In this study we investigated how individual differences regarding impulsivity and time perspective predict the experience of waiting during a time interval without distraction. Each participant (N = 82) filled out self-report questionnaires on impulsiveness (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale) and time perspective (Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory). Participants were individually shut into an empty room for exactly 7.5 minutes and afterwards asked to report their subjective impressions regarding the experience of time, self and affective reactions. Correlation analyses showed that being more relaxed and having a positive feeling was related to shorter duration estimates and a perceived faster passage of time. Individual traits of impulsivity, present-hedonistic and present-fatalistic time orientation were associated with relative overestimation of duration and a perceived slower passage of time; future orientation was related to an underestimation of duration and a perceived faster passage of time. Multiple regression models show that impulsivity alone predicts the variance related to time estimation of the waiting period. Multiple path analyses reveal that individuals who are more impulsive are less relaxed while waiting and accordingly overestimate duration and feel a slower passage of time. Negative affect and an impulsive present orientation are related to an overestimation of duration and the feeling of a slower passage of time while waiting.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Antonides G. , Verhoef P. C. , & Van Aalst M. (2002). Consumer perception and evaluation of waiting time: A field experiment. J. Consumer Psychol., 12, 193–202.
Baker J. , & Cameron M. (1996). The effects of the service environment on affect and consumer perception of waiting time: An integrative review and research propositions. J. Acad. Marketing Sci. , 24, 338–349.
Banca P. , Lange I. , Worbe Y. , Howell N. A. , Irvine M. , Harrison N. A. , Moutoussis M. , & Voon V. (2016). Reflection impulsivity in binge drinking: Behavioural and volumetric correlates. Addict. Biol., 21, 504–515.
Barratt E. S. , Stanford M. S. , Dowdy L. , Liebman M. J. , & Kent T. A. (1999). Impulsive and premeditated aggression: A factor analysis of self-reported acts. Psychiatry Res., 86, 163–173.
Baumann A. A. , & Odum A. L. (2012). Impulsivity, risk taking, and timing. Behav. Process., 90, 408–414.
Benjamini Y. , & Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B Stat. Methodol. , 57, 289–300.
Berlin H. A. , & Rolls E. T. (2004). Time perception, impulsivity, emotionality, and personality in self-harming borderline personality disorder patients. J. Pers. Disord. , 18, 358–378.
Block R. A. , & Reed M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem., 4, 656–665.
Block R. A. , George E. J. , & Reed M. A. (1980). A watched pot sometimes boils: A study of duration experience. Acta Psychol., 46, 81–94.
Bradley M. M. , & Lang P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry , 25, 49–59.
Cahoon D. , & Edmonds E. M. (1980). The watched pot still won’t boil: Expectancy as a variable in estimating the passage of time. Bull. Psychonom. Soc., 16, 115–116.
Conti R. (2001). Time flies: Investigating the connection between intrinsic motivation and the experience of time. J. Pers. , 69, 1–26.
Costa R. M. , Pestana J. , Costa D. , & Wittmann M. (2016). Altered states of consciousness are related to higher sexual responsiveness. Consc. Cogn. , 42, 135–141.
Craig A. D. (2009). Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B , 364, 1933–1942.
Craig A. D. (2015). How Do You Feel? An Interoceptive Moment with Your Neurobiological Self . Princeton, MA, USA: Princeton University Press.
Dalley J. W. , Everitt B. J. , & Robbins T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron, 69, 680–694.
Danckert J. A. , & Allman A. A. A. (2005). Time flies when you’re having fun: Temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain Cogn., 59, 236–245.
Droit-Volet S. , & Gil S. (2009). The time-emotion paradox. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B , 364, 1943–1954.
Droit-Volet S. , & Meck W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends Cogn. Sci., 11, 504–513.
Droit-Volet S. , Fayolle S. , Lamotte M. , & Gil S. (2013). Time, emotion and the embodiment of timing. Timing Time Percept. , 1, 99–126.
Egger S. , Reichl P. , Hoßfeld T. , & Schatz R. (2012). “Time is bandwidth”? Narrowing the gap between subjective time perception and quality of experience. In: Proc. 2012 IEEE Int. Conf. Commun. (ICC), Ottawa, ON, Canada, pp. 1325–1330.
Fahlman S. A. , Mercer-Lynn K. B. , Flora D. B. , & Eastwood J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment , 20, 68–85.
Fleisig D. , Ginzburg K. , & Zakay D. (2009). A model of waiting’s duration judgment. NeuroQuantology, 7, 58–65.
French R. M. , Addyman C. , Mareschal D. , & Thomas E. (2014). Unifying prospective and retrospective interval-time estimation: A fading-Gaussian activation-based model of interval-timing. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci., 126, 141–150.
Gil S. , & Droit-Volet S. (2012). Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cogn. Emot. , 26, 847–862.
Glicksohn J. (2001). Temporal cognition and the phenomenology of time: A multiplicative function for apparent duration. Consc. Cogn., 10, 1–25.
Glicksohn J. , Leshem R. , & Aharoni R. (2006). Impulsivity and time estimation: Casting a net to catch a fish. Pers. Individ. Dif., 40, 261–271.
Hendin H. (1982). Suicide in America. New York, NY, USA: Norton.
Hornik J. (1992). Time estimation and orientation mediated by transient mood. J. Socio Econ., 21, 209–227.
Katz K. L. , Larson B. M. , & Larson R. C. (1991). Prescription for the waiting-in-line blues: Entertain, enlighten, and engage. MIT Sloan Manag. Rev., 32, 44–53.
Lambrechts A. , Mella N. , Pouthas V. , & Noulhiane M. (2011). Subjectivity of time perception: A visual emotional orchestration. Front. Integr. Neurosci. , 5, 73. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00073.
Lamotte M. , Chakroun N. , Droit-Volet S. , & Izaute M. (2014). Metacognitive questionnaire on time: Feeling of the passage of time. Timing Time Percept. , 2, 339–359.
Lucci C. R. (2013). Time, self, and intertemporal choice. Front Neurosci., 7, 40. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00040.
MacKinnon D. P. , Fairchild A. J. , & Fritz M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 58, 593–614.
Meck W. H. , & Church R. M. (1983). A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Animal Behav. Proc., 9, 320–334.
Moreira D. , Pinto M. , Almeida F. , & Barbosa F. (2016). Time perception deficits in impulsivity disorders: A systematic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. , 27, 87–92.
Mueller A. K. , Berger J. , Tucha O. , & Falter C. M. (2014). Selective impairment of timing functions in non-clinical impulsiveness. Timing Time Percept. , 2, 20–34.
Osuna E. E. (1985). The psychological cost of waiting. J. Math. Psychol. , 29, 82–105.
Petter E. A. , & Merchant H. (2016). Temporal processing by intrinsic neural network dynamics. Timing Time Percept. , 4, 399–410.
Pfeifer E. , Sarikaya A. , & Wittmann M. (2016). Changes in states of consciousness during a period of silence after a session of depth relaxation music therapy (DRMT). Music Med. , 8, 180–186.
Pollatos O. , Laubrock J. , & Wittmann M. (2014). Interoceptive focus shapes the experience of time. PloS One , 9, e86934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086934.
Preacher K. J. , & Hayes A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Meth., 40, 879–891.
Reise S. P. , Moore T. M. , Sabb F. W. , Brown A. K. , & London E. D. (2013). The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11: Reassessment of its structure in a community sample. Psychol. Assess., 25, 631–642.
Sackett A. M. , Meyvis T. , Nelson L. D. , Converse B. A. , & Sackett A. L. (2010). You’re having fun when time flies: The hedonic consequences of subjective time progression. Psychol. Sci., 21, 111–117.
Sharma L. , Kohl K. , Morgan T. A. , & Clark L. A. (2013). “Impulsivity”: Relations between self-report and behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 104, 559–575.
Sharma L. , Markon K. E. , & Clark L. A. (2014). Toward a theory of distinct types of “impulsive” behaviors: A meta-analysis of self-report and behavioral measures. Psychol. Bull., 140, 374–408.
Simen P. , Rivest F. , Ludvig E. A. , Balci F. , & Killeen P. (2013). Timescale invariance in the pacemaker-accumulator family of timing models. Timing Time Percept. , 1, 159–188.
Staddon J. E. R. , & Higa J. J. (1999). Time and memory: Towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing. J. Exp. Analys. Behav., 71, 215–251.
Taatgen N. A. , Van Rijn H. , & Anderson J. (2007). An integrated theory of prospective time interval estimation: The role of cognition, attention, and learning. Psychol. Rev., 114, 577–598.
Tanaka R. , & Yotsumoto Y. (2017). Passage of time judgments is relative to temporal expectation. Front. Psychol., 8 (187). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00187.
Taylor S. (1994). Waiting for service: The relationship between delays and evaluations of service. J. Marketing , 58, 56–69.
Treisman M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock” . Psychol. Monogr.: Gen. Appl., 77, 1–31.
Treisman M. (2013). The information-processing model of timing (Treisman, 1963): Its sources and further development. Timing Time Percept. , 1, 131–158.
Turel O. , & Bechara A. (2016) A triadic reflective-impulsive-interoceptive awareness model of general and impulsive information system use: Behavioral tests of neuro-cognitive theory. Front. Psychol. , 7, 601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00601.
Twenge J. M. , Catanese K. R. & Baumeister R. F. (2003). Social exclusion and the deconstructed state: Time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. , 85, 409–423.
Van Rijn H. , Gu B. M. , & Meck W. H. (2014). Dedicated clock/timing-circuit theories of time perception and timed performance. In: H. Merchant & V. de Lafuente (Eds), Neurobiology of Interval Timing. New York, NY, USA: Springer, 75–99.
Varela F. J. , Thompson E. & Rosch E. (2016). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press (first ed. 1991).
Wackermann J. , & Ehm W. (2006). The dual klepsydra model of internal time representation and time reproduction. J. Theor. Biol., 239, 482–493.
Wackermann J. , Meissner K. , Tankersley D. , & Wittmann M. (2014). Effects of emotional valence and arousal on acoustic duration reproduction assessed via the ‘dual klepsydra model’. Front. Neurorobotics, 8, 11. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2014.00011.
Watt J. D. (1991). Effect of boredom proneness on time perception. Psychol. Rep., 69, 323–327.
Wearden J. (2016). The Psychology of Time Perception. London, UK: Macmillan Publishers.
Weiner L. , Wittmann M. , Bertschy G. , & Giersch A. (2016). Dispositional mindfulness and subjective time in healthy individuals. Front. Psychol. , 7,786. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00786.
Wilson T. D. , Reinhard D. A. , Westgate E. C. , Gilbert D. T. , Ellerbeck N. , Hahn C. , Brown C. L. & Shaked A. (2014). Just think: The challenges of the disengaged mind. Science, 345, 75–77.
Wittmann M. (2009). The inner sense of time. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. , 364, 1955–1967.
Wittmann M. (2013). The inner sense of time: How the brain creates a representation of duration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 14, 217–223.
Wittmann M. (2016). Felt Time: The Psychology of How We Perceive Time. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press.
Wittmann M. , & Paulus M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity, and time perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. , 12, 7–12.
Wittmann M. , & Paulus M. P. (2016). How the experience of time shapes decision-making. In: M. Reuter & C. Montag (Eds), Neuroeconomics (pp. 133–144). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.
Wittmann M. , Leland D. S. , Churan J. , & Paulus M. P. (2007). Impaired time perception and motor timing in stimulant-dependent subjects. Drug Alcohol Depend. , 90, 183–192.
Wittmann M. , Simmons A. N. , Flagan T. , Lane S. D. , Wackermann J. , & Paulus M. P. (2011). Neural substrates of time perception and impulsivity. Brain Res. , 1406, 43–58.
Wittmann M. , Otten S. , Schötz E. , Sarikaya A. , Lehnen H. , Jo H. G. , Kohls N. , Schmidt S. , & Meissner K. (2014). Subjective expansion of extended time-spans in experienced meditators. Front. Psychol. , 5,1586. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01586.
Wittmann M. , Rudolph T. , Linares Gutierrez D. , & Winkler I. (2015). Time perspective and emotion regulation as predictors of age-related subjective passage of time. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health, 12, 16027–16042.
Wittmann M. , Fiedler H. , Gros W. , Mossbridge J. , & Lucci C. R. (2017). Individual differences related to present and future mental orientation predict the sense of time. https://psyarxiv.com/ezmhp. Accessed October 11, 2017.
Zakay D. (2014). Psychological time as information: The case of boredom. Front. Psychol. , 5,917. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00917.
Zakay D. (2015). The temporal-relevance temporal-uncertainty model of prospective duration judgment. Consc. Cogn. , 38,182–190.
Zakay D. , & Block R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. , 6, 12–16.
Zimbardo P. G. , & Boyd J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-difference metric. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. , 77, 1271–1288.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 3526 | 625 | 45 |
Full Text Views | 417 | 47 | 3 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 302 | 73 | 8 |
In this study we investigated how individual differences regarding impulsivity and time perspective predict the experience of waiting during a time interval without distraction. Each participant (N = 82) filled out self-report questionnaires on impulsiveness (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale) and time perspective (Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory). Participants were individually shut into an empty room for exactly 7.5 minutes and afterwards asked to report their subjective impressions regarding the experience of time, self and affective reactions. Correlation analyses showed that being more relaxed and having a positive feeling was related to shorter duration estimates and a perceived faster passage of time. Individual traits of impulsivity, present-hedonistic and present-fatalistic time orientation were associated with relative overestimation of duration and a perceived slower passage of time; future orientation was related to an underestimation of duration and a perceived faster passage of time. Multiple regression models show that impulsivity alone predicts the variance related to time estimation of the waiting period. Multiple path analyses reveal that individuals who are more impulsive are less relaxed while waiting and accordingly overestimate duration and feel a slower passage of time. Negative affect and an impulsive present orientation are related to an overestimation of duration and the feeling of a slower passage of time while waiting.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 3526 | 625 | 45 |
Full Text Views | 417 | 47 | 3 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 302 | 73 | 8 |