. Cortex 17, 2553–2561.
Cardini F., Costantini M., Galati G., Romani G. L., Làdavas E., Serino A. (2011). Viewing one’s own face being touched modulates tactile perception: an fMRI study, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 503–513.
Cardini F., Tajadura-Jiménez A., Serino A., Tsakiris M. (2013). It feels like it’s me: interpersonal multisensory stimulation enhances visual remapping of touch from other to self, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 630–637.
Chan A. W. Y., Baker C. I. (2015). Seeing is not feeling: posterior parietal but not somatosensory cortex engagement during touch observation, J. Neurosci. 35, 1468–1480.
Cioffi M. C., Banissy M. J., Moore J. W. (2016). ‘Am I moving?’ An illusion of agency and ownership in mirror-touch synaesthesia, Cognition 146, 426–430.
Coelho A. M. Jr., Turner S. A., Bramblett C. A. (1983). Allogrooming and social status: an assessment of the contributions of female behavior to the social organization of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas), Primates 24, 184–197.
Coll M. P., Bird G., Catmur C., Press C. (2015). Cross-modal repetition effects in the mu rhythm indicate tactile mirroring during action observation, Cortex 63, 121–131.
Critchley H. D., Wiens S., Rotshtein P., Öhman A., Dolan R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness, Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195.
Davis M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126.
De Guzman M., Bird G., Banissy M. J., Catmur C. (2016). Self–other control processes in social cognition: from imitation to empathy, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371(1686), 20150079. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2015.0079.
Deschrijver E., Wiersema J. R., Brass M. (2015). The interaction between felt touch and tactile consequences of observed actions: an action-based somatosensory congruency paradigm, Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1162–1172.
Doherty R. W. (1997). The emotional contagion scale: a measure of individual differences, J. Nonverb. Behav. 21, 131–154.
Ebisch S. J., Perrucci M. G., Ferretti A., Del Gratta C., Romani G. L., Gallese V. (2008). The sense of touch: embodied simulation in a visuotactile mirroring mechanism for observed animate or inanimate touch, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1611–1623.
Ebisch S. J., Ferri F., Salone A., Perrucci M. G., D’Amico L., Ferro F. M., Romani G. L., Gallese V. (2011). Differential involvement of somatosensory and interoceptive cortices during the observation of affective touch, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1808–1822.
Elbert T., Pantev C., Wienbruch C., Rockstroh B., Taub E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players, Science 270(5234), 305–307.
Ernst J., Northoff G., Böker H., Seifritz E., Grimm S. (2013). Interoceptive awareness enhances neural activity during empathy, Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1615–1624.
Eshkevari E., Rieger E., Longo M. R., Haggard P., Treasure J. (2012). Increased plasticity of the bodily self in eating disorders, Psychol. Med. 42, 819–828.
Filippetti M. L., Johnson M. H., Lloyd-Fox S., Dragovic D., Farroni T. (2013). Body perception in newborns, Curr. Biol. 23, 2413–2416.
Filippetti M. L., Lloyd-Fox S., Longo M. R., Farroni T., Johnson M. H. (2014). Neural mechanisms of body awareness in infants, Cereb. Cortex 25, 3779–3787.
Filippetti M. L., Orioli G., Johnson M. H., Farroni T. (2015). Newborn body perception: sensitivity to spatial congruenc, Infancy 20, 455–465.
Fini C., Cardini F., Tajadura-Jiménez A., Serino A., Tsakiris M. (2013). Embodying an outgroup: the role of racial bias and the effect of multisensory processing in somatosensory remapping, Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 165. DOI:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00165.
Gallace A., Spence C. (2010). The science of interpersonal touch: an overview, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 246–259.
Gallese V., Sinigaglia C. (2010). The bodily self as power for action, Neuropsychologia 48, 746–755.
Gazzola V., Aziz-Zadeh L., Keysers C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans, Curr. Biol. 16, 1824–1829.
Gillmeister H. (2014). A new perceptual paradigm to investigate the visual remapping of others’ tactile sensations onto one’s own body shows “mirror touch” for the hands, Fr. Psychol. 5, 95. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00095.
Godde B., Stauffenberg B., Spengler F., Dinse H. R. (2000). Tactile coactivation-induced changes in spatial discrimination performance, J. Neurosci. 20, 1597–1604.
Heydrich L., Blanke O. (2013). Distinct illusory own-body perceptions caused by damage to posterior insula and extrastriate cortex, Brain 136, 790–803.
Heyes C. (2014). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind, Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 131–143.
Hogeveen J., Obhi S. S., Banissy M. J., Santiesteban I., Press C., Catmur C., Bird G. (2015). Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior frontal cortex in the control of imitation, Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1003–1009.
Holle H., Banissy M. J., Wright T., Bowling N., Ward J. (2011). “That’s not a real body”: identifying stimulus qualities that modulate synaesthetic experiences of touch, Consc. Cogn. 20, 720–726.
Holle H., Banissy M. J., Ward J. (2013). Functional and structural brain differences associated with mirror-touch synaesthesia, Neuroimage 83, 1041–1050.
Hrbek A., Karlberg P., Olsson T. (1973). Development of visual and somatosensory evoked responses in pre-term newborn infants, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 34, 225–232.
Hunter E. C., Sierra M., David A. S. (2004). The epidemiology of depersonalisation and derealisation, Soc. Psychiat. Psychiat. Epidemiol. 39, 9–18.
Ishida H., Nakajima K., Inase M., Murata A. (2010). Shared mapping of own and others’ bodies in visuotactile bimodal area of monkey parietal cortex, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 83–96.
Kalisch T., Ragert P., Schwenkreis P., Dinse H. R., Tegenthoff M. (2009). Impaired tactile acuity in old age is accompanied by enlarged hand representations in somatosensory cortex, Cereb. Cortex 19, 1530–1538.
Kanayama N., Sato A., Ohira H. (2009). The role of gamma band oscillations and synchrony on rubber hand illusion and crossmodal integration, Brain Cogn. 69, 19–29.
Keysers C., Gazzola V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 666–671.
Keysers C., Wicker B., Gazzola V., Anton J. L., Fogassi L., Gallese V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch, Neuron 42, 335–346.
Keysers C., Kaas J. H., Gazzola V. (2010). Somatosensation in social perception, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 417–428.
Kress I. U., Minati L., Ferraro S., Critchley H. D. (2011). Direct skin-to-skin vs. indirect touch modulates neural responses to stroking vs. tapping, Neuroreport 22, 646–651.
Kuehn E., Trampel R., Mueller K., Turner R., Schütz-Bosbach S. (2013). Judging roughness by sight — a 7-tesla fMRI study on responsivity of the primary somatosensory cortex during observed touch of self and others, Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1882–1895.
Kuehn E., Mueller K., Turner R., Schütz-Bosbach S. (2014). The functional architecture of S1 during touch observation described with 7 T fMRI, Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 119–140.
Kuehn E., Mueller K., Lohmann G., Schuetz-Bosbach S. (2016). Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile, Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1555–1571.
Lamm C., Decety J., Singer T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain, Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502.
Lamm C., Silani G., Singer T. (2015). Distinct neural networks underlying empathy for pleasant and unpleasant touch, Cortex 70, 79–89.
Lamm C., Bukowski H., Silani G. (2016). From shared to distinct self–other representations in empathy: evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371(1686), 20150083. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2015.0083.
Lane R. D., Sechrest L., Riedel R. (1998). Sociodemographic correlates of alexithymia, Compr. Psychiat. 39, 377–385.
Lankinen K., Smeds E., Tikka P., Pihko E., Hari R., Koskinen M. (2016). Haptic contents of a movie dynamically engage the spectator’s sensorimotor cortex, Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 4061–4068.
Legrand D. (2006). The bodily self: the sensori-motor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousness, Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 5, 89–118.
Light K. C., Grewen K. M., Amico J. A. (2005). More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women, Biol. Psychol. 69, 5–21.
Lockwood P. L. (2016). The anatomy of empathy: vicarious experience and disorders of social cognition, Behav. Brain Res. 311, 255–266.
Mahayana I. T., Banissy M. J., Chen C. Y., Walsh V., Juan C. H., Muggleton N. G. (2014). Motor empathy is a consequence of misattribution of sensory information in observers, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 47. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00047.
Maister L., Banissy M. J., Tsakiris M. (2013). Mirror-touch synaesthesia changes representations of self-identity, Neuropsychologia 51, 802–808.
Marshall P. J., Meltzoff A. N. (2014). Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infants, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369(1644), 20130620. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2013.0620.
Marshall P. J., Saby J. N., Meltzoff A. N. (2013). Imitation and the developing social brain: infants somatotopic EEG patterns for acts of self and other, Int. J. Psychol. Res. (Medellin) 6, 22–29.
Martinez-Jauand M., Gonzalez-Roldan A. M., Munoz M. A., Sitges C., Cifre I., Montoya P. (2012). Somatosensory activity modulation during observation of other’s pain and touch, Brain Res. 1467, 48–55.
Mattila A. K., Salminen J. K., Nummi T., Joukamaa M. (2006). Age is strongly associated with alexithymia in the general population, J. Psychosom. Res. 61, 629–635.
Mehling W. E., Price C., Daubenmier J. J., Acree M., Bartmess E., Stewart A. (2012). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA), PLoS One 7, e48230. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0048230.
Meyer K., Kaplan J. T., Essex R., Damasio H., Damasio A. (2011). Seeing touch is correlated with content-specific activity in primary somatosensory cortex, Cereb. Cortex 21, 2113–2121. DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhq289.
Molenberghs P., Cunnington R., Mattingley J. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 341–349.
Molnar-Szakacs I., Uddin L. Q. (2013). Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron system, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 571. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571.
Mooradian T. A., Davis M., Matzler K. (2011). Dispositional empathy and the hierarchical structure of personality, Am. J. Psychol. 124, 99–109.
Mussap A. J., Salton N. (2006). A ‘rubber-hand’ illusion reveals a relationship between perceptual body image and unhealthy body change, J. Health Psychol. 11, 627–639.
Nummenmaa L., Smirnov D., Lahnakoski J. M., Glerean E., Jääskeläinen I. P., Sams M., Hari R. (2014). Mental action simulation synchronizes action–observation circuits across individuals, J. Neurosci. 34, 748–757.
Otsuru N., Hashizume A., Nakamura D., Endo Y., Inui K., Kakigi R., Yuge L. (2014). Sensory incongruence leading to hand disownership modulates somatosensory cortical processing, Cortex 58, 1–8.
Paradiso S., Vaidya J. G., McCormick L. M., Jones A., Robinson R. G. (2008). Aging and alexithymia: association with reduced right rostral cingulate volume, Am. J. Ger. Psychiat. 16, 760–769.
Pascual-Leone A., Torres F. (1993). Plasticity of the sensorimotor cortex representation of the reading finger in Braille readers, Brain 116, 39–52.
Pihko E., Nangini C., Jousmäki V., Hari R. (2010). Observing touch activates human primary somatosensory cortex, Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1836–1843.
Pineda J. A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing”, Brain Res. Rev. 50, 57–68.
Ragert P., Schmidt A., Altenmüller E., Dinse H. R. (2004). Superior tactile performance and learning in professional pianists: evidence for meta-plasticity in musicians, Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 473–478.
Remijn G. B., Kikuchi M., Shitamichi K., Ueno S., Yoshimura Y., Nagao K., Tsubokawa T., Kojima H., Higashida H., Minabe Y. (2014). Somatosensory evoked field in response to visuotactile stimulation in 3-to 4-year-old children, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 170. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00170.
Rigato S., Ali J. B., Van Velzen J., Bremner A. J. (2014). The neural basis of somatosensory remapping develops in human infancy, Curr. Biol. 24, 1222–1226.
Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192.
Rochat P., Striano T. (2000). Perceived self in infancy, Infant Behav. Dev. 23, 513–530.
Rossetti A., Miniussi C., Maravita A., Bolognini N. (2012). Visual perception of bodily interactions in the primary somatosensory cortex, Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2317–2323.
Ruby P., Decety J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency, Nat. Neurosci. 4, 546–550.
Ruffman T., Henry J. D., Livingstone V., Phillips L. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: implications for neuropsychological models of aging, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 863–881.
Rutherford M. D. (2004). The effect of social role on theory of mind reasoning, Br. J. Psychol. 95, 91–103.
Saby J. N., Meltzoff A. N., Marshall P. J. (2013). Infants’ somatotopic neural responses to seeing human actions: I’ve got you under my skin, PLoS One 8, e77905. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0077905.
Saby J. N., Meltzoff A. N., Marshall P. J. (2015). Neural body maps in human infants: somatotopic responses to tactile stimulation in 7-month-olds, Neuroimage 118, 74–78.
Santiesteban I., Banissy M. J., Catmur C., Bird G. (2012). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction, Curr. Biol. 22, 2274–2277.
Santiesteban I., Bird G., Tew O., Cioffi M. C., Banissy M. J. (2015a). Mirror-touch synaesthesia: difficulties inhibiting the other, Cortex 71, 116–121.
Santiesteban I., Shah P., White S., Bird G., Heyes C. (2015b). Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera control, Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 844–849.
Schaefer M., Flor H., Heinze H. J., Rotte M. (2006). Dynamic modulation of the primary somatosensory cortex during seeing and feeling a touched hand, Neuroimage 29, 587–592.
Schaefer M., Xu B., Flor H., Cohen L. G. (2009). Effects of different viewing perspectives on somatosensory activations during observation of touch, Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2722–2730.
Schaefer M., Heinze H. J., Rotte M. (2012). Embodied empathy for tactile events: interindividual differences and vicarious somatosensory responses during touch observation, Neuroimage 60, 952–957.
Schaefer M., Rotte M., Heinze H. J., Denke C. (2013). Mirror-like brain responses to observed touch and personality dimensions, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 227. DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00227.
Schubert R., Ritter P., Wustenberg T., Preuschhof C., Curio G., Sommer W. et al., (2008). Spatial attention related SEP amplitude modulations covary with BOLD signal in S1 — a simultaneous EEG–fMRI study, Cereb. Cortex 18, 2686–2700.
Serino A., Pizzoferrato F., Làdavas E. (2008). Viewing a face especially one’s own face being touched enhances tactile perception on the face, Psychol. Sci. 19, 434–438.
Serino A., Giovagnoli G., Làdavas E. (2009). I feel what you feel if you are similar to me, Plos One 4(3), e4930. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0004930.
Sierra M., Lopera F., Lambert M. V., Phillips M. L., David A. S. (2002). Separating depersonalisation and derealisation: the relevance of the “lesion method”, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 72, 530–532.
Simeon D. (2004). Depersonalisation disorder: a contemporary overview, CNS Drugs 18, 343–354.
Simeon D., Giesbrecht T., Knutelska M., Smith R. J., Smith L. M. (2009). Alexithymia, absorption, and cognitive failures in depersonalization disorder: a comparison to posttraumatic stress disorder and healthy volunteers, J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 197, 492–498.
Singer T., Seymour B., O’Doherty J., Kaube H., Dolan R. J., Frith C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain, Science 303, 1157–1162.
Spengler F., Godde B., Dinse H. R. (1995). Effects of ageing on topographic organization of somatosensory cortex, Neuroreport 6, 469–473.
Streltsova A., McCleery J. P. (2014). Neural time-course of the observation of human and non-human object touch, Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 333–341.
Tamir D. I., Mitchell J. P. (2010). Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizing, Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 107, 10827–10832.
Taylor M. J., Boor R., Ekert P. G. (1996). Preterm maturation of the somatosensory evoked potential, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 100, 448–452.
Thakkar K. N., Nichols H. S., McIntosh L. G., Park S. (2011). Disturbances in body ownership in schizophrenia: evidence from the rubber hand illusion and case study of a spontaneous out-of-body experience, PloS One 6, e27089. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0027089.
Tsakiris M. (2008). Looking for myself: current multisensory input alters self-face recognition, PloS One 3, e4040. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0004040.
Tsakiris M., Tajadura-Jiménez A., Costantini M. (2011). Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 278(1717), 2470–2476.
Vandenbroucke S., Crombez G., Loeys T., Goubert L. (2015). Vicarious experiences and detection accuracy while observing pain and touch: the effect of perspective taking, Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 77, 1781–1793.
Ward J., Banissy M. J. (2015). Explaining mirror-touch synaesthesia, Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 118–147.
White R. C., Davies A. M. A. (2012). Specular vision–touch synaesthesia: two reference frames, Perception 41, 871–874.
Wood A., Rychlowska M., Korb S., Niedenthal P. (2016). Fashioning the face: sensorimotor simulation contributes to facial expression recognition, Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 227–240.
Wood R., Gallese V., Cattaneo L. (2010). Visuotactile empathy within the primary somatosensory cortex revealed by short-latency afferent inhibition, Neuroscience Lett. 473, 28–31.
Yang T., Banissy M. J. (2016). Empathy and aging: mechanisms of empathy throughout adulthood, in: Psychology and Neurobiology of Empathy, Watt D. F., Panksepp J. (Eds). Nova Science Publishers, New York.
Zmyj N., Jank J., Schutz-Bosbach S., Daum M. M. (2011). Detection of visual-tactile contingency in the first year after birth, Cognition 120, 82–89.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 972 | 172 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 314 | 51 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 163 | 55 | 2 |
Touch is our most interpersonal sense, and so it stands to reason that we represent not only our own bodily experiences, but also those felt by others. This review will summarise brain and behavioural research on vicarious tactile perception (mirror touch). Specifically, we will focus on vicarious touch across the lifespan in typical and atypical groups, and will identify the knowledge gaps that are in urgent need of filling by examining what is known about how individuals differ within and between typical and atypical groups.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 972 | 172 | 22 |
Full Text Views | 314 | 51 | 1 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 163 | 55 | 2 |