Several studies have examined how music may affect the evaluation of food and drink, but the vast majority have not observed how this interaction unfolds in time. This seems to be quite relevant, since both music and the consumer experience of food/drink are time-varying in nature. In the present study we sought to fix this gap, using Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS), a method developed to record the dominant sensory attribute at any given moment in time, to examine the impact of music on the wine taster’s perception. More specifically, we assessed how the same red wine might be experienced differently when tasters were exposed to various sonic environments (two pieces of music plus a silent control condition). The results revealed diverse patterns of dominant flavours for each sound condition, with significant differences in flavour dominance in each music condition as compared to the silent control condition. Moreover, musical correspondence analysis revealed that differences in perceived dominance of acidity and bitterness in the wine were correlated in the temporality of the experience, with changes in basic auditory attributes. Potential implications for the role of attention in auditory flavour modification and opportunities for future studies are discussed.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Ashkenazi, A. and Marks, L. E. (2004). Effect of endogenous attention on detection of weak gustatory and olfactory flavors, Percept. Psychophys. 66, 596–608.
Bachorik, J. P., Bangert, M., Loui, P., Lark, K., Berger, J., Rowe, R. and Schlaug, G. (2009). Emotion in motion: investigating the time-course of emotional judgments of musical stimuli, Music Percept. 26, 355–364.
Bigand, E., Parncutt, R. and Lerdahl, F. (1996). Perception of musical tension in short chord sequences: the influence of harmonic function, sensory dissonance, horizontal motion, and musical training, Percept. Psychophys. 58, 125–141.
Biggs, L., Juravle, G. and Spence, C. (2016). Haptic exploration of plateware alters the perceived texture and taste of food, Food Qual. Pref. 50, 129–134.
Burzynska, J. (2018). Assessing Oenosthesia: blending wine and sound, Int. J. Food Des. 3, 83–101.
Chen, K., Zhou, B., Chen, S., He, S. and Zhou, W. (2013). Olfaction spontaneously highlights visual saliency map, Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 280, 20131729. DOI:10.1098/rspb.2013.1729.
Chiou, R. and Rich, A. N. (2012). Cross-modality correspondence between pitch and spatial location modulates attentional orienting, Perception 41, 339–353.
Crisinel, A.-S., Cosser, S., King, S., Jones, R., Petrie, J. and Spence, C. (2012). A bittersweet symphony: systematically modulating the taste of food by changing the sonic properties of the soundtrack playing in the background, Food Qual. Pref. 24, 201–204.
Foote, J. (2000). Automatic audio segmentation using a measure of audio novelty, in: Proc. IEEE Int. Conf. Multimed. Expo, pp. 452–455.
Galmarini, M. V., Dufau, L., Loiseau, A.-L., Visalli, M. and Schlich, P. (2018). Wine and cheese: two products or one association? A new method for assessing wine-cheese pairing, Beverages 4, 13. DOI:10.3390/beverages4010013.
Goldstein, D. G. and Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: the recognition heuristic, Psychol. Rev. 109, 75–90.
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Kang, O. E. and Leong, Y. C. (2012). On musical dissonance, Music Percept. 30, 19–35.
Kantono, K., Hamid, N., Shepherd, D., Yoo, M. J. Y., Grazioli, G. and Carr, B. T. (2016). Listening to music can influence the hedonic and sensory perceptions of gelati, Appetite 100, 244–255.
Kantono, K., Hamid, N., Shepherd, D., Lin, Y. H. T., Brard, C., Grazioli, G. and Carr, B. T. (2018). The effect of music on gelato perception in different eating contexts, Food Res. Int. 113, 43–56.
Kantono, K., Hamid, N., Shepherd, D., Lin, Y. H. T., Skiredj, S. and Carr, B. T. (2019). Emotional and electrophysiological measures correlate to flavour perception in the presence of music, Physiol. Behav. 199, 154–164.
Klapetek, A., Ngo, M. K. and Spence, C. (2012). Does crossmodal correspondence modulate the facilitatory effect of auditory cues on visual search? Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 74, 1154–1167.
Knoeferle, K. M., Woods, A., Käppler, F. and Spence, C. (2015). That sounds sweet: using cross-modal correspondences to communicate gustatory attributes, Psychol. Market. 32, 107–120.
Knöferle, K. and Spence, C. (2012). Crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes, Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 992–1006.
Lartillot, O. and Toiviainen, P. (2007). A Matlab toolbox for musical feature extraction from audio, in: Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Digital Audio Effects, pp. 237–244. Bordeaux, France.
Mantonakis, A., Rodero, P., Lesschaeve, I. and Hastie, R. (2009). Order in choice: effects of serial position on preferences, Psychol. Sci. 20, 1309–1312.
Marks, L. E. and Wheeler, M. E. (1998). Attention and the detectability of weak taste stimuli, Chem. Sens. 23, 19–29.
Meillon, S., Urbano, C. and Schlich, P. (2009). Contribution of the Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS) method to the sensory description of subtle differences in partially dealcoholized red wines, Food Qual. Pref. 20, 490–499.
Mesz, B., Trevisan, M. A. and Sigman, M. (2011). The taste of music, Perception 40, 209–219.
Mossbridge, J. A., Grabowecky, M. and Suzuki, S. (2011). Changes in auditory frequency guide visual–spatial attention, Cognition 121, 133–139.
Müller, M. (2015). Fundamentals of Music Processing: Audio, Analysis, Algorithms, Applications. Springer International Publishing, Cham, Switzerland.
Ng, M., Lawlor, J. B., Chandra, S., Chaya, C., Hewson, L. and Hort, J. (2012). Using quantitative descriptive analysis and temporal dominance of sensations analysis as complementary methods for profiling commercial blackcurrant squashes, Food Qual. Pref. 25, 121–134.
Nunes, C. A. and Pinheiro, A. C. M. (2015). Sensomaker User Guide. http://ufla.br/sensomaker/. Retrieved 6 October 2016.
Oberfeld, D., Hecht, H., Allendorf, U. and Wickelmaier, F. (2009). Ambient lighting modifies the flavor of wine, J. Sens. Stud. 24, 797–832.
Parrott, S., Guzman-Martinez, E., Ortega, L., Grabowecky, M., Huntington, M. D. and Suzuki, S. (2015). Direction of auditory pitch-change influences visual search for slope from graphs, Perception 44, 764–778.
Pineau, N., Schlich, P., Cordelle, S., Mathonnière, C., Issanchou, S., Imbert, A., Rogeaux, M., Etiévant, P. and Köster, E. (2009). Temporal Dominance of Sensations: construction of the TDS curves and comparison with time–intensity, Food Qual. Pref. 20, 450–455.
Pinheiro, A. C. M., Nunes, C. A. and Vietoris, V. (2013). Sensomaker: a tool for sensorial characterization of food products, Cienc. Agrotecnol. 37, 199–201.
Rabin, M. D. and Cain, W. S. (1989). Attention and learning in the perception of odor mixtures, in: Perception of Complex Smells and Tastes, D. G. Laing, W. S. Cain, R. L. McBride and B. W. Ache (Eds), pp. 173–188. Academic Press, Sydney, Australia.
Reinoso Carvalho, F., Van Ee, R., Rychtarikova, M., Touhafi, A., Steenhaut, K., Persoone, D., Spence, C. and Leman, M. (2015). Does music influence the multisensory tasting experience? J. Sens. Stud. 30, 404–412.
Reinoso Carvalho, F., Wang, Q. (J.), Van Ee, R., Persoone, D. and Spence, C. (2017). “Smooth operator”: music modulates the perceived creaminess, sweetness, and bitterness of chocolate, Appetite 108, 383–390.
Simner, J., Cuskley, C. and Kirby, S. (2010). What sound does that taste? Cross-modal mappings across gustation and audition, Perception 39, 553–569.
Sokolowsky, M. and Fischer, U. (2012). Evaluation of bitterness in white wine applying descriptive analysis, time-intensity analysis, and temporal dominance of sensations analysis, Anal. Chim. Acta 732, 46–52.
Spence, C. (2016). Oral referral: on the mislocalization of odours to the mouth, Food Qual. Pref. 50, 117–128.
Spence, C. and Wang, Q. J. (2015). Wine & music (II): can you taste the music? Modulating the experience of wine through music and sound, Flavour 4, 33. DOI:10.1186/s13411-015-0043-z.
Spence, C., Ketenmann, B., Kobal, G. and Mcglone, F. P. (2000). Selective attention to the chemosensory modality, Percept. Psychophys. 62, 1265–1271.
Spence, C., Richards, L., Kjellin, E., Huhnt, A.-M., Daskal, V., Scheybeler, A., Velasco, C. and Deroy, O. (2013). Looking for crossmodal correspondences between classical music and fine wine, Flavour 2, 29. DOI:10.1186/2044-7248-2-29.
Spence, C., Velasco, C. and Knoeferle, K. (2014). A large sample study on the influence of the multisensory environment on the wine drinking experience, Flavour 3, 8. DOI:10.1186/2044-7248-3-8.
Spence, C., Reinoso Carvalho, F., Velasco, C. and Wang, Q. J. (2019). Extrinsic auditory contributions to food perception & consumer behaviour: an interdisciplinary review, Multisens. Res. 32, 275–318. DOI:10.1163/22134808-20191403.
Stafford, L. D., Fernandes, M. and Agobiani, E. (2012). Effects of noise and distraction on alcohol perception, Food Qual. Pref. 24, 218–224.
Stafford, L. D., Agobiani, E. and Fernandes, M. (2013). Perception of alcohol strength impaired by low and high volume distraction, Food Qual. Pref. 28, 470–474.
Stevenson, R. J. (2012). The role of attention in flavour perception, Flavour 1, 2. DOI:10.1186/2044-7248-1-2.
Tempere, S., Hamtat, M.-L., de Revel, G. and Sicard, G. (2016). Comparison of the ability of wine experts and novices to identify odorant signals: a new insight in wine expertise, Aust. J. Grape Wine Res. 22, 190–196.
van Ee, R., van Boxtel, J. J. A., Parker, A. L. and Alais, D. (2009). Multisensory congruency as a mechanism for attentional control over perceptual selection, J. Neurosci. 29, 11641–11649.
Velasco, C., Jones, R., King, S. and Spence, C. (2013). Assessing the influence of the multisensory environment on the whisky drinking experience, Flavour 2, 23. DOI:10.1186/2044-7248-2-23.
Visalli, M. (2016). Results of the Analysis of the Wine-Cheese Data Collected During the Social Program, Presentation Given at EuroSense 2016. Dijon, France.
von Békésy, G. (1963). Hearing theories and complex sounds, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 35, 588–601.
Wang, Q. (J.) and Spence, C. (2015). Assessing the effect of musical congruency on wine tasting in a live performance setting, i-Perception 6. DOI:10.1177/2041669515593027.
Wang, Q. (J.) and Spence, C. (2016). ‘Striking a sour note’: assessing the influence of consonant and dissonant music on taste perception, Multisens. Res. 29, 195–208.
Wang, Q. J. and Spence, C. (2018a). A smooth wine? Haptic influences on wine evaluation, Int. J. Gastron. Food Sci. 14, 9–13.
Wang, Q. (J.) and Spence, C. (2018b). Assessing the influence of music on wine perception amongst wine professionals, Food Sci. Nutr. 52, 211–217.
Wang, Q. (J.) and Spence, C. (2018c). ‘A sweet smile’: more than merely a metaphor? Cogn. Emot. 32, 1052–1061.
Wang, Q. (J.), Woods, A. and Spence, C. (2015). “What’s your taste in music?” a comparison of the effectiveness of various soundscapes in evoking specific tastes, i-Perception 6. DOI:10.1177/2041669515622001.
Wolpert, R. S. (1990). Recognition of melody, harmonic accompaniment, and instrumentation: musicians vs. nonmusicians, Music Percept. 8, 95–105.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 2096 | 282 | 29 |
Full Text Views | 93 | 8 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 119 | 17 | 0 |
Several studies have examined how music may affect the evaluation of food and drink, but the vast majority have not observed how this interaction unfolds in time. This seems to be quite relevant, since both music and the consumer experience of food/drink are time-varying in nature. In the present study we sought to fix this gap, using Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS), a method developed to record the dominant sensory attribute at any given moment in time, to examine the impact of music on the wine taster’s perception. More specifically, we assessed how the same red wine might be experienced differently when tasters were exposed to various sonic environments (two pieces of music plus a silent control condition). The results revealed diverse patterns of dominant flavours for each sound condition, with significant differences in flavour dominance in each music condition as compared to the silent control condition. Moreover, musical correspondence analysis revealed that differences in perceived dominance of acidity and bitterness in the wine were correlated in the temporality of the experience, with changes in basic auditory attributes. Potential implications for the role of attention in auditory flavour modification and opportunities for future studies are discussed.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 2096 | 282 | 29 |
Full Text Views | 93 | 8 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 119 | 17 | 0 |