Emotional distortions to time are consistently reported in laboratory studies; however, their underlying causes remain unclear. One suggestion is that emotion-induced changes in attentional processes may contribute to emotional distortions to time. The current study tested this possibility by examining the relationship between eye movement and perceptions of the duration of emotional events. Participants completed a verbal estimation task in which they estimated the duration of positively, negatively and neutrally valenced images from the International Affective Picture System images. Time to first fixation and dwell time were recorded throughout. The results showed no significant relationships between measures of eye movement and measures of emotional distortion to time, despite the emotion manipulation successfully influencing the time before the participants first fixated on the to-be-timed stimulus. This suggests that for suprasecond intervals emotion-induced changes in overt attention processing do not contribute towards emotional distortions to time.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept. Psychophys., 59, 972–982. doi: 10.3758/BF03205512.
Bannerman, R. L., Milders, M., & Sahraie, A. (2010). Attentional bias to brief threat-related faces revealed by saccadic eye movements. Emotion, 10, 733–783. doi: 10.1037/a0019354.
Bayliss, A. P., Schuch, S., & Tipper, S. P. (2010). Gaze cueing elicited by emotional faces is influenced by affective context. Vis. Cogn., 18, 1214–1232. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2010.484657.
Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1996). Models of psychological time revisited. In , H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and Mind (pp. 171–195). Kirkland, WA, USA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Millar, N. H. (2000). Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety. Cogn. Emot., 14, 789–808. doi: 10.1080/02699930050156636.
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., Bonham-Carter, C., Fergusson, E., Jenkins, J., & Parr, M. (1997). Attentional biases for emotional faces. Cogn. Emot., 11, 25–42. doi: 10.1080/026999397380014.
Brosch, T., Pourtois, G., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Additive effects of emotional, endogenous, and exogenous attention: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1779–1787. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.056.
Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relativity theory and time perception: single or multiple clocks? PLoS ONE, 4, e6268. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006268.
Burle, B., & Casini, L. (2001). Dissociation between activation and attention effects in time estimation: Implications for internal clock models. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 27, 195–205. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.1.195.
Calvo, M. G., & Lang, P. J. (2005). Parafoveal semantic processing of emotional visual scenes. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 31, 502–519. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.502.
Carretié, L. (2014). Exogenous (automatic) attention to emotional stimuli: a review. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci., 14, 1228–1258. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0270-2.
Cheng, R.-K., Tipples, J., Narayanan, N. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Clock speed as a window into dopaminergic control of emotion and time perception. Timing Time Percept., 4, 99–122. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002064.
Coull, J. T., Vidal, F., Nazarian, B., & Macar, F. (2004). Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science, 303, 1506–1508. doi: 10.1126/science.1091573.
Droit-Volet, S., Bigand, E., Ramos, D., & Bueno, J. L. O. (2010). Time flies with music whatever its emotional valence. Acta Psychol., 135, 226–232. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.003.
Droit-Volet, S., Brunot, S., & Niedenthal, P. (2004). Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cogn. Emot., 18, 849–858. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000194.
Droit-Volet, S., & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends Cogn. Sci., 11, 504–513. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008.
Eckstein, M. K., Guerra-Carrillo, B., Singley, A. T. M., & Bunge, S. A. (2017). Beyond eye gaze: What else can eyetracking reveal about cognition and cognitive development? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci., 25, 69–91. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.001.
Enns, J. T., Brehaut, J. C., & Shore, D. I. (1999). The duration of a brief event in the mind’s eye. J. Gen. Psychol., 126, 355–372. doi: 10.1080/00221309909595371.
Fayolle, S., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2015). Fear and time: Fear speeds up the internal clock. Behav. Proc., 120, 135–140. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.09.014.
Fernandes, A. C., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2020). A meta-analytical review of the familiarity temporal effect: Testing assumptions of the attentional and the fluency-attributional accounts. Psychol. Bull., 146, 187–217. doi: 10.1037/bul0000222.
Gan, T., Wang, N., Zhang, Z., Li, H., & Luo, Y.-J. (2009). Emotional influences on time perception: evidence from event-related potentials. Neuroreport, 20, 839–843. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832be7dc.
Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 423, 52–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749–6632.1984.tb23417.x.
Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2011). Time perception in response to ashamed faces in children and adults. Scand. J. Psychol., 52(2), 138–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00858.x.
Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cogn. Emot., 26, 847–862. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.625401.
Gil, S., Rousset, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2009). How liked and disliked foods affect time perception. Emotion, 9, 457–463. doi: 10.1037/a0015751.
Gowen, E., Abadi, R. V., Poliakoff, E., Hansen, P. C., & Miall, R. C. (2007). Modulation of saccadic intrusions by exogenous and endogenous attention. Brain Res., 1141, 154–167. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.047.
Grommet, E. K., Droit-Volet, S., Gil, S., Hemmes, N. S., Baker, A. H., & Brown, B. L. (2011). Time estimation of fear cues in human observers. Behav. Proc., 86, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.10.003.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 54, 917–924. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.917.
Hood, B. M., & Atkinson, J. (1993). Disengaging visual attention in the infant and adult. Infant Behav. Dev., 16, 405–422. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80001-O.
Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, 312–317. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.312.
Lake, J. I. (2016). Recent advances in understanding emotion-driven temporal distortions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci., 8, 214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.009.
Lake, J. I., LaBar, K. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 64, 403–420. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.003.
Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and biobehavioral assessment: Computer applications. In J. B. Sidowski, J. H. Johnson, & T. A. Williams (Eds), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp. 119–137). Norwood, NJ, USA: Ablex.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL, USA: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention.
Lejeune, H. (1998). Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time. Behav. Proc., 44, 127–145. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(98)00045-X.
Lejeune, H. (2000). Prospective timing, attention and the switch: A response to ‘Gating or switching? gating is a better model of prospective timing’ by Zakay. Behav. Proc., 52, 71–76. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00136-4.
Lui, M. A., Penney, T. B., & Schirmer, A. (2011). Emotion effects on timing: attention versus pacemaker accounts. PLoS ONE, 6, e21829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021829.
Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cogn. Brain Res., 21, 139–170. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.06.01.
Mathews, A., Fox, E., Yiend, J., & Calder, A. (2003). The face of fear: Effects of eye gaze and emotion on visual attention. Vis. Cogn., 10, 823–835. doi: 10.1080/13506280344000095.
Matsunaka, R., & Hiraki, K. (2014). Fearful gaze cueing: gaze direction and facial expression independently influence overt orienting responses in 12-month-olds. PloS ONE, 9, e89567. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089567.
Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Temporal cognition: Connecting subjective time to perception, attention, and memory. Psychol. Bull., 142, 865–907. http://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000045.
Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Directed attention prolongs the perceived duration of a brief stimulus. Percept. Psychophys., 60, 1305–1317. doi: 10.3758/BF03207993.
Mella, N., Conty, L., & Pouthas, V. (2011). The role of physiological arousal in time perception: psychophysiological evidence from an emotion regulation paradigm. Brain Cogn., 75, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.012.
Mella, N., & Pouthas, V. (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for an accumulation process in the timing of emotional stimuli. In: In: A. Vatakis, A. Esposito, M. Giagkou, F. Cummins, G. Papadelis (Eds), Multidisciplinary aspects of time and time perception (pp. 173–185). Berlin, Germany: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-21478-3_14.
Milstein, D. M., & Dorris, M. C. (2007). The influence of expected value on saccadic preparation. J. Neurosci., 27, 4810–4818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0577-07.2007.
Mogg, K., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2007). Anxiety and orienting of gaze to angry and fearful faces. Biol. Psychol., 76, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.005.
Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vis. Res., 29, 1631–1647. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90144-2.
Noulhiane, M., Mella, N., Samson, S., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion, 7, 697–704. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.697.
Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6, 257–268. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.257.
Ogden, R. S. (2013). The effect of facial attractiveness on temporal perception. Cogn. Emot., 27, 1292–1304. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.769426.
Ogden, R. S., Henderson, J., McGlone, F., & Richter, M. (2019). Time distortion under threat: Sympathetic arousal predicts time distortion only in the context of negative, highly arousing stimuli. PLoS ONE, 14, e0216704. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216704.
Ogden, R. S., Moore, D., Redfern, L., & McGlone, F. (2014). The effect of pain and the anticipation of pain on temporal perception: A role for attention and arousal. Cogn. Emot., 29, 910–922. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.954529.
Ogden, R. S., Moore, D., Redfern, L., & McGlone, F. (2015). Stroke me for longer this touch feels too short: The effect of pleasant touch on temporal perception. Consc. Cogn., 36, 306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.006.
Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention. Vis. Res., 42, 107–123. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00250-4.
Pawling, R., Trotter, P. D., McGlone, F. P., & Walker, S. C. (2017). A positive touch: C-tactile afferent targeted skin stimulation carries an appetitive motivational value. Biol. Psychol., 129, 186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.057.
Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychol. Sci., 17, 292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x.
Piovesan, A., Mirams, L., Poole, H., Moore, D., & Ogden, R. (2019). The relationship between pain-induced autonomic arousal and perceived duration. Emotion, 19, 1148–1161. doi: 10.1037/emo0000512.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 32, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231.
Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: What is magic and what is not. Biol. Psychol., 92, 492–512. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007.
Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 62, 1457–1506. doi: 10.1080/17470210902816461.
Sears, C. R., Thomas, C. L., LeHuquet, J. M., & Johnson, J. C. S. (2010). Attentional biases in dysphoria: An eye-tracking study of the allocation and disengagement of attention. Cogn. Emot., 24, 1349–1368. doi: 10.1080/02699930903399319.
Seifried, T., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Exogenous visual attention prolongs perceived duration. Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 73, 68–85. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0005-6.
Smith, S. D., McIver, T. A., Di Nella, M. S. J., & Crease, M. L. (2011). The effects of valence and arousal on the emotional modulation of time perception: Evidence for multiple stages of processing. Emotion, 11, 1305–1313. doi: 10.1037/a0026145.
Soto, D., Heinke, D., Humphreys, G. W., & Blanco, M. J. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 31, 248–261. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.248.
Subramanian, R., Shankar, D., Sebe, N., & Melcher, D. (2014). Emotion modulates eye movement patterns and subsequent memory for the gist and details of movie scenes. J. Vis., 14, 31. doi: 10.1167/14.3.31.
Tamm, M., Uusberg, A., Allik, J., & Kreegipuu, K. (2014). Emotional modulation of attention affects time perception: Evidence from event-related potentials. Acta Psychol., 149, 148–156. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.008.
Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Irwin, D. E., & Zelinsky, G. J. (1999). Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 25, 1595–1608. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1595.
Tipples, J. (2010). Time flies when we read taboo words. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 17, 563–568. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.4.563.
van Hedger, K., Necka, E. A., Barakzai, A. K., & Norman, G. J. (2017). The influence of social stress on time perception and psychophysiological reactivity. Psychophysiology, 54, 706–712. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12836.
Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011.
Watts, F. N. and Sharrock, R. 1984. Fear and time-estimation. Percept. Mot. Skills, 59, 597–598. doi: 10.2466/pms.1984.59.2.597.
Wearden, J. (2016). The psychology of time perception. Palgrave Macmillan, London, UK.
Wearden, J. H., O’Rourke, S. C., Matchwick, C., Min, Z., & Maeers, S. (2010). Task switching and subjective duration. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 63, 531–543. doi: 10.1080/17470210903024768.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol. Bull., 120, 3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3.
Wittman, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, 364, 1955–1967. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0003.
Yamada, Y., & Kawabe, T. (2011). Emotion colors time perception unconsciously. Consc. Cogn., 20, 1835–1841. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.06.016.
Zakay, D. (2000). Gating or switching? Gating is a better model of prospective timing (a response to ‘switching or gating?’ by Lejeune). Behav. Proc., 52, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00141-8.
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1995). An attentional-gate model of prospective time estimation. In: M. Richelle, V. De Keyser, G. d’Ydewalle, & A. Vandierendonck (Eds), Time and the dynamic control of behavior (pp. 167–178). Liège, Belgium: Université de Liège.
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci., 6, 12–16. doi: 10.1111/1467–8721.ep11512604.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 471 | 99 | 6 |
Full Text Views | 24 | 6 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 22 | 9 | 0 |
Emotional distortions to time are consistently reported in laboratory studies; however, their underlying causes remain unclear. One suggestion is that emotion-induced changes in attentional processes may contribute to emotional distortions to time. The current study tested this possibility by examining the relationship between eye movement and perceptions of the duration of emotional events. Participants completed a verbal estimation task in which they estimated the duration of positively, negatively and neutrally valenced images from the International Affective Picture System images. Time to first fixation and dwell time were recorded throughout. The results showed no significant relationships between measures of eye movement and measures of emotional distortion to time, despite the emotion manipulation successfully influencing the time before the participants first fixated on the to-be-timed stimulus. This suggests that for suprasecond intervals emotion-induced changes in overt attention processing do not contribute towards emotional distortions to time.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 471 | 99 | 6 |
Full Text Views | 24 | 6 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 22 | 9 | 0 |