Emotion and Time Perception in Children and Adults: The Effect of Task Difficulty

in Timing & Time Perception
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

In the present study, adults and children aged five and eight years were given a temporal bisection task involving emotional stimuli (angry and neutral faces) and three levels of discrimination difficulty that differed as a function of the ratio used between the short and the long standard duration (very easy, easy, and difficult). In addition, their cognitive capacities in terms of working memory and attention inhibition were assessed by neuropsychological tests. In the very easy temporal task (ratio of 1:4), the results showed that the psychophysical functions were shifted toward the left in all participants for the angry faces compared to the neutral faces, with a significant lowering of the Bisection Point, suggesting that the stimulus duration was judged to last longer for the emotional stimuli. In addition, the results did not show any relationship between the magnitude of this lengthening effect and individual cognitive capacities as assessed by the neuropsychological tests. The individual differences in working memory capacities only explained differences in sensitivity to time. However, when the difficulty of the temporal task increased, the children’s performance decreased and it was no longer possible to test for the emotional effect. Unlike the children, the adults were still able to discriminate time in the emotional task. However, the emotional effect was no longer observed. In conclusion, our study on temporal task difficulty shows the influence of available cognitive resources on the emergence of an emotional effect on time perception.

Emotion and Time Perception in Children and Adults: The Effect of Task Difficulty

in Timing & Time Perception

Sections

References

AdolphsR.GosselinF.BuchananT. W.TranelD.SchynsP. & DamasioA. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature4336872.

AndersonA. K. & PhelpsE. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature411305309.

AndersonA. K.ChristoffK.PanitzD.De RosaE & GabrieliJ. D. E. (2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. J. Neurosci.2356275633.

BaddeleyA. D. & HitchG. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology8485493.

Bar-HaimY.KeremA.LamyD. & ZakayD. (2010). When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cogn. Emot.24255263.

BayardS.ErkesJ. & MoroniC. (2009). Test du Stroop Victoria-Adaptation Francophone. Gignac, France: CPCN-LR.

BjorklundD. F. (Ed.). (2013). Children’s Strategies: Contemporary Views of Cognitive Development. Psychology PressNew York, NY, USA.

BradleyM. M. & LangP. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry254959.

BuhusiC. V. & OprisanS. A. (2013). Time-scale invariance as an emergent property in a perceptron with realistic, noisy neurons. Behav. Proc.956070.

ChurchR. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. Ann. NY Acad. Sci423566582.

Cocenas-SilvaR. Bueno J. L. O & Droit-VoletS. (2013). Emotion and long-term memory for duration: Resistance against interference. Behav. Proc.97610.

DirnbergerG.HesselmannG.RoisierJ. P.PremingerS.JahanshahiM. & PazR. (2012). Give it time: Neural evidence for distorted time perception and enhanced memory encoding in emotion situations. Neuroimage63591599.

Droit-VoletS. (2013a). Time perception, emotions and mood disorders. J. Physiol. Paris107255264.

Droit-VoletS. (2013b). Time perception in children: A neurodevelopmental approach. Neuropsychologia51220234.

Droit-VoletS. & GilS. (2009). The time–emotion paradox. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.364(1525) 19431953.

Droit-VoletS. & MeckW. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends Cogn. Sci.11504513.

Droit-VoletS. & ZélantiP. S. (2013). Development of time sensitivity and information processing speed. PloS One8 e71424. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071424.

Droit-VoletS.BrunotS. & NiedenthalP. (2004). Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cogn. Emot.18849858.

Droit-VoletS.MermillodM.Cocenas-SilvaR. & GilS. (2010). The effect of expectancy of a threatening event on time perception in human adults. Emotion10908914.

Droit-VoletS.FayolleS.LamotteM. & GilS. (2013). Time, emotion and the embodiment of timing. Timing Time Percept.199126.

EberhardtL. V.HuckaufA. & KlieglK. M. (2016). Effects of neutral and fearful emotional states on duration estimation of neutral and fearful face stimuli. Timing Time Percept. 43047.

EkmanP. & FriesenW. V. (1976). Pictures Of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA, USA: Consulting Psychologists.

FayolleS. L. & Droit-VoletS. (2014). Time perception and dynamics of facial expressions of emotions. PLoS One9 :e97944. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097944.

FayolleS.GilS. & Droit-Volet (2015). Fear and time: Fear speeds up the internal clock. Behav. Proc.120135140.

GableP. A. & PooleB. D. (2012). Time flies when you’re having approach-motivated fun: Effects of motivational intensity on time perception. Psychol. Sci.23879886.

GanT.WangN.ZhangZ.LiH. & LuoY. J. (2009). Emotional influences on time perception: Evidence from event-related potentials. Neuroreport20839843.

GibbonJ. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychol. Rev.84279325.

GibbonJ.ChurchR. M. & MeckW. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Ann. NY Acad. Sci4235277.

GilS. & Droit-VoletS. (2011). Time flies in the presence of angry faces, depending on the temporal task used!Acta Psychol.136354362.

GilS. & Droit-VoletS. (2012). Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cogn. Emot.26847862.

GilS.NiedenthalP. M. & Droit-VoletS. (2007). Anger and time perception in children. Emotion7219225.

GrommetE. K.Droit-VoletS.GilS.HemmesN. S.BakerA. H. & BrownB. (2010) Effects of a fear cue on time estimation in human observers. Behav. Proc.868893.

GrosA.GiroudM.BejotY.RouaudO.GuilleminS.Aboa EbouléC.ManeraV.DaumasA. & Lemesle MartinM. (2015) A time estimation task as a possible measure of emotions: Difference depending on the nature of the stimulus used. Front. Behav. Neurosci.9143. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00143.

HalbertsmaH. & Van RijnH. (2016). An evaluation of the effect of auditory emotional stimuli on interval timing. Timing Time Percept. 44862.

HolmesE. M. (2007). Event-related brain potential correlates of emotional face processingNeuropsychologia451531.

IzardC. E. (2011). Forms and functions of emotions: Matters of emotion–cognition interactions. Emot. Rev3371378.

KlieglK. M.Limbrecht-EcklundtK.DürrL.TraueH. C. & HuckaufA. (2015) The complex duration perception of emotional faces: Effects of face direction. Front. Psychol.662. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00262.

KorkmanM.KirkU. & KempS. (1998). Manual for the NEPSY. San Antonio, TX, USA: Psychological corporation.

LamartineA. (1820). Le Lac. Paris, France : Meditation Poétique.

LeDouxJ. E. (2007). The amygdala. Curr. Biol.23868874.

LiuP.YangW.YuanX.BiC.ChenA. & HuangX. (2015) Individual alerting efficiency modulates time perception. Front. Psychol. 6386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00386.

LuiM. A.PenneyT. B. & SchirmerA. (2011). Emotion effects on timing: Attention versus pacemaker accounts. PloS One6 e21829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021829.

MatellM. S. & MeckW. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. Bioessays2294103.

MatellM. S. & MeckW. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cogn. Brain Res.21139170.

NoulhianeM.MellaN.SamsonS.RagotR. & PouthasV. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion7697704.

ÖhmanA. (2007). Has evolution primed humans to “beware the beast”?Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA1041639616397.

ÖhmanA. & WiensS. (2003). On the automaticity of autonomic responses in emotion: An evolutionary perspective. In DavidsonR. J.SchererK. R. & GoldsmithH. H. (Eds) Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 256275). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

Okon-SingerH.TzelgovJ. & HenikA. (2007). Distinguishing between automaticity and attention in the processing of emotionally significant stimuli. Emotion7147157.

OprisanS. A. & BuhusiC. V. (2014). What is all the noise about in interval timing?Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.369(1637) 20120459. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0459.

PascalisO. & KellyD. J. (2009). The origins of face processing in humans: Phylogeny and ontogeny. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.4200209.

PascalisO.De Martin de VivésX.AnzuresG.QuinnP. C.SlaterA. M.TanakaJ. W. & LeeK (2011.) Development of face processing. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci.2666675.

PessoaL. (2012). Beyond brain regions: Network perspective of cognition–emotion interactions. Behav. Brain Sci.35158159.

PessoaL. (2013). The Cognitive–Emotional Brain: From Interactions to Integration. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press.

PessoaL. & AdolphsR. (2011). Emotion and the brain: Multiple roads are better than one. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.12425425.

PessoaL.McKennaM.GutierrezE. & UngerleiderL. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA991145811463.

SchirmerA. (2011). How emotions change time. Front. Integr. Neurosci.558. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00058.

SchirmerA. (2015). Emotion. Thousand Oaks CAUSA : SAGE Publications, Inc.

SchirmerA.NgT.EscoffierN. & PenneyT. B. (2016). Emotional voices slow down time: Behavioral and neural correlates. Timing Time Percept. 47998.

TipplesJ. (2008). Negative emotionality influences the effects of emotion on time perception. Emotion8127131.

TracyJ. L. & RobinsR. W. (2008). The automaticity of emotion recognition. Emotion88195.

TreismanM.CookW.NaishP. L. W. & MacCroneJ. K. (1994). The internal clock: Electroencephalography evidence for oscillatory processes underlying time perception. Q. J. Exp. Psychol-A47241289.

Van DillenL. F. & KooleS. L. (2009). How automatic is “automatic vigilance”? The role of working memory in attentional interference of negative information. Cogn. Emot.2311061117.

Van RijnH.GuB-N. & MeckW. H. (2014). Dedicated clock/timing circuit theories of time perception and time performance. In MerchantH. and de LafuenteV. (Eds) Neurobiology of Interval Timing. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology Vol. 829New-York, NY, USA: Spinger.

Van VolkinburgH. & BalsamP. (2014). Effects of emotional valence and arousal on time perception. Timing Time Perception2360378.

VuilleumierP.ArmonyJ. L.DriverJ. & DolanR. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron30829841.

VuilleumierP.ArmonyJ. L.DriverJ. & DolanR. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional ex- pressions. Nat. Neurosci.6624631.

WechslerD. (1998). Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX, USA: Psychological Corporation.

YamadaY. & KawabeT. (2011). Emotion colors time perception unconsciously. Consc. Cogn.2018351841.

ZélantiP. S. & Droit-VoletS. (2011). Cognitive abilities explaining age-related changes in time perception of short and long durations. J. Exp. Child Psychol.109143157.

Figures

  • View in gallery

    Emotional assessment of facial expressions: mean arousal ratings for the angry and neutral faces for each age group (top panel) or each temporal discrimination difficulty condition (very easy, easy, difficult; bottom panel).

  • View in gallery

    Proportion of long responses plotted against the comparison durations for the angry and the neutral faces in the different difficult discrimination conditions (ratio of 1:4, 1:2, 2:3) for the adults, the eight-year-olds and the five-year-olds.

  • View in gallery

    Proportion of long responses plotted against the comparison durations for the different difficult discrimination conditions (ratio of 1:4, 1:2, 2:3) for the adults, the eight-year-olds and the five-year-olds.

  • View in gallery

    Proportion of long responses for the angry and the neutral faces for the five-year-olds, the eight-year-olds and the adults (averaged over the seven comparison durations and the different difficult discrimination conditions).

Index Card

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 21 21 6
Full Text Views 7 7 7
PDF Downloads 2 2 2
EPUB Downloads 0 0 0