Song stability and neighbour recognition in a migratory songbird, the black redstart

In: Behaviour

Territorial male songbirds have the ability to discriminate between the songs of their neighbours and those of strangers and for a few species it has been shown that they maintain this ability from one breeding season to the next. To better understand the acoustic basis of this long-term discrimination ability we studied song stability across two breeding seasons in a migratory songbird with high inter-annual return rates and territory stability, the black redstart, Phoenicurus ochruros. Strophe repertoires of 14 males (≥2 years old) were stable from one breeding season to the next and high strophe sharing occurred for males within the same group of houses or hamlets (81%) in contrast to only limited sharing between different hamlets (15%). However, subtle differences exist between the renditions of the same strophe sung by neighbouring males and these differences equally show an inter-annual stability, providing an acoustic basis for long-term discrimination abilities. Playback tests showed the existence of a strong dear-enemy effect: males reacted less aggressively to the familiar, often shared song of a neighbour than to a stranger unshared song and this pattern was maintained when birds returned from migration one year later. We discuss on one side the possible mechanisms leading to the observed patterns of song sharing and on the other side the significance of stable vocal signatures for neighbour recognition.

  • ApfelbeckB.KieferS.MortegaG.M.GoymannW.KipperS. (2012). Testosterone affects song modulation during simulated territorial intrusions in male black redstarts (Phoenicurus ochruros). — PLoS ONE 7: e52009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BeecherM.D.AkçayC. (in press). Friends and enemies: how social dynamics shape communication and song learning in song sparrows. — In: Encyclopedia of animal behaviorVol. 3 ( YasukawaK., ed.). PraegerWestport, CT.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BeecherM.D.BrenowitzE.A. (2005). Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 20: 143-149.

  • BlumenrathS.H.DabelsteenT.PedersenS.B. (2007). Vocal neighbour–mate discrimination in female great tits despite high song similarity. — Anim. Behav. 73: 789-796.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BrieferE.AubinT.LehongreK.RybakF. (2008). How to identify dear enemies: the group signature in the complex song of the skylark Alauda arvensis. — J. Exp. Biol. 211: 317-326.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CuccoM.MalacarneG. (1999). Is the song of black redstart males an honest signal of status?Condor 101: 689-694.

  • DavisM.S. (1987). Acoustically mediated neighbor recognition in the North American bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21: 185-190.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DraganoiuT.I.NagleL.MusseauR.KreutzerM. (2005). Parental care and brood division in a songbird, the black redstart. — Behaviour 142: 1495-1514.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DraganoiuT.I.NagleL.MusseauR.KreutzerM. (2006a). In a songbird, the black redstart, parents use acoustic cues to discriminate between their different fledglings. — Anim. Behav. 71: 1039-1046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DraganoiuT.I.PasteauM.NagleL. (2006b). Do different elements of black redstart song have different threat values?J. Ornithol. 147: 158-159.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FisherJ. (1954). Evolution and bird sociality. — In: Evolution as a process ( HuxleyJ.HardyA.C.FordE.B., eds). Allen and UnwinLondon, p.  71-83.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ForstmeierW.BalsbyT.J.S. (2002). Why mated dusky warblers sing so much: territory guarding and male quality announcement. — Behaviour 139: 89-111.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FoxS.F.BairdT.A. (1992). The dear enemy phenomenon in the collared lizard, Crotaphytus collaris, with a cautionary note on experimental methodology. — Anim. Behav. 44: 780-782.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GilbertG.TylerG.A.SmithK.W. (2002). Local annual survival of booming male great bittern Botaurus stellaris in Britain, in the period 1990–1999. — Ibis 144: 51-61.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GodardR. (1991). Long-term memory of individual neighbours in a migratory songbird. — Nature 350: 228-229.

  • GravaT.MathevonN.PlaceE.BalluetP. (2008). Individual acoustic monitoring of the European eagle owl Bubo bubo. — Ibis 150: 279-287.

  • HardouinL.A.TabelP.BretagnolleV. (2006). Neighbour–stranger discrimination in the little owl, Athene noctua. — Anim. Behav. 72: 105-112.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JaegerR.G. (1981). Dear enemy recognition and the costs of agression between salamanders. — Am. Nat. 117: 962-974.

  • KipperS.KieferS. (2010). Age-related changes in birds’ singing styles: on fresh tunes and fading voices?Adv. Stud. Behav. 41: 77-118.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LandmannA.KollinskyC. (1995). Territory defence in black redstarts, Phoenicurus ochruros: effects of intruder and owner age?Ethology 101: 121-129.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LangenT.A.TripetF.NonacsP. (2000). The red and the black: habituation and the dear-enemy phenomenon in two desert Pheidole ants. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48: 285-292.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MathevonN.AubinT.VielliardJ.Da SilvaM.L.SebeF.BoscoloD. (2008). Singing in the rain forest: how a tropical bird song transfers information. — PLoS ONE 3: e1580.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McGregorP.K.KrebsJ.R. (1982). Song types in a population of great tits (Parus major): their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals. — Behaviour 79: 126-152.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MusseauR. (2007). Stratégies d’occupation spatiale chez le rougequeue noir (Phoenicurus ochruros) en période de reproduction. Habitat, fidélité, philopatrie et dispersion. — Ecole Pratique des Hautes EtudesParis.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MyrbergA.A.RiggioR.J. (1985). Acoustically mediated recognition by a coral reef fish (Pomacentrus partitus). — Anim. Behav. 33: 411-416.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NelsonD.A.PoeselA. (2009). Does learning produce song conformity or novelty in white-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys?Anim. Behav. 78: 433-440.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NicolaiB. (1992). Gesangdialekt beim Hausrotschwanz (Phoenicurus ochruros): Tradierung und Gesangslernen. — Rudolstädter Nat. Hist. Schr. 4: 83-90.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NordbyJ.J.CampbellS.E.BeecherM.D. (2007). Selective attrition and individual song repertoire development in song sparrows. — Anim. Behav. 74: 1413-1418.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • OsiejukT.S.RatyńskaK.CyganJ.P.DaleS. (2005). Frequency shift in homologue syllables of the ortolan bunting Emberiza hortulana. — Behav. Proc. 68: 69-83.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PeakeT.M.McgregorP.K.SmithK.W.TylerG.GilbertG.GreenR.E. (1998). Individuality in corncrake Crex crex vocalizations. — Ibis 140: 120-127.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RosellF.BjørkøyliT. (2002). A test of the dear enemy phenomenon in the Eurasian beaver. — Anim. Behav. 66: 1059-1067.

  • SedlácekO.CikánováB.FuchsR. (2006). Heterospecific rival recognition in the black redstart (Phoenicurus ochruros). — Ornis Fennica 83: 153-161.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • StoddardP.K. (1996). Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. — In: Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds ( KroodsmaD.E.MillerE.H., eds). Cornell University PressIthaca, NY, p.  356-374.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TemelesE.J. (1994). The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they ‘dear enemies’?Anim. Behav. 47: 339-350.

  • WeedenJ.S.FallsJ.B. (1959). Differential responses of male ovenbirds to recorded songs of neighboring and more distant individuals. — Auk 76: 343-351.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WegglerM. (2001). Age-related reproductive success in dichromatic male black redstarts Phoenicurus ochruros: why are yearlings handicapped?Ibis 143: 264-272.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WęgrzynE.LniowkiK.OsiejukT.S. (2009). Introduce yourself at the beginning–possible identification function of the initial part of the song in the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus. — Ornis Fennica 86: 61-70.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WilsonE.O. (1975). Sociobiology, the abridged edition. — Harvard University PressCambridge, MA.

  • WilsonP.L.VehrencampS.L. (2001). A test of the deceptive mimicry hypothesis in song-sharing song sparrows. — Anim. Behav. 62: 1197-1205.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 68 41 0
Full Text Views 212 5 1
PDF Downloads 7 3 0