A comparative assessment of handedness and its potential neuroanatomical correlates in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus)

In: Behaviour

The evolutionary origins of human right-handedness remain poorly understood. Some have hypothesized that tool use served as an important preadaptation for the eventual evolution of population-level right-handedness. In contrast, others have suggested that complex gestural and vocal communication served as prerequisite for the evolution of human right-handedness. In this study, we tested these competing hypotheses by comparing the handedness of bonobos and chimpanzees, two closely related species of Pan, on three different measures of hand use including simple reaching, manual gestures and coordinated bimanual actions. Chimpanzees are well known for their tool using abilities whereas bonobos rarely use tools in the wild. In contrast, many have suggested that bonobos have a more flexible gestural and vocal communication system than chimpanzees. The overall results showed that chimpanzees were significantly more right-handed than bonobos for all three measures suggesting that adaptations for tool use rather than communication may have led to the emergence of human right-handedness. We further show that species differences in handedness may be linked to variation in the size and asymmetry of the motor-hand area of the precentral gyrus. The results are discussed within the context of evolutionary theories of handedness, as well as some limitations in the approach to handedness measurement in nonhuman primates.

  • AlkadhiH.KolliasS.S. (2004). Pli de passage fronto-parietal moyen of Broca separates the motor homoculus. — Am. J. Neuroradiol. 25: 809-812.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • AnnettM. (2002). Handedness and brain asymmetry: the right shift theory. — Psychology Press, Hove.

  • BaileyP.von BoninG.McCullochW.S. (1950). The isocortex of the chimpanzee. — University of Illinois Press, Urbana-Champaign, IL.

  • BiroD.Inoue-NakamuraN.TonookaR.YamakoshiG.SousaC.MatsuzawaT. (2003). Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. — Anim. Cogn. 6: 213-223.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BiroD.SousaC.MatsuzawaT. (2006). Ontogeny and cultural propagation of tool use by wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: case studies in nut cracking and leaf folding. — In: Cognitive development of chimpanzees ( MatsuzawaT.TomonagaT.TanakaM., eds). Springer, New York, NY, p.  476-507.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BoeschC. (1991). Handedness in wild chimpanzees. — Int. J. Primatol. 6: 541-558.

  • BogartS.L.ManginJ.-F.SchapiroS.J.ReamerL.BennettA.J.PierreP.J.HopkinsW.D. (2012a). Cortical sulci asymmetries in chimpanzees and macaques: a new look at an old idea. — NeuroImage 61: 533-541.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BogartS.L.PruetzJ.D.OrmistonL.K.RussellJ.L.MeguerditchianA.HopkinsW.D. (2012b). Termite fishing laterality in the Fongoli savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus): further evidence of a left hand preference. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 149: 591-598.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BogartS.L.BennettA.J.SchapiroS.J.ReamerL.A.HopkinsW.D. (2014). Different early rearing experiences have long term effects on cortical organziation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Dev. Sci. 17: 161-174.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BolingW.OlivierA.BittarR.ReutensD. (1999). Localization of hand motor activation in Broca’s pli de passage moyen. — J. Neurosurg. 91: 903-910.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BoysenS.T.KuhlmeierK.HallidayP.HallidayY.M. (1999). Tool use in captive gorillas. — In: The mentality of gorillas and orangutans ( ParkerS.T.MitchellR.W.MilesH.L., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BradshawB.RogersL. (1993). The evolution of lateral asymmetries, language, tool-use and intellect. — Academic Press, San Diego, CA.

  • BradshawJ.L. (1997). Human evolution: a neuropsychological perspective. — Psychology Press, Hove.

  • BrakkeK.E.Savage-RumbaughE.S. (1995). The development of language skills in bonobo and chimpanzee — I. Comprehension. — Lang. Commun. 15: 121-148.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BrakkeK.E.Savage-RumbaughE.S. (1996). The development of language skills in Pan — II. Production. — Lang. Commun. 16: 361-380.

  • ButterworthG.ItakuraS. (1998). Development of precision grips in chimpanzees. — Dev. Sci. 1: 39-43.

  • ByrneR.W.ByrneJ.M. (1991). Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla berengei). — Cortex 27: 521-536.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CashmoreL. (2009). Can hominin “handedness” be accuartely assessed? — Ann. Hum. Biol. 36: 624-641.

  • CashmoreL.UominiN.ChapelainA. (2008). The evolution of handedness in humans and great apes: a review and current issues. — J. Anthropol. Sci. 86: 7-35.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ChapelainA.HogervorstE. (2009). Hand preferences for bimanual coordination in 29 bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Behav. Brain Res. 196: 15-29.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ChapelainA.HogervorstE.MbonzoP.HopkinsW.D. (2011). Hand preferences for bimanual coordination in 77 bonobos (Pan paniscus): replication and extension. — Int. J. Primatol. 32: 491-510.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ChristelM.I.KitzelS.NiemitzC. (1998). How precisely do bonobos (Pan paniscus) grasp small objects? — Int. J. Primatol. 19: 165-194.

  • ClayZ.ZuberbuhlerK. (2011). Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. — PLoS One 6: e18786.

  • CorballisM.C. (1992). The lopsided brain: Evolution of the generative mind. — Oxford University Press, New York, NY.

  • CorballisM.C. (2002). From hand to mouth: the origins of language. — Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

  • CoulonO.ClouchouxC.OperatoG.DauchotK.SiriguA.AntonJ.-L. (2006). Cortical localization via surface parameterization: a sulcus-based approach. — NeuroImage 31: S46.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CykowskiM.D.CoulonO.KochunovP.V.AmuntsK.LancasterJ.L.LairdA.R.GlahnC.FoxP.T. (2008). The central sulcus: an observer-independent characterization of sulcal landmarks and depth asymmetry. — Cerebr. Cort. 18: 1999-2009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DaddaM.CantalupoC.HopkinsW.D. (2006). Further evidence of an association between handedness and neuroanatomical asymmetries in the primary cortex of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Neuropsychologia 44: 2482-2486.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DavatzikosC.BryanR.N. (2002). Morphometric analysis of cortical sulci using parametric ribbons: a study of the central sulcus. — J. Comput. Assis. Tomogr. 26: 298-307.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • de WaalF.B.M. (1989). Behavioral contrasts between bonobo and chimpanzee. — In: Understanding chimpanzees ( HeltneP.G.MarquardtL.A., eds). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, p.  154-175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FischerA.WiebeV.PääboS.PrzeworskiM. (2004). Evidence for a complex demographic history of chimpanzees. — Mol. Biol. Evol. 21: 799-808.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ForresterG.S.LeavensD.A.QuaresminiC.VallortigaraG. (2011). Target animacy influences gorilla handedness. — Anim. Cogn. 14: 903-907.

  • ForresterG.S.QuaresminiC.LeavensD.A.SpiezioC.VallortigaraG. (2012). Target animacy influences chimpanzee handedness. — Anim. Cogn. 15: 1121-1127.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FrostG.T. (1980). Tool behavior and the origin of laterality. — J. Hum. Evol. 9: 447-459.

  • GallupG.G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. — Science 167: 86-87.

  • GibsonK.R.IngoldT. (1993). Tools, language and cognition in human evolution. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

  • GreenfieldP.M. (1991). Language, tools, and brain: the ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. — Behav. Brain Sci. 14: 531-550.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GreenfieldP.M.Savage-RumbaughE.S. (1990). Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: processes of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language. — In: “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative developmental perspectives ( ParkerS.T.GibsonK.R., eds). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, p.  540-578.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GruberT.ClayZ.ZuberbuhlerK. (2010). A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a femle bias in the Pan lineage. — Anim. Behav. 80: 1023-1033.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HammondG. (2002). Correlates of human handedness in primary motor cortex: a review and hypothesis. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26: 285-292.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HareB. (2007). From nonhuman to human mind. what changed and why? — Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 16: 60-64.

  • HareB.TomaselloM. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. — Anim. Behav. 68: 571-581.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HareB.MelisA.P.WoodsV.HastingsS.WranghamR. (2007). Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. — Curr. Biol. 17: 619-623.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HarrisonR.M.NystromP. (2008). Handedness in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Folia Primatologica 79: 253-268.

  • Herculano-HouzelS.MotaB.WongP.KaasJ.H. (2010). Connectivity-driven white matter scaling and folding in primate cerebral cortex. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107: 19008-19013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HerrmannE.HareB.CallJ.TomaselloM. (2010). Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. — PLoS One 5: e12438.

  • HopkinsW.D. (1995a). Hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees: cross-sectional analysis. — J. Comp. Psychol. 109: 291-297.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D. (1995b). Hand preferences in juvenile chimpanzees: continuity in development. — Dev. Psychol. 31: 619-625.

  • HopkinsW.D. (2007). Hemispheric specialization in chimpanzees: evolution of hand and brain. — In: Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience ( ShackelfordT.KeenanJ.P.PlatekS.M., eds). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, p.  99-120.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.CantalupoC. (2004). Handedness in chimpanzees is associated with asymmetries in the primary motor but not with homologous language areas. — Behav. Neurosci. 118: 1176-1183.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.de WaalF.B.M. (1995). Behavioral laterality in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): replication and extension. — Int. J. Primatol. 16: 261-276.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.PilcherD.L. (2001). Neuroanatomical localization of the motor hand area with magnetic resonance imaging: the left hemisphere is larger in Great Apes. — Behav. Neurosci. 115: 1159-1164.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.RussellJ.L. (2004). Further evidence of a right hand advantage in motor skill by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Neuropsychologia 42: 990-996.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.Savage-RumbaughE.S. (1991). Vocal communication as a function of differential rearing experiences in Pan paniscus: a preliminary report. — Int. J. Primatol. 12: 559-583.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.CantalupoC.WesleyM.J.HostetterA.B.PilcherD. (2002). Grip morphology and hand use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a left hemisphere specialization in motor skill. — J. Exp. Psychol. 131: 412-423.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.WesleyM.J.IzardM.K.HookM.SchapiroS.J. (2004). Chimpanzees are predominantly right-handed: replication in three colonies of apes. — Behav. Neurosci. 118: 659-663.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.RussellJ.FreemanH.BuehlerN.ReynoldsE.SchapiroS. (2005a). The distribution and development of handedness for manual gestures in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Psych. Sci. 16: 487.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.RussellJ.HookM.BracciniS.SchapiroS. (2005b). Simple reaching is not so simple: association between hand use and grip preferences in captive chimpanzees. — Int. J. Primatol. 26: 259-277.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.RussellJ.L.FreemanH.BuehlerN.ReynoldsE.SchapiroS.J. (2005c). The distribution and development of handedness for manual gestures in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). — Psychol. Sci. 16: 487-493.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.RussellJ.L.HookM.BracciniS.SchapiroS.J. (2005d). Simple reaching is not so simple: association between hand use and grip preferences in captive chimpanzees. — Int. J. Primatol. 26: 259-277.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.RussellJ.L.CantalupoC. (2007). Neuroanatomical correlates of handedness for tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implication for theories on the evolution of language. — Psychol. Sci. 18: 971-977.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.LynH.CantalupoC. (2009). Volumetric and lateralized differences in selected brain regions of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 988-997.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.CoulonO.ManginJ.F. (2010). Observer-independent characterization of sulcal landmarks and depth asymmetry in the central sulcus of the chimpanzee brain. — Neuroscience 171: 544-551.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.PhillipsK.A.BaniaA.CalcuttS.E.GardnerM.RussellJ.L.SchaefferJ.A.von LonsdorfE.RossS.SchapiroS.J. (2011). Hand preferences for coordinated bimanual actions in 777 great apes: implications for the evolution of handedness in Hominins. — J. Hum. Evol. 60: 605-611.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HopkinsW.D.PikaS.LiebalK.BaniaA.MeguerditchianA.GardnerM.SchapiroS.J. (2012). Handedness for manual gestures in great apes: a meta-analysis. — In: Current developments in non-human primate gesture research ( PikaS.LiebalK., eds). John Benjamins Publishing, Amsterdam.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HumleT.MatsuzawaT. (2009). Laterality in hand use across four tool use behaviors among the wild chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 40-48.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HyattC.W.HopkinsW.D. (1994). Self-awareness in bonobos and chimpanzees: a comparative perspective. — In: Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives ( ParkerS.T.MitchellR.W.BocciaM.L., eds). Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, p.  248-253.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones-EngelL.E.BardK.A. (1996). Precision grips in young chimpanzees. — Am. J. Primatol. 39: 1-15.

  • LambertM. (2012). Hand preference for bimanual and unimanual feeding in captive gorillas: extension in a second colony of apes. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 148: 641-647.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LeavensD.A.HopkinsW.D. (1998). Intentional communication by chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. — Dev. Psychol. 34: 813-822.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LilakA.L.PhillipsK.A. (2007). Consistency in hand preference across low-level and high-level tasks in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). — Am. J. Primatol. 69: 1-12.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LlorenteM.PalouL.CarrascoL.RibaD.MosqueraM.ColellM.FabreM.FeliuO. (2010). Population-level right handedness for a coordinated bimanual task in naturalistic housed chimpanzees: replication and extension in 114 animals from Zambia and Spain. — Am. J. Primatol. 71: 1-10.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LynH. (2007). Mental representation of symbols as revealed by vocabulary errors in two bonobos (Pan paniscus). — Anim. Cogn. 10: 461-475.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LynH.GreenfieldP.M.Savage-RumbaughE.S. (2006). The development of representational play in chimpanzees and bonobos: evolutionary implications, pretense, and the role of interspecies communication. — Cogn. Dev. 21: 199-213.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LynH.GreenfieldP.M.Savage-RumbaughE.S. (2011). Semiotic combinations in Pan: a cross-species comparison of communication in a chimpanzee and a bonobo. — First Lang. 31: 300-325.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ManginJ.F.RiviereD.CachiaA.DuchesnayE.CointepasY.Papadopoulos-OrfanosD.CollinsD.L.EvansA.C.RegisJ. (2004). Object-based morphometry of the cerebral cortex. — Med. Imag. 23: 968-982.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MarchantL.F.McGrewW.C. (2007). Ant fishing by wild chimpanzees is not lateralised. — Primates 48: 22-26.

  • McGrewW.C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: implications for human evolution. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

  • McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F. (1992). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: hand preference or handedness? — Curr. Anthropol. 33: 114-119.

  • McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F. (1993). Are gorillas right-handed or not? — Hum. Evol. 8: 17-23.

  • McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F. (1996). On which side of the apes? — In: Great ape societies ( McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F.NishidaT., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p.  255-272.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McGrewW.C.MarchantL.F. (1997). Using the tools at hand: manual laterality and elementary technology in Cebus spp. and Pan spp. — Int. J. Primatol. 18: 787-810.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MeguerditchianA.CalcuttS.E.von LonsdorfE.RossS.R.HopkinsW.D. (2010a). Captive gorillas are right-handed for bimanual feeding. — Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 141: 638-645.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MeguerditchianA.VauclairJ.HopkinsW.D. (2010b). Captive chimpanzees use their right hand to communicate with each other: implications for the origins of hemispheric specialization for language. — Cortex 46: 40-48.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MeunierH.VauclairJ. (2007). Hand preferences on unimanual and bimanual tasks in white-face capuchins. — Am. J. Primatol. 69: 1064-1069.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ParishA.R. (1996). Female relationships in bonobos (Pan paniscus) — evidence for bonding, cooperation, and female dominance in a male-philopatric species. — Hum. Nat. Interdisc. Biosoc. Perspect. 7: 61-96.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ParishA.R.de WaalF.B.M.HaigD. (2000). The other “closest living relative”: how bonobos (Pan paniscus) challenge traditional assumptions about females, dominance, intra- and intersexual interactions, and hominid evolution. — Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 907: 97-113.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ParkerS.KerrM.MarkowitzH.GouldJ. (1999). A survey of tool use in zoo gorillas. — In: The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans: comparative perspectives ( ParkerS.T.MitchellR.W., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PhillipsK.A.SherwoodC.C. (2005). Primary motor cortex asymmetry is correlated with handedness in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). — Behav. Neurosci. 119: 1701-1704.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PikaS.LiebalK.TomaselloM. (2005). The gestural repertoire of bonobos (Pan paniscus): flexibility and use. — Am. J. Primatol. 65: 39-61.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PollickA.S.de WaalF.B.M. (2007). Ape gestures and language evolution. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104: 8184-8189.

  • RillingJ.K.ScholzJ.PreussT.M.GlasserM.F.ErrangiB.V.BehrensT.E.J. (2012). Differences between chimpanzees and bonobos in neural systems supporting social cognition. — Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7: 369-379.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RosatiA.G.HareB. (2012). Chimpanzees and bonobos exhibit divergent spatial memory development. — Dev. Sci. 15: 840-853.

  • Savage-RumbaughE.S. (1984). Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes: contrast in preverbal communicative competence. — In: The pygmy chimpanzee: evolutionary biology and behavior ( SusmanR.L., ed.). Plenum Press, New York, NY, p.  395-413.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ShaferD.D. (1997). Hand preference behaviors shared by two groups of captive bonobos. — Primates 38: 303-313.

  • SpinozziG.CastorninaM.G.TruppaV. (1998). Hand preferences for unimanual and coordinated-bimanual tasks in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). — J. Comp. Psychol. 112: 183-191.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • StanfordC.B. (1998). The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos: empirical evidence and shifting assumptions. — Curr. Anthropol. 39: 399-420.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TablowoE.ForresterG.S. (2013). Stuctured bimanual actions and hand transfers reveal population-level right handedness in captive gorillas. — Anim. Behav. 86: 1049-1057.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TaglialatelaJ.P.Savage-RumbaughE.S.BakerL.A. (2003). Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus. — Int. J. Primatol. 24: 1-17.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TonookaR.MatsuzawaT. (1995). Hand preferences in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in simple reaching for food. — Int. J. Primatol. 16: 17-34.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TothN.SchickK.D.Savage-RumbaughE.S.SevcikR.A.RumbaughD.M. (1993). Pan the tool-maker: investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo (Pan paniscus). — J. Archaeol. Sci. 20: 81-91.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • UominiN.T. (2009). The prehistory of handedness: archeological data and comparative ethology. — J. Hum. Evol. 57: 411-419.

  • van EssenD.C. (1997). A tension-based theory of morphogenesis and compact wiring in the central nervous system. — Nature 385: 313-318.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • von LonsdorfE.HopkinsW.D. (2005). Wild chimpanzees show population level handedness for tool use. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102: 12634-12638.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WestergaardG.C.LussierI.D.HigleyJ.D. (2001). Between-species variation in the development of hand preference among macaques. — Neuropsychologia 39: 1373-1378.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WhitenA.GoodallJ.McGrewW.NishidaT.ReynoldsV.SugiyamaY.TutinC.WranghamR.BoeschC. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. — Behaviour 138: 1489-1525.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WhitenA.GoodallJ.McGrewW.C.NishidaT.ReynoldsV.SugiyamaY.TutinC.E.G.WranghamR.W.BoeschC. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. — Nature 399: 682-685.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WonY.-J.HeyJ. (2005). Divergence population genetics of chimpanzees. — Mol. Biol. Evol. 22: 297-307.

  • YousryT.A.SchmidU.D.AlkadhiH.SchmidtD.PeraudA.BuettnerA.WinklerP. (1997). Localization of the motor hand area to a knob on the precentral gyrus. A new landmark. — Brain 120: 141-157.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Content Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 198 150 11
Full Text Views 118 112 1
PDF Downloads 5 3 0