The Effect of Emotional Spoken Words on Time Perception Depends on the Gender of the Speaker

in Timing & Time Perception
Restricted Access
Get Access to Full Text
Rent on DeepDyve

Have an Access Token?



Enter your access token to activate and access content online.

Please login and go to your personal user account to enter your access token.



Help

Have Institutional Access?



Access content through your institution. Any other coaching guidance?



Connect

The aim of the present study was to investigate the influence of the emotional content of words marking brief intervals on the perceived duration of these intervals. Three independent variables were of interest: the gender of the person pronouncing the words, the gender of participants, and the valence (positive or negative) of the words in conjunction with their arousing properties. A bisection task was used and the tests, involving four different combinations of valence and arousing conditions (plus a neutral condition), were randomized within trials. The main results revealed that when the valence is negative, participants responded ‘short’ more often when words were pronounced by women rather than by men, and this effect occurred independently of the arousal condition. The results also revealed that overall, males responded ‘longer’more often than females. Finally, in the negative and low arousal condition, the Weber ratio was higher (lower sensitivity) when a male voice was used than when a female voice was used. This study shows that the gender of the person producing the stimuli whose duration is to be judged should be taken into account when analyzing the effect of emotion on time perception.

Sections

References

Angrilli A. , Cherubini P. , Pavese A. , & Manfredini S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept. Psychophys. , 59, 972982.

Arbuckle J. , & Williams B. D. (2003). Students’ perceptions of expressiveness: Age and gender effects on teacher evaluations. Sex Roles , 49, 507516.

Bertinetto P. M. , Burani C. , Laudanna A. , Marconi L. , Ratti D. , Rolando C. , & Thornton A. M. (2005). Corpus e lessico di frequenza dell’italiano scritto (CoLFIS) [Corpus and frequency lexicon for written Italian]. Retrieved from http://www.istc.cnr.it/grouppage/colfisEng.

Block R. A. , Hancock P. A. , & Zakay D. (2000). Sex differences in duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Mem. Cogn. , 28, 13331346.

Bradley M. M. , & Lang P. J. (1999). International affective digitized sounds (IADS): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings . Technical report C-1, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA.

Droit-Volet S. , Mermillod M. , Cocenas-Silva R. , & Gil S. (2010). The effect of expectancy of a threatening event on time perception in human adults. Emotion , 10, 908914.

Effron D. A. , Niedenthal P. M. , Gil S. , & Droit-Volet S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion , 6, 19.

Fallow K. M. , & Voyer D. (2013). Degree of handedness, emotion, and the perceived duration of auditory stimuli. Lateral. Asymm. Body, Brain Cogn., 18, 671692.

Gibbon J. , Church R. M. , & Meck W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. , 423, 5277.

Gil S. , & Droit-Volet S. (2011). How do emotional facial expressions influence our perception of time? In Masmoudi S. , Dai D. Y. , and Naceur A. (Eds), Attention, Representation, and Human Performance: Integration of Cognition, Emotion & Motivation (pp. 6174). New York, NY, USA: Psychology Press.

Gil S. , & Droit-Volet S. (2012). Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cogn. Emot. , 26, 847862.

Gil S. , Niedenthal P. M. , & Droit-Volet S. (2007). Anger and temporal perception in children. Emotion , 7, 219225.

Goslin J. , Galluzzi C. , & Romani C. (2013). PhonItalia: A phonological lexicon for Italian. Behav. Res. Methods, 46, 872886.

Grassi M. (2010). Sex difference in subjective duration of looming and receding sounds. Perception , 39, 14241426.

Grondin S. , Ivry R. , Franz E. , Perreault L. , & Metthé L. (1996). Markers’ influence on the duration discrimination of intermodal intervals. Percept. Psychophys. , 58, 424433.

Grondin S. , Laflamme V. & Gontier E. (2014). Effect on perceived duration and sensitivity to time when observing disgusted faces and disgusting mutilation pictures. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. , 26, 847862.

Grondin S. , Laflamme V. , Bienvenue P. , Labonté K. , & Roy M.-L. (2015). Sex effect in the temporal perception of faces expressing anger and shame. Int. J. Comp. Psychol., 28. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/8vx215m0.

Kliegl K. M. , Limbrecht-Ecklundt K. , Dürr L. , Traue H. C. , & Huckauf A. (2015). The complex duration perception of emotional faces: Effects of face direction. Front. Psychol. , 6, 262 . doi 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00262.

Lake J. I. , LaBar K. S. , Meck W. H. (2014). Hear it playing low and slow: How pitch level differentially influences time perception. Acta Psychol. , 149, 169177.

Mioni G. , Meligrana L. , Grondin S. , Perini F. , Bartolomei L. , & Stablum F. (2015). The effects of the facial expression of emotions on time perception in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. , 21, 110 .

Mondillon L. , Niedenthal P. M. , Gil S. , & Droit-Volet S. (2007). Imitation of in-group versus out-group members’ facial expressions of anger: A test with a time perception task. Soc Neurosci. , 2, 223237.

Montefinese M. , Ambrosini E. , Fairfield B. , & Mammarella N. (2014). The adaptation of the affective norms for English words (ANEW) for Italian. Behav. Res. Methods , 46, 887903.

Noulhiane M. , Mella N. , Samson S. , Ragot R. , & Poutas V. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion , 7, 697704.

Pfeuty M. , & Peretz I. (2010). Abnormal pitch-time interference in congenital amusia: Evidence from an implicit test. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. , 72, 763774.

Schirmer A. , Ng T. , Escoffier N. , & Penney T. B. (2016). Emotional voices distort time: Behavioral and neural correlates. Timing Time Percept. , 4, 7998.

Tipples J. (2010). Time flies when read taboo words. Psychonom. Bull.Rev. , 17, 563568.

Voyer D. , & Reuangrith E. (2015). Perceptual asymmetries in a time estimation task with emotional sounds. Lateral. Asymm. Body, Brain Cogn., 20, 211231.

Wackermann J. , Meissner K. , Tankersley D. , & Wittmann M. (2014). Effects of emotional valence and arousal on acoustic duration reproduction assessed via the “dual klepsydra model”. Front. Neurorobot. , 8, 11. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2014.00011.

Woodland J. & Voyer D. (2011). Context and intonation in the perception of sarcasm. Metaphor Symb , 26, 227239.

Woodrow H. (1928). Behavior with respect to short temporal stimulus forms. J. Exp. Psychol., 11, 167198.

Figures

  • Probability of responding ‘long’ (grouped data) of male participants, for male and female voice conditions, as a function of comparison intervals in each of five emotional conditions. The error bars indicate ± 1 SE.

    View in gallery
  • Probability of responding ‘long’ (grouped data) of female participants, for male and female voice conditions, as a function of comparison intervals in each of five emotional conditions. The error bars indicate ± 1 SE.

    View in gallery
  • Point of subjective equality (PSE, in ms) for male and female voice conditions in each of five emotional conditions. The error bars indicate ± 1 SE.

    View in gallery

Information

Content Metrics

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 43 43 8
Full Text Views 17 17 9
PDF Downloads 3 3 1
EPUB Downloads 7 7 5