Emotional stimuli like emotional faces have been frequently shown to be temporally overestimated compared to neutral ones. This effect has been commonly explained by induced arousal caused by emotional processing leading to the acceleration of an inner-clock-like pacemaker. However, there are some studies reporting contradictory effects and others point to relevant moderating variables. Given this controversy, we aimed at investigating the processes underlying the temporal overestimation of emotional faces by combining behavioral and electrophysiological correlates in a temporal bisection task. We assessed duration estimation of angry and neutral faces using anchor durations of 400 ms and 1600 ms while recording event-related potentials. Subjective ratings and the early posterior negativity confirmed encoding and processing of stimuli’s emotionality. However, temporal ratings did not differ between angry and neutral faces. In line with this behavioral result, the Contingent Negative Variation (CNV), an electrophysiological index of temporal accumulation, was not modulated by the faces’ emotionality. Duration estimates, i.e., short or long responses toward stimuli of ambiguous durations of 1000 ms, were nevertheless associated with a differential CNV amplitude. Interestingly, CNV modulation was already observed at 600–700 ms after stimulus onset, i.e., long before stimulus offset. The results are discussed in light of the information-processing model of time perception as well as regarding possible factors of the experimental setup moderating temporal overestimation of emotional stimuli. In sum, combining behavioral and electrophysiological measures seems promising to more clearly understand the complex processes leading to the illusion of temporal lengthening of emotional faces.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Allman, M. J. , Teki, S. , Griffiths, T. D. , & Meck, W. H. (2014). Properties of the internal clock: first-and second-order principles of subjective time. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 65, 743–771. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117.
Angrilli, A. , Cherubini, P. , Pavese, A. , & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept. Psychophys. , 59, 972–982. doi: 10.3758/BF03205512.
Ashley, V. , Vuilleumier, P. , & Swick, D. (2004). Time course and specificity of event-related potentials to emotional expressions. NeuroReport , 15, 211–216. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200401190-00041.
Bar-Haim, Y. , Kerem, A. , Lamy, D. , & Zakay, D. (2010). When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cogn. Emot. , 24, 255–263. doi: 10.1080/02699930903387603.
Bentin, S. , Allison, T. , Puce, A. , Perez, E. , & McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. , 8, 551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551.
Bertrand, O. , Perrin, F. , & Pernier, J. (1985). A theoretical justification of the average reference in topographic evoked potential studies. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. , 62, 462–464. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(85)90058-9.
Blau, V. C. , Maurer, U. , Tottenham, N. , & McCandliss, B. D. (2007). The face-specific N170 component is modulated by emotional facial expression. Behav. Brain Funct. , 3, 7. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-7.
Bless, H. & Fiedler, K. (1995). Affective states and the influence of activated general knowledge. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 766–778. doi: 10.1177/0146167295217010.
Block, R. A. , Hancock, P. A. , & Zakay, D. (2000). Sex differences in duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Mem. Cogn., 28, 1333–1346. doi: 10.3758/BF03211834.
Bradley, M. M. , Codispoti, M. , Sabatinelli, D. , & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.300.
Bradley, M. M. , & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry, 25, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9.
Buetti, S. , & Lleras, A. (2012). Perceiving control over aversive and fearful events can alter how we experience those events: an investigation of time perception in spider-fearful individuals. Front. Psychol. , 3, 337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00337.
Burle, B. , & Casini, L. (2001). Dissociation between activation and attention effects in time estimation: Implications for internal clock models. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. , 27, 195–205. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.1.195.
Calder, A. J. , Burton, A. M. , Miller, P. , Young, A. W. , & Akamatsu, S. (2001). A principal component analysis of facial expressions. Vision Res. , 41, 1179–1208. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00002-5.
Canli, T. , Desmond, J. E. , Zhao, Z. , & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. , 99, 10789–10794. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162356599.
Chambon M. , Droit-Volet S. , & Niedenthal P. M. (2008). The effect of embodying the elderly on time perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol., 44, 672–678. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.04.014.
Doi, H. , & Shinohara, K. (2009). The perceived duration of emotional face is influenced by the gaze direction. Neurosci. Lett. , 457, 97–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.04.004.
Droit-Volet, S. , & Gil, S. (2009). The time-emotion paradox. Philos. Trans. R Soc. B Biol. Sci. , 364, 1943–1953. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0013.
Droit-Volet, S. , & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends Cogn. Sci. , 11, 504–513. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008.
Droit-Volet, S. , Brunot, S. , & Niedenthal, P. (2004). Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cogn. Emot. , 18, 849–858. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000194.
Droit-Volet, S. , Fayolle, S. , & Gil, S. (2016). Emotion and time perception in children and adults: the effect of task difficulty. Timing Time Percept. , 4, 7–29. doi: 10.1163/22134468-03002055.
Eberhardt, L. V. , Huckauf, A. , & Kliegl, K. (2016). Effects of neutral and fearful mood on duration estimation of neutral and fearful face stimuli. Timing Time Percept., 4, 30–47. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002060.
Effron, D. A. , Niedenthal, P. M. , Gil, S. , & Droit-Volet, S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion , 6, 1–9. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.1.
Espinosa-Fernández, L. , Miró, E. , Cano, M. , & Buela-Casal, G. (2003). Age-related changes and gender differences in time estimation. Acta Psychol. , 112, 221–232. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00093-8.
Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychol. Res., 65, 250–259. doi: 10.1007/s004260100065.
Gibbon, J. , Church, R. M. , & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. , 423, 52–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x.
Gil, S. , & Droit-Volet, S. (2011). “Time flies in the presence of angry faces”... depending on the temporal task used! Acta Psychol., 136(3), 354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.010.
Gil, S. , & Droit-Volet, S. (2012a). How do emotional facial expressions influence our perception of time? In S. Masmoudi , D. Y. Dai , & A. Naceur (Eds), Attention, representation, and human performance: Integration of cognition, emotion and motivation (pp. 61–74). New York, NY, USA: Psychology Press.
Gil, S. , & Droit-Volet, S. (2012b). Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cogn. Emot. , 26, 847–862. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.625401.
Grommet, E. K. , Droit-Volet, S. , Gil, S. , Hemmes, N. S. , Baker, A. H. , & Brown, B. L. (2011). Time estimation of fear cues in human observers. Behav. Proc. , 86, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.10.003.
Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Atten., Percept. Psychophys., 72, 561–582. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.3.561.
Grondin, S. , Laflamme, V. , Bienvenue, P. , Labonté, K. , & Roy, M.-L. (2015). Sex effect in the temporal perception of faces expressing anger and shame. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. , 28, 27200.
Halbertsma, H. N. , & Van Rijn, H. (2016). An evaluation of the effect of auditory emotional stimuli on interval timing. Timing Time Percept. , 4, 48–62. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002061.
Hamann, S. , & Canli, T. (2004). Individual differences in emotion processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. , 14, 233–238. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.010.
Hancock, P. A. , & Rausch, R. (2010). The effects of sex, age, and interval duration on the perception of time. Acta Psychol., 133, 170–179. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.11.005.
Haxby, J. V. , Hoffman, E. A. , & Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry , 51, 59–67. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01330-0.
Herbert, C. , Junghofer, M. , & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology , 45, 487–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x.
Hinojosa, J. A. , Mercado, F. , & Carretié, L. (2015). N170 sensitivity to facial expression: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. , 55, 498–509. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.002.
Jazayeri, M. , & Shadlen, M. N. (2010). Temporal context calibrates interval timing. Nat. Neurosci. , 13, 1020–1026. doi: 10.1038/nn.2590.
Keuper, K. , Zwanzger, P. , Nordt, M. , Eden, A. , Laeger, I. , Zwitserlood, P. , Kissler, J. , Junghofer, M. , & Dobel, C. (2014). How ‘love’ and ‘hate’ differ from ‘sleep’: Using combined electro/magnetoencephalographic data to reveal the sources of early cortical responses to emotional words. Hum. Brain Mapp., 35, 875–888. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22220.
Kiefer, M. , Schuch, S. , Schenck, W. , & Fiedler, K. (2007). Mood states modulate activity in semantic brain areas during emotional word encoding. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1516–1530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl062.
Kiefer, M. , Sim, E.-J. , Helbig, H. , & Graf, M. (2011). Tracking the time course of action priming on object recognition: evidence for fast and slow influences of action on perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 23, 1864–1874. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21543c.
Kiefer, M. , Neff, U. , Schmid, M. M. , Spitzer, M. , Connemann, B. J. , & Schönfeldt-Lecuona, C. (2017). Brain activity to transitional objects in patients with borderline personality disorder. Sci. Rep. , 7, 13121. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13508-8.
Kissler, J. , Assadollahi, R. , & Herbert, C. (2006). Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: insights from ERP studies. Progress in Brain Research , 156, 147–183. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56008-X.
Kissler, J. , Herbert, C. , Peyk, P. , & Junghofer, M. (2007). Buzzwords: early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychological Science , 18, 475–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01924.x.
Kliegl, K. M. , Watrin, L. , & Huckauf, A. (2015a). Duration perception of emotional stimuli: Using evaluative conditioning to avoid sensory confounds. Cogn. Emot., 29, 1350–1367. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.978841.
Kliegl, K. M. , Limbrecht-Ecklundt, K. , Dürr, L. , Traue, H. C. , & Huckauf, A. (2015b). The complex duration perception of emotional faces: effects of face direction. Front. Psychol., 6, 262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00262.
Kononowicz, T. W. , & Penney, T. B. (2016). The contingent negative variation (CNV): timing isn’t everything. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. , 8, 231–237.
Kononowicz, T. W. , & Van Rijn, H. (2011). Slow potentials in time estimation: the role of temporal accumulation and habituation. Front. Integr. Neurosci. , 5, 48. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00048.
Kononowicz, T. W. , Sander, T. , & Van Rijn, H. (2015). Neuroelectromagnetic signatures of the reproduction of supra-second durations. Neuropsychologia, 75, 201–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.001.
Kononowicz, T. W. , van Rijn, H. , & Meck, W. H. (2018). Timing and time perception: A critical review of neural timing signatures before, during, and after the to-be-timed interval. In J.T. Wixted (Ed.), Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol. 1 (pp. 1–38). Hoboken, NY, USA, John Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1002/9781119170174.epcn114.
Kring, A. M. , & Gordon, A. H. (1998). Sex differences in emotion: expression, experience, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. , 74, 686–703. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.686.
Lake, J. I. (2016). Recent advances in understanding emotion-driven temporal distortions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci., 8, 214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.009.
Lake, J. I. , Meck, W. H. , & LaBar, K. S. (2016). Discriminative fear learners are resilient to temporal distortions during threat anticipation. Timing Time Percept., 4, 63–78. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002063.
Langner, O. , Dotsch, R. , Bijlstra, G. , Wigboldus, D. H. J. , Hawk, S. T. , & Van Knippenberg, A. (2010a). Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cogn. Emot. , 24, 1377–1388. doi: 10.1080/02699930903485076.
Langner, O. , Dotsch, R. , Bijlstra, G. , Wigboldus, D. H. , Hawk, S. T. , & Van Knippenberg, A. (2010b). Support material for the article: Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database (RaFD). Retrieved from http://www.socsci.ru.nl/rafd/CEM_136.09R_support.pdf.
Loftus, E. F. , Schooler, J. W. , Boone, S. M. , & Kline, D. (1987). Time went by so slowly: Overestimation of event duration by males and females. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 1, 3–13. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350010103.
Macar, F. , & Vidal, F. (2004). Event-related potentials as indices of time processing: a review. J. Psychophysiol. , 18, 89–104. doi; 10.1027/0269-8803.18.23.89.
Macar, F. , Vidal, F. , & Casini, L. (1999). The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes. Exp. Brain Res. , 125, 271–280. doi: 10.1007/s002210050683.
Makeig, S. , Jung, T.-P. , Bell, A. J. , Ghahremani, D. , & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 94, 10979–10984. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979.
Meck, W. H. , Penney, T. B. , & Pouthas, V. (2008). Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 18, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002.
Mella, N. , Conty, L. , & Pouthas, V. (2011). The role of physiological arousal in time perception: Psychophysiological evidence from an emotion regulation paradigm. Brain and Cognition , 75, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.012.
Noulhiane, M. , Mella, N. , Samson, S. , Ragot, R. , & Pouthas, V. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion, 7, 697–704. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.697.
Ng, K. K. , Tobin, S. , & Penney, T. B. (2011). Temporal accumulation and decision processes in the duration bisection task revealed by contingent negative variation. Front. Integr. Neurosci., 5, 77. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00077.
Nunez, P. L. (1981). Electrical fields of the brain: The neurophysics of EEG. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.
Olofsson, J. K. , Nordin, S. , Sequeira, H. , & Polich, J. (2008). Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings. Biol. Psychol., 77, 247–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006.
Opitz, B. , & Degner, J. (2012). Emotionality in a second language: It’s a matter of time. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1961–1967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.021.
Pfeuty, M. , Ragot, R. , & Pouthas, V. (2005). Relationship between CNV and timing of an upcoming event. Neurosci. Lett. , 382, 106–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.067.
Pittino, F. , Kliegl, K. M. , & Huckauf, A. (2017). Subjective, physiological, and behavioural responses towards evaluatively conditioned stimuli. Cogn. Emot.. 32, 1082–1096. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1386625.
Pouthas, V. (2003). Electrophysiological evidence for specific processing of temporal information in humans. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 439–456). Boca Raton, FL, USA: CRC Press.
Povel, D.-J. , & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Percept. , 2, 411–440. doi: 10.2307/40285311.
Rammsayer, T. , & Troche, S. (2010). Sex differences in the processing of temporal information in the sub-second range. Pers. Individ. Dif. , 49, 923–927. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.031.
Rellecke, J. , Sommer, W. , & Schacht, A. (2012). Does processing of emotional facial expressions depend on intention? Time-resolved evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biol. Psychol., 90, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.002.
Remington, E. D. , Egger, S. W. , Narain, D. , Wang, J. , & Jazayeri, M. (2018). A dynamical systems perspective on flexible motor timing. Trends Cogn. Sci., 22, 938–952. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.010.
Rossion, B. , Delvenne, J.-F. , Debatisse, D. , Goffaux, V. , Bruyer, R. , Crommelinck, M. , & Guérit, J.-M. (1999). Spatio-temporal localization of the face inversion effect: an event-related potentials study. Biol. Psychol. , 50, 173–189. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00013-7.
Rossion, B. , Gauthier, I. , Tarr, M. J. , Despland, P. , Bruyer, R. , Linotte, S. , & Crommelinck, M. (2000). The N170 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: an electrophysiological account of face-specific processes in the human brain. NeuroReport , 11, 69–72. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200001170-00014.
Rugg, M. D. , & Coles, M. G. H. (1995). The ERP and cognitive psychology: Conceptual issues. In M. D. Rugg & M. G. H. Coles (Eds), Electrophysiology of mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition (pp. 27-39). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Russell, J. A. , & Mehrabian, A. (1977). Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions. J. Res. Pers. , 11, 273–294. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566 (77)90037-X.
Sarigiannidis, I. , Grillon, C. , Ernst, M. , Roiser, J. P. , & Robinson, O. J. (2020). Anxiety makes time pass quicker while fear has no effect. Cognition , 197, 104116. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104116.
Schirmer, A. (2011). How emotions change time. Front. Integr. Neurosci. , 5, 58. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00058.
Schirmer, A. , Lui, M. , Maess, B. , Escoffier, N. , Chan, M. , & Penney, T. B. (2006). Task and sex modulate the brain response to emotional incongruity in Asian listeners. Emotion , 6, 406–417. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.406.
Schirmer, A. , Ng, T. , Escoffier, N. , & Penney, T. B. (2016). Emotional voices distort time: behavioral and neural correlates. Timing Time Percept., 4, 79–98. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002058.
Schupp, H. T. , Öhman, A. , Junghöfer, M. , Weike, A. I. , Stockburger, J. , & Hamm, A. O. (2004). The facilitated processing of threatening faces: an ERP analysis. Emotion, 4, 189–200. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.189.
Schwarz, N. & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgment, The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning (pp. 55–72). New York. NY, USA: Pergamon Press.
Sequeira, H. , Hot, P. , Silvert, L. , & Delplanque, S. (2009). Electrical autonomic correlates of emotion. Int. J. Psychophysiol., 71, 50–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.009.
Spreckelmeyer, K. N. , Kutas, M. , Urbach, T. , Altenmuller, E. , & Munte, T. F. (2009). Neural processing of vocal emotion and identity. Brain Cogn. , 69, 121–126. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.06.003.
Stewart, L. H. , Ajina, S. , Getov, S. , Bahrami, B. , Todorov, A. , & Rees, G. (2012). Unconscious evaluation of faces on social dimensions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. , 141, 715–727. doi: 10.1037/a0027950.
Stroth, S. , Kubesch, S. , Dieterle, K. , Ruchsow, M. , Heim, R. , & Kiefer, M. (2009). Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential indices for executive control in healthy adolescents. Brain Res., 1269, 114–124. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.073.
Tipples, J. (2008). Negative emotionality influences the effects of emotion on time perception. Emotion , 8, 127–131. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.127.
Tipples, J. (2011). When time stands still: Fear-specific modulation of temporal bias due to threat. Emotion , 111, 74–80. doi: 10.1037/a0022015.
Tomas, J. , & Španić, A. M. (2016). Angry and beautiful: the interactive effect of facial expression and attractiveness on time perception. Psihol. Teme, 25, 299–315.
Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychol. Monogr. , 77, 1–31. doi: 10.1037/h0093864.
Treisman, M. , & Brogan, D. (1992). Time perception and the internal clock: Effects of visual flicker on the temporal oscillator. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. , 4, 41–70. doi: 10.1080/09541449208406242.
Wang, J. , Hosseini, E. , Meirhaeghe, N. , Akkad, A. , & Jazayeri, M. (2019). Reinforcement regulates timing variability in thalamus. bioRxiv, 583328. doi: 10.1101/583328.
Wittmann, M. (2013). The inner sense of time: how the brain creates a representation of duration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. , 14, 217–223. doi: 10.1038/nrn3452.
Yoon, S. , Shim, M. , Kim, H. S. , & Lee, S.-H. (2016). Enhanced early posterior negativity to fearful faces in patients with anxiety disorder. Brain Topogr., 29, 262–272. doi: 10.1007/S10548-015-0456-0.
Zakay, D. , & Block, R. A. (1995). An attentional gate model of prospective time estimation. In M. Richelle , V. De Keyser , G. d’Ydewalle , & A. Vandierendonck (Eds), Time and the dynamic control of behavior (pp. 167–178). Liège, Belgium: Université de Liège.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 752 | 112 | 5 |
Full Text Views | 25 | 2 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 36 | 4 | 0 |
Emotional stimuli like emotional faces have been frequently shown to be temporally overestimated compared to neutral ones. This effect has been commonly explained by induced arousal caused by emotional processing leading to the acceleration of an inner-clock-like pacemaker. However, there are some studies reporting contradictory effects and others point to relevant moderating variables. Given this controversy, we aimed at investigating the processes underlying the temporal overestimation of emotional faces by combining behavioral and electrophysiological correlates in a temporal bisection task. We assessed duration estimation of angry and neutral faces using anchor durations of 400 ms and 1600 ms while recording event-related potentials. Subjective ratings and the early posterior negativity confirmed encoding and processing of stimuli’s emotionality. However, temporal ratings did not differ between angry and neutral faces. In line with this behavioral result, the Contingent Negative Variation (CNV), an electrophysiological index of temporal accumulation, was not modulated by the faces’ emotionality. Duration estimates, i.e., short or long responses toward stimuli of ambiguous durations of 1000 ms, were nevertheless associated with a differential CNV amplitude. Interestingly, CNV modulation was already observed at 600–700 ms after stimulus onset, i.e., long before stimulus offset. The results are discussed in light of the information-processing model of time perception as well as regarding possible factors of the experimental setup moderating temporal overestimation of emotional stimuli. In sum, combining behavioral and electrophysiological measures seems promising to more clearly understand the complex processes leading to the illusion of temporal lengthening of emotional faces.
All Time | Past 365 days | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 752 | 112 | 5 |
Full Text Views | 25 | 2 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 36 | 4 | 0 |