This study tests the hypothesis that patients with bilateral glaucoma exhibit different eye movements compared to normally-sighted people when viewing computer displayed photographs of everyday scenes. Thirty glaucomatous patients and 30 age-related controls with normal vision viewed images on a computer monitor whilst eye movements were simultaneously recorded using an eye tracking system. The patients demonstrated a significant reduction in the average number of saccades compared to controls (; mean reduction of 7% (95% confidence interval (CI): 3–11%)). There was no difference in average saccade amplitude between groups but there was between-person variability in patients. The average elliptical region scanned by the patients by a bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) analysis, was more restricted compared to controls (; mean reduction of 23% (95% (CI): 11–35%)). A novel analysis mapping areas of interest in the images indicated a weak association between severity of functional deficit and a tendency to not view regions typically viewed by the controls. In conclusion, some eye movements in some patients with bilateral glaucomatous defects differ from normal-sighted people of a similar age when viewing images of everyday scenes, providing evidence for a potential new window for looking into the functional consequences of the disease.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Acik A., Onat S., Schumann F., Einhauser W., Konig P. (2009). Effects of luminance contrast and its modifications on fixation behavior during free viewing of images from different categories, Vision Research 49, 1541–1553.
Andrews T. J., Coppola D. M. (1999). Idiosyncratic characteristics of saccadic eye movements when viewing different visual environments, Vision Research 39, 2947–2953.
Antes J. R. (1974). The time course of picture viewing, J. Exper. Psychol. 103, 62–70.
Bellmann C., Feely M., Crossland M. D., Kabanarou S. A., Rubin G. S. (2004). Fixation stability using central and pericentral fixation targets in patients with age-related macular degeneration, Ophthalmology 111, 2265–2270.
Bertera J., Rayner K. (2000). Eye movements and the span of the effective stimulus in visual search, Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 62, 576–585.
Brandt S. A., Stark L. W. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene, J. Cognit. Neurosci. 9, 27–38.
Bullimore M. A., Bailey I. L. (1995). Reading and eye movements in age-related maculopathy, Optomet. Vision Sci. 72, 125–138.
Coeckelbergh T. R., Cornelissen F. W., Brouwer W. H., Kooijman A. C. (2002). The effect of visual field defects on eye movements and practical fitness to drive, Vision Research 42, 669–677.
Crabb D. P., Viswanathan A. C. (2005). Integrated visual fields: a new approach to measuring the binocular field of view and visual disability, Graefes Arch. Clin. Exper. Ophthalmol. 243, 210–216.
Crabb D. P., Fitzke F. W., Hitchings R. A., Viswanathan A. C. (2004). A practical approach to measuring the visual field component of fitness to drive, Brit. J. Ophthalmol. 88, 1191–1196.
Crabb D. P., Smith N. D., Rauscher F. G., Chisholm C. M., Barbur J. L., Edgar D. F., Garway-Heath D. F. (2010). Exploring eye movements in patients with glaucoma when viewing a driving scene, PLoS One 5, e9710.
Crossland M. D., Rubin G. S. (2002). The use of an infrared eyetracker to measure fixation stability, Optomet. Vis. Sci. 79, 735–739.
Einhauser W., Konig P. (2003). Does luminance-contrast contribute to a saliency map for overt visual attention? Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1089–1097.
Foulsham T., Teszka R., Kingstone A. (2011). Saccade control in natural images is shaped by the information visible at fixation: evidence from asymmetric gaze-contingent windows, Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 266–283.
Frey H.-P., Honey C., König P. (2008). What’s color got to do with it? The influence of color on visual attention in different categories, J. Vision 8 (on-line).
Goldstein R. B., Woods R. L., Peli E. (2007). Where people look when watching movies: Do all viewers look at the same place? Comput. Biol. Med. 37, 957–964.
González E., Teichman J., Lillakas L., Markowitz S., Steinbach M. (2006). Fixation stability using radial gratings in patients with age-related macular degeneration, Can. J. Ophthalmol. 41, 333–339.
Green J., Siddall H., Murdoch I. (2002). Learning to live with glaucoma: a qualitative study of diagnosis and the impact of sight loss, Social Sci. Med. 55, 257–267.
Greene H. H., Rayner K. (2001). Eye-movement control in direction-coded visual search, Perception 30, 147–157.
Hall E. C., Cluffreda K. J. (2001). Eccentric viewing training in macular degeneration using auditory ocular motor biofeedback, J. Behav. Optomet. 12, 87–93.
Harding G., Bloj M. (2010). Real and predicted influence of image manipulations on eye movements during scene recognition, J. Vision 10, 1–17.
Hartmann C. W., Rhee D. J. (2006). The patient’s journey: glaucoma, Brit. Med. J. 333, 738–739.
Hawkins A. S., Szlyk J. P., Ardickas Z., Alexander K. R., Wilensky J. T. (2003). Comparison of contrast sensitivity, visual acuity and Humphrey visual field testing in patients with glaucoma, J. Glaucoma 12, 134–138.
Haymes S. A., Leblanc R. P., Nicolela M. T., Chiasson L. A., Chauhan B. C. (2007). Risk of falls and motor vehicle collisions in glaucoma, Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 48, 1149–1155.
Haymes S. A., Leblanc R. P., Nicolela M. T., Chiasson L. A., Chauhan B. C. (2008). Glaucoma and on-road driving performance, Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 49, 3035–3041.
Henderson J. M. (2003). Human gaze control during real-world scene perception, Trends Cognit. Sci. 7, 498–504.
Irwin D. E. (1991). Information integration across saccadic eye movements, Cognit. Psychol. 23, 420–456.
Itti L., Koch C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention, Vision Research 40, 1489–1506.
Jampel H. D., Schwartz A., Pollack I., Abrams D., Weiss H., Miller R. (2002). Glaucoma patients’ assessment of their visual function and quality of life, J. Glaucoma 11, 154–163.
Kanonidou E., Proudlock F. A., Gottlob I. (2010). Reading strategies in mild to moderate strabismic amblyopia: an eye movement investigation, Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 51, 3502–3508.
Komatsu H. (2006). The neural mechanisms of perceptual filling-in, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 220–231.
Kotecha A., O’leary N., Melmoth D., Grant S., Crabb D. P. (2009). The functional consequences of glaucoma for eye-hand coordination, Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 50, 203–213.
Krieger G., Rentschler I., Hauske G., Schill K., Zetzsche C. (2000). Object and scene analysis by saccadic eye-movements: an investigation with higher-order statistics, Spatial Vision 13, 201–214.
Mackworth N. H., Morandi A. J. (1967). The gaze selects informative details within pictures, Percept. Psychophys. 2, 547–552.
Matin E. (1974). Saccadic suppression: a review and an analysis, Psychol. Bull. 81, 899–917.
Nelson P., Aspinall P., O’Brien C. (1999). Patients’ perception of visual impairment in glaucoma: a pilot study, Brit. J. Ophthalmol. 83, 546–552.
Quigley H. A., Broman A. T. (2006). The number of people with glaucoma worldwide in 2010 and 2020, Brit. J. Ophthalmol. 90, 262–267.
Ramulu P. (2009). Glaucoma and disability: which tasks are affected, and at what stage of disease? Curr. Opin. Ophthalmol. 20, 92–98.
Ramulu P. Y., West S. K., Munoz B., Jampel H. D., Friedman D. S. (2009). Glaucoma and reading speed: the Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project, Arch. Ophthalmol. 127, 82–87.
Rayner K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception and visual search, Qtly J. Exper. Psychol. 62, 1457–1506.
Reinagel P., Zador A. M. (1999). Natural scene statistics at the centre of gaze, Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 10, 341–350.
Safran A. B., Landis T. (1996). Plasticity in the adult visual cortex: implications for the diagnosis of visual field defects and visual rehabilitation, Curr. Opin. Ophthalmol. 7, 53–64.
Saida S., Ikeda M. (1979). Useful visual field size for pattern perception, Percept. Psychophys. 25, 119–125.
Seiple W., Szlyk J. P., Mcmahon T., Pulido J., Fishman G. A. (2005). Eye-movement training for reading in patients with age-related macular degeneration, Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 46, 2886–2896.
Shaw M. E. (2005). Increasing compliance with glaucoma therapy: “so, convince me I have something wrong with my eyes”, Insight 30, 7–9.
Smith N. D., Crabb D. P., Garway-Heath D. F. (2011). An exploratory study of visual search performance in glaucoma, Ophthal. Physl. Optom. 31, 225–232.
Stamper R. L. (1989). Psychophysical changes in glaucoma, Surv. Ophthalmol., 33 Suppl., 309–318.
Szlyk J. P., Mahler C. L., Seiple W., Edward D. P., Wilensky J. T. (2005). Driving performance of glaucoma patients correlates with peripheral visual field loss, J. Glaucoma 14, 145–150.
Tatler B. W., Wade N. J., Kaulard K. (2007). Examining art: dissociating pattern and perceptual influences on oculomotor behaviour, Spatial Vision 21, 165–184.
Turano K. A., Geruschat D. R., Baker F. H., Stahl J. W., Shapiro M. D. (2001). Direction of gaze while walking a simple route: persons with normal vision and persons with retinitis pigmentosa, Optomet. Vis. Sci. 78, 667–675.
Viswanathan A. C., Mcnaught A. I., Poinoosawmy D., Fontana L., Crabb D. P., Fitzke F. W., Hitchings R. A. (1999). Severity and stability of glaucoma: patient perception compared with objective measurement, Arch. Ophthalmol. 117, 450–454.
Vivekananda-Schmidt P., Anderson R. S., Reinhardt-Rutland A. H., Shields T. J. (2004). Simulated impairment of contrast sensitivity: performance and gaze behavior during locomotion through a built environment, Optomet. Vis. Sci. 81, 844–852.
Vlaskamp B. N., Hooge I. T. (2006). Crowding degrades saccadic search performance, Vision Research 46, 417–425.
Vukicevic M., Fitzmaurice K. (2009). Eccentric viewing training in the home environment: can it improve the performance of activities of daily living? J. Vis. Impairment Blindness 103, 277–290.
White S. C., Atchison K. A., Gornbein J. A., Nattiv A., Paganini-Hill A., Service S. K. (2006). Risk factors for fractures in older men and women: The Leisure World Cohort Study, Gend. Med. 3, 110–123.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 785 | 111 | 16 |
Full Text Views | 207 | 20 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 48 | 15 | 0 |
This study tests the hypothesis that patients with bilateral glaucoma exhibit different eye movements compared to normally-sighted people when viewing computer displayed photographs of everyday scenes. Thirty glaucomatous patients and 30 age-related controls with normal vision viewed images on a computer monitor whilst eye movements were simultaneously recorded using an eye tracking system. The patients demonstrated a significant reduction in the average number of saccades compared to controls (; mean reduction of 7% (95% confidence interval (CI): 3–11%)). There was no difference in average saccade amplitude between groups but there was between-person variability in patients. The average elliptical region scanned by the patients by a bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) analysis, was more restricted compared to controls (; mean reduction of 23% (95% (CI): 11–35%)). A novel analysis mapping areas of interest in the images indicated a weak association between severity of functional deficit and a tendency to not view regions typically viewed by the controls. In conclusion, some eye movements in some patients with bilateral glaucomatous defects differ from normal-sighted people of a similar age when viewing images of everyday scenes, providing evidence for a potential new window for looking into the functional consequences of the disease.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 785 | 111 | 16 |
Full Text Views | 207 | 20 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 48 | 15 | 0 |