The present study examines whether time perspectives (i.e., Past Negative, Past Positive, Present Hedonistic, Present Fatalistic and Future), Deviation from the Balanced Time Perspective (DBTP), and personality traits (i.e., extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience) are good candidates to explain subjective well-being and psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Using an online survey, we recruited 1483 participants during the Italian mandatory lockdown. Multiple regressions analysis showed that time perspectives strongly predicted subjective well-being, depression, and stress. Participants highly oriented on Past Positive and Future had higher subjective well-being, while participants highly oriented on Past Negative and Present Fatalistic had lower subjective well-being. Regarding DBTP, participants who greatly deviated from the optimal temporal balance reported lower well-being and were more distressed. Regarding the impact of personality traits, extraversion and neuroticism were the factors that better explained subjective well-being, level of stress, and depression, whereas higher levels of openness were associated with lower levels of depression. Finally, DBTP was a significant moderator in the relationships of extraversion and neuroticism with subjective well-being. Implications for time-perspective theory and practical contributions of the study are discussed.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Antaramian, S. (2015). Assessing psychological symptoms and well-being: application of a dual-factor mental health model to understand college student performance. J. Psychoeduc. Assess., 33, 419–429. doi: 10.1177/0734282914557727.
Anglim, J., & Horwood, S. (2021). Effect of the COVID-19 pandemic and big five personality on subjective and psychological well-being. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci., 12, 1527–1537. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620983047.
Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in family practice: a rapid technic. Postgrad. Med.,52, 81–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/00325481.1972.11713319.
Biviá-Roig, G., La Rosa, V. L., Gómez-Tébar, M., Serrano-Raya, L., Amer-Cuenca, J. J., Caruso, S., Commodari, E., Barraswa-Shaw, A., & Lisón, J. F. (2020). Analysis of the impact of the confinement resulting from COVID-19 on the lifestyle and psychological wellbeing of Spanish pregnant women: an internet-based cross-sectional survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health,17, 5933. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165933.
Boniwell, I. & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Balancing time perspective in pursuit of optimal functioning. In P. A., Linley & S., Joseph (Eds) Positive Psychology in Practice (pp.165–178). Hoboken, NJ, USA: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939338.ch10.
Boniwell, I., Osin, E., Linley, A. P. and Ivanchenko, G. V. (2010). A question of balance: Time perspective and well-being in British and Russian samples. J. Pos. Psychol., 5, 24–40. doi: 10.1080/17439760903271181.
Boyd, J. N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2005). Time perspective, health, and risk-taking. In J. N., Boyd & P. G., Zimbardo, Understanding Behavior in the Context of Time (pp. 97–119). London, UK: Psychology Press.
Bruno, G., Panzeri, A., Granziol, U., Alivernini, F., Chirico, A., Galli, F., Lucidi, F., Spoto, A., Vidotto, G., & Bertamini, M. (2021). The Italian COVID-19 psychological research consortium (IT C19PRC): general overview and replication of the UK study. J. Clin. Med.,10, 52. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10010052.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am. Psychol., 54, 165–181. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.3.165.
Castelli, L., Di Tella, M., Benfante, A., & Romeo, A. (2020). The spread of COVID-19 in the Italian population: anxiety, depression, and post-traumatic stress symptoms. Can. J. Psychiatry, 65, 731–732. https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743720938598.
Ceccato, I., Palumbo, R., Di Crosta, A., Marchetti, D., La Malva, P., Maiella, R., Marin, A., Mammarella, N., Verrocchio, M. C., & Di Domenico, A. (2021). “What’s next?” Individual differences in expected repercussions of the COVID-19 pandemic. Pers. Indiv. Differ.,174, 110674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110674.
Cellini, N., Canale, N., Mioni, G., & Costa, S. (2020). Changes in sleep pattern, sense of time and digital media use during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. J. Sleep Res., 29, e13074. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13074.
Chapman, G. B., Brewer, N. T., Coups, E. J., Brownlee, S., Leventhal, H., & Leventhal, E. A. (2001). Value for the future and preventive health behavior. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl., 7, 235–250. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.7.3.235.
Chiesi, F., Vizza, D., Valente, M., Bruno, R., Lau, C., Campagna, M. R., Lo Iacono, M., & Bruno, F. (2022). Positive personal resources and psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic: resilience, optimism, hope, courage, trait mindfulness, and self-efficacy in breast cancer patients and survivors. Support. Care Cancer, 2022, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00520-022-07123-1.
Chiorri, C., Bracco, F., Piccinno, T., Modafferi, C., & Battini, V. (2015). Psychometric properties of a revised version of the Ten Item Personality Inventory.Eur. J. Psychol. Assess., 31, 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015–5759/a000215.
Cini, C., Bortot, G., Sforza, S., Mantovani, A., Landi, L., Esposito, C., Escolino, M., & Masieri, L. (2020). Paediatric urology practice during COVID-19 pandemic. J. Pediatr. Urol., 16, 295–296. doi: 10.1016/j.jpurol.2020.04.023.
Commodari, E., & La Rosa, V. L. (2020). Adolescents in quarantine during COVID-19 pandemic in Italy: perceived health risk, beliefs, psychological experiences and expectations for the future. Front. Psychol.,11, 559951. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.559951.
Commodari, E., La Rosa, V. L., Carnemolla, G., & Parisi, J. (2021). The psychological impact of the lockdown on Italian university students during the first wave of COVID-19 pandemic: psychological experiences, health risk perceptions, distance learning, and future perspectives. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol.,9, 2 https://doi.org/10.13129/2282-1619/mjcp-3009.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull., 95, 542–575.
Diener, E. (1999). Introduction to the special section on the structure of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 76, 803–804. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.5.803.
Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 68, 653–663. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.68.4.653.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess.,49, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.
Diener, E., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (1997). Recent findings on subjective well-being. Indian J. Clin. Psychol., 24, 25–41.
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 54, 403–425. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145056.
Diener, E., Pressman, S. D., Hunter, J., & Delgadillo-Chase, D. (2017). If, why, and when subjective well-being influences health, and future needed research. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being, 9, 133–167. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12090.
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018). Advances in subjective well-being research. Nat. Hum. Behav., 2, 253–260. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6.
Di Giorgio, E., Di Riso, D., Mioni, G., & Cellini, N. (2020). The interplay between mothers’ and children behavioral and psychological factors during COVID-19: an Italian study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 30, 1401–1412. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01631-3.
Ding, Y., Xu, J., Huang, S., Li, P., Lu, C., & Xie, S. (2020). Risk perception and depression in public health crises: evidence from the COVID-19 crisis in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health,17, 5728. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165728.
Drake, L., Duncan, E., Sutherland, F., Abernethy, C., & Henry, C. (2008). Time perspective and correlates of wellbeing. Time Soc.,17, 47–61. https://doi.org/10.1177/0961463X07086304.
Epel, E. S., Bandura, A. and Zimbardo, P. G. (1999) Escaping homelessness: the influences of the self-efficacy and time perspective on coping with homelessness. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol., 29, 575–596. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559–1816.1999.tb01402.x.
Eryilmaz, A., & Ercan, L. (2011). Investigating of the subjective well-being based on gender, age and personality traits. Turk Psikol. Danışma Rehberlik Derg., 4, 139–149.
Eysenbach, G. (2004). Improving the quality of web surveys: the Checklist for Reporting Results of Internet E-Surveys (CHERRIES). J. Med. Internet Res., 6(3), e34. doi: 10.2196/jmir.6.3.e34.
Freeston, M. H., Mawn, L., Tiplady, A., Bottesi, G., & Thwaites, S. (2020). Towards a model of uncertainty distress in the context of Coronavirus (COVID-19). Cogn. Behav. Therap., 13, e31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1754470X2000029X.
Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Personal., 37, 504–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1.
Greenspoon, P. J., & Saklofske, D. H. (2001). Toward an integration of subjective well-being and psychopathology. Soc. Indic. Res., 54, 81–108. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007219227883.
Grondin, S., Mendoza-Duran, E., & Rioux, P.-A. (2020). Pandemic, quarantine, and psychological time. Front. Psychol., 11, 581036. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.581036.
Gutiérrez, J. L. G., Jiménez, B. M., Hernández, E. G., & Puente, C. P. (2005). Personality and subjective well-being: big five correlates and demographic variables. Pers. Indiv. Differ.,38, 1561–1569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.015.
Hall, P. A., & Fong, G. T. (2003). The effects of a brief time perspective intervention for increasing physical activity among young adults. Psychol. Health, 18, 685–706. https://doi.org/10.1080/0887044031000110447.
Hamilton, J. M., Kives, K. D., Micevski, V. and Grace, S. L. (2003). Time perspective and health-promoting behaviour in a cardiac rehabilitation population. Behav.Med., 28, 132–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/08964280309596051.
Higuchi, S., Mihara, S., Kitayuguchi, T., Miyakoshi, H., Ooi, M., Maezono, M., Nishimura, K., & Matsuzaki, T. (2020). Prolonged use of Internet and gaming among treatment seekers arising out of social restrictions related to COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci., 74, 607–608. doi: 10.1111/pcn.13127.
Holman, E. A., & Grisham, E. L. (2020). When time falls apart: The public health implications of distorted time perception in the age of COVID-19. Psychol. Trauma,12(S1), S63–S65. doi: 10.1037/tra0000756.
Italian National Institute of Health (n.d.). https://www.iss.it/.
Jamovi Project (2021). Jamovi(Version 1.6) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org.
Jankowski, K. S., Zajenkowski, M., & Stolarski, M. (2020). What are the optimal levels of time perspectives? Deviation from the balanced time perspective-revisited (DBTP-r). Psychol. Belg.,60, 164–183. https://dx.doi.org/10.5334%2Fpb.487.
Johnson, S. U., Ebrahimi, O. V., & Hoffart, A. (2020). PTSD symptoms among health workers and public service providers during the COVID-19 outbreak. PloS ONE,15, e0241032. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241032.
Jovančević, A., & Milićević, N. (2020). Optimism-pessimism, conspiracy theories and general trust as factors contributing to COVID-19 related behavior – A cross-cultural study. Pers. Indiv. Differ., 167, 110216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110216.
Kooij, D. T. A. M., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., & Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol., 103, 867–893. doi: 10.1037/apl0000306.
Korff, J., & Biemann, T. (2020). Adjusting the lookout: Subjective health, loneliness, and life satisfaction predict future time perspective. Psychol. Aging, 35, 1170–1183. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000513.
Kwong, A. S. F., Pearson, R. M., Adams, M. J., Northstone, K., Tilling, K., Smith, D., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Bould, /h., Warne, N., Zammit, S., Gunnell, D. J., Moran, P. A., Micali, N., Reichenberg, A., Hickman, M., Rai, D., Haworth, S., Campbell, A., Altschul, D., Flaig, R., McIntosh, A. M., Lawlor, D. A., Porteous, D., & Timpson, N. J. (2021). Mental health before and during the COVID-19 pandemic in two longitudinal UK population cohorts. B. J. Psychiatry,218, 334–343. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2020.242.
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychol. Aging, 17, 125–139. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.125.
La Rosa, V. L., Gori, A., Faraci, P., Vicario, C. M., & Craparo, G. (2021). Traumatic Distress, Alexithymia, Dissociation, and Risk of Addiction During the First Wave of COVID-19 in Italy: results from a cross-sectional online survey on a non-clinical adult sample. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict., 2021, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s11469-021-00569-0.
Loose, T., Wittmann, M., & Vásquez-Echeverría, A. (2021). Disrupting times in the wake of the pandemic: Dispositional time attitudes, time perception and temporal focus. Time Soc., 31, 110–131. https://doi.org/10.1177/0961463X211027420.
Lucas, R. E., & Diener, E. (2009). Personality and subjective well-being. In E., Diener (Ed.), The Science of Well-Being: The Collected Works of Ed Diener (pp. 75–102). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
Marko, K. W., & Savickas, M. L. (1998). Effectiveness of a career time perspective intervention. J. Vocat. Behav., 52, 106–119. https://doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.1996.1566.
Mazza, C., Ricci, E., Biondi, S., Colasanti, M., Ferracuti, S., Napoli, C., & Roma, P. (2020). A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Italian people during the COVID-19 pandemic: immediate psychological responses and associated factors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health, 17, 3165. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17093165.
Mioni, G., Wittmann, M., Prunetti, E., & Stablum, F. (2020). Time perspective and the subjective passage of time in patients with borderline personality disorders. Timing Time Percept., 8, 86–101. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-20191165.
Mondo, M., Sechi, C., & Cabras, C. (2019). Psychometric evaluation of three versions of the Italian Perceived Stress Scale. Curr. Psychol., 40, 1884–1892. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-0132-8.
Morselli, D. (2013). The olive tree effect: Future time perspective when the future is uncertain. Cult. Psychol.,19, 305–322. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X13489319.
Nikčević, A. V., Marino, C., Kolubinski, D. C., Leach, D., & Spada, M. M. (2021). Modelling the contribution of the Big Five personality traits, health anxiety, and COVID-19 psychological distress to generalised anxiety and depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Affect. Disord.,279, 578–584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.053.
Nowakowska, I. (2020). Lonely and thinking about the past: the role of time perspectives, Big Five traits and perceived social support in loneliness of young adults during COVID-19 social distancing. Curr. Issues Pers. Psychol.,8, 175–184. https://doi.org/10.5114/cipp.2020.97289.
Paredes, M. R., Apaolaza, V., Fernandez-Robin, C., Hartmann, P., & Yañez-Martinez, D. (2021). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on subjective mental well-being: The interplay of perceived threat, future anxiety and resilience. Pers. Indiv. Differ.,170, 110455. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110455.
Pfefferbaum, B., & North, C. S. (2020). Mental health and the Covid-19 pandemic. N. Engl. J. Med.,383, 510–512. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp2008017.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol.,88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.
Przepiorka, A., & Sobol-Kwapinska, M. (2021). People with positive time perspective are more grateful and happier: gratitude mediates the relationship between time perspective and life satisfaction. J. Happiness Stud.,22, 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00221-z.
Rodriguez, L. M., Litt, D. M., & Stewart, S. H. (2020). Drinking to cope with the pandemic: The unique associations of COVID-19-related perceived threat and psychological distress to drinking behaviors in American men and women. Addict. Behav.,110, 106532. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106532.
Rossi, R., Socci, V., Talevi, D., Mensi, S., Niolu, C., Pacitti, F., Di Marco, A., Rossi, A., Siracusano, A., & Di Lorenzo, G. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic and lockdown measures impact on mental health among the general population in Italy. Front. Psychiatry,11, 790. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00790.
Rupprecht, F. S., Martin, K., Kamin, S. T., & Lang, F. R. (2021). COVID-19 and perceiving finitude: Associations with future time perspective, death anxiety, and ideal life expectancy. Psychol. Aging, 37, 260–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000661.
Sailer, U., Rosenberg, P., Al Nima, A., Gamble, A., Gärling, T., Archer, T., & Garcia, D. (2014). A happier and less sinister past, a more hedonistic and less fatalistic present and a more structured future: time perspective and well-being. PeerJ,2, e303. doi: 10.7717/peerj.303.
Saklofske, D. H., Kelly, I. W., & Janzen, B. L. (1995). Neuroticism, depression, and depression proneness. Pers. Indiv. Differ.,18, 27–31.
Salari, N., Hosseinian-Far, A., Jalali, R., Vaisi-Raygani, A., Rasoulpoor, S., Mohammadi, M., Rasoulpoor, S., & Khaledi-Paveh, B. (2020). Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Global. Health, 16, 57. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w.
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: integrating process models of life satisfaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 82, 582–593.
Sobol, M., Blachnio, A., & Przepiórka, A. (2020). Time of pandemic: Temporal perspectives related to compliance with public health regulations concerning the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc. Sci. Med.,265, 113408. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113408.
Stolarski, M. (2016). Not restricted by their personality: Balanced Time Perspective moderates well-established relationships between personality traits and well-being. Pers. Indiv. Differ.,100, 140–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.037.
Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., & Van Beek, W. (2015). Time Perspective Theory: Review, Research and Application. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., & Zimbardo, P. (2018). Putting time in a wider perspective: The past, the present, and the future of time perspective theory. In V., Zeigler-Hill & T. K., Shackelford (Eds), The SAGE Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences: Volume I: The Science of Personality and Individual Differences (pp. 592–628). Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526451163.n28.
Stolarski, M., Zajenkowski, M., Jankowski, K. S., & Szymaniak, K. (2020). Deviation from the balanced time perspective: A systematic review of empirical relationships with psychological variables. Pers. Indiv. Differ., 156, 109772. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109772.
Strickhouser, J. E., Zell, E., & Krizan, Z. (2017). Does personality predict health and well-being? A metasynthesis. Health Psychol., 36, 797–810. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000475.
Sun, J., Kaufman, S. B., & Smillie, L. D. (2018). Unique associations between Big Five personality aspects and multiple dimensions of well-being. J. Pers., 86, 158–172. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12301.
Swim, J., Clayton, S., Doherty, T., Gifford, R., Howard, G., Reser, J., Stem, P., & Weber, E. (2009). Psychology and Global Climate Change: Addressing a Multi-Faceted Phenomenon and Set of Challenges. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/science/about/publications/climate-change.
Sword, R. M., Sword, R. K., Brunskill, S. R., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2014). Time perspective therapy: A new time-based metaphor therapy for PTSD. J. Loss Trauma, 19, 197–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2013.763632.
Takahashi, M., Shirayama, Y., Muneoka, K., Suzuki, M., Sato, K., & Hashimoto, K. (2013). Low openness on the revised NEO personality inventory as a risk factor for treatment-resistant depression. PloS ONE,8, e71964. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071964.
Trzebiński, J., Cabański, M., & Czarnecka, J. Z. (2020). Reaction to the COVID-19 pandemic: the influence of meaning in life, life satisfaction, and assumptions on world orderliness and positivity. J. Loss Trauma, 25, 544–557. https://doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2020.1765098.
Wang, C., & Zhao, H. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 on anxiety in Chinese university students. Front. Psychol., 11, 1168. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01168.
Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., & Ho, R. C. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health, 17, 1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051729.
Weinstock, L. M., & Whisman, M. A. (2006). Neuroticism as a common feature of the depressive and anxiety disorders: a test of the revised integrative hierarchical model in a national sample. J. Abnorm. Psychol.,115, 68–74. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.68.
Zacher, H., & Rudolph, C. W. (2021). Individual differences and changes in subjective wellbeing during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol., 76, 50–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000702.
Zhang, J. W., & Howell, R. T. (2011). Do time perspectives predict unique variance in life satisfaction beyond personality traits? Pers. Individ. Dif., 50, 1261–1266. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.021.
Zhang, J. W., Howell, R. T., & Stolarski, M. (2013). Comparing three methods to measure a balanced time perspective: the relationship between a balanced time perspective and subjective well-being. J. Happiness Stud., 14, 169–184. doi: 10.1007/s10902-012-9322-x.
Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 77, 1271–1288. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-07368-2_2.
Zimbardo, P., & Boyd, J. (2008). The time paradox: The new psychology of time that will change your life. Simon and Schuster, New York, NY, USA.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 680 | 399 | 21 |
Full Text Views | 35 | 29 | 5 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 33 | 19 | 1 |
The present study examines whether time perspectives (i.e., Past Negative, Past Positive, Present Hedonistic, Present Fatalistic and Future), Deviation from the Balanced Time Perspective (DBTP), and personality traits (i.e., extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience) are good candidates to explain subjective well-being and psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Using an online survey, we recruited 1483 participants during the Italian mandatory lockdown. Multiple regressions analysis showed that time perspectives strongly predicted subjective well-being, depression, and stress. Participants highly oriented on Past Positive and Future had higher subjective well-being, while participants highly oriented on Past Negative and Present Fatalistic had lower subjective well-being. Regarding DBTP, participants who greatly deviated from the optimal temporal balance reported lower well-being and were more distressed. Regarding the impact of personality traits, extraversion and neuroticism were the factors that better explained subjective well-being, level of stress, and depression, whereas higher levels of openness were associated with lower levels of depression. Finally, DBTP was a significant moderator in the relationships of extraversion and neuroticism with subjective well-being. Implications for time-perspective theory and practical contributions of the study are discussed.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 680 | 399 | 21 |
Full Text Views | 35 | 29 | 5 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 33 | 19 | 1 |