Abstract
High predation risk during development induces phenotypic changes in animals. However, little is known about how these plastic responses affect signalling and competitiveness during contests. Herein, we have studied the consequences of anti-predator plasticity during the intra-sexual competition of Pelvicachromis taeniatus, a cichlid fish with mutual mate choice. We staged contests between adult size-matched siblings of the same sex derived from different environments: one fish was regularly exposed to conspecific alarm cues since the larval stage (simulating predator presence), the other fish to control conditions. Rearing environment did not affect the winner of contests or total aggression within a fight. However, contest behaviour differed between treatments. The effects were especially pronounced in alarm cue-exposed fish that lost a contest: they generally displayed lower aggression than winners but also lower aggression than losers of the control treatment. Thus, perceived predator presence modulates intra-sexual competition behaviour by increasing the costs associated with fighting.
1. Introduction
Animal contests during intra-sexual competition are a central aspect of sexual selection (Earley & Hsu, 2013; McCullough et al., 2016). These contests often follow ritualized behavioural sequences whose underlying principles are similar across diverse taxa (Archer & Huntingford, 1994): contests start with the exchange of low-cost aggressive behaviours such as displays that signal individual strength. If individuals greatly differ in competitiveness, then the weaker contestant will often withdraw at this early stage. Otherwise, the fight will escalate and include overt aggression such as attacks that can cause severe injuries. At this stage, the individual that is able to maintain its level of aggression is predicted to win the fight by the war-of attrition model (Maynard-Smith, 1974). In contrast, the loser will stop displaying any aggressive behaviour, and instead show submissive behaviour and retreat so as to avoid further damage. Such fight patterns have been observed in multiple species, for example in cichlids (Jakobsson et al., 1979; Mosler, 1985), in lizards (Rand & Rand, 1976; Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 2011) and in crickets (Hofmann & Schildberger, 2001). Still, recent research revealed substantial variation in animal signalling during contests, and the underlying mechanisms remain a puzzle in biology (Field & Briffa, 2013; Tibbetts, 2014). Environmental variation may affect the perceived costs and benefits of signals and developmental plasticity has been suggested as a mechanism that breaks down signal reliability (Ingleby et al., 2010; Hunt & Hosken, 2014). Although contest behaviour has evolved in fluctuating natural environments, few studies have considered the effect of different developmental environments on contest behaviour later in life (Royle et al., 2005; Palaoro et al., 2017).
One of the best-studied cases of adaptive plasticity comes in the form of behavioural and morphological changes resulting from exposure to predator-related cues. Increased vigilances and the development of morphological defences are beneficial in the presence of predators but they may conflict with fitness-enhancing activities such as foraging, competition and mate choice, generating a trade-off between investment in anti-predator responses and other activities (Sih, 1980; Helfman, 1989; Houston et al., 1993; reviewed in Kishida et al., 2010). Accordingly, anti-predator plasticity leads to competitive disadvantages against non-plastic individuals in animal contests. Predator-exposed Rana sylvatica tadpoles are worse competitors (Relyea, 2002) and high-risk wild-caught damselfish (Pomacentrus amboinensis) have competitive disadvantages against low-risk individuals in a predator-free environment (Ferrari et al., 2019). This trade-off is modulated by cognitive rules that follow the threat-sensitivity hypothesis. This hypothesis postulates that the level of predation risk alters the costs and benefits of fitness-related behaviours. Thus, with increasing risk, animals should invest more in costly anti-predator strategies and less into other behaviours (Helfman, 1989).
Our first hypothesis is that individuals growing up in a predatory environment should be worse competitors and less likely to win intra-sexual contests. Second, following the threat-sensitivity hypothesis, we hypothesize that growing up in the presence of alarm cues should alter the costs and benefits of fighting, leading to a lower investment in contests. To test these hypotheses, we study the effect of a predatory developmental environment on fighting behaviour during intra-sexual competition. Our study species is Pelvicachromis taeniatus, a small, socially monogamous Western African river cichlid with mutual mate choice and intra-sexual competition in both sexes (Thünken et al., 2011, Baldauf et al., 2011) that responds with adaptive plasticity to perceived predation risk (Meuthen et al., 2016, 2018a).
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Experimental fish
In 2007 we collected adult P. taeniatus from the Moliwe river (Cameroon, 04°04′N, 09°16′E) as a breeding stock. P. taeniatus becomes sexually mature at an age of approximately one year and lives up to six years. In 2012, 12 different adult F1 pairs were bred to generate the clutches used in this study. Clutches were then split into two equally-sized groups that were, from hatching onwards for five days a week over three years, exposed to two different chemical cues. These were either a control treatment of distilled water (‘control fish’) or chemical alarm cues derived from ground whole conspecifics (‘alarm cue-exposed fish’) in a concentration of 7.2 mg/l, which induces clear behavioural and morphological anti-predator responses in P. taeniatus (Meuthen et al., 2016, 2018a; Meuthen et al., 2018b) and in other fish (Chivers & Smith, 1994a). Conspecific alarm cues are innately recognized cues (Chivers & Smith, 1994a,b) and the effects of prolonged exposure to them mirror findings from natural water bodies where predators are present (Stabell & Lwin, 1997; Laforsch et al., 2006; Meuthen et al., 2019b). Also, in contrast to predator odours, fish do not appear to habituate to conspecific alarm cues even after repeated exposure (Imre et al., 2016). In 2015, three-year-old adult fish derived from this split-clutch design were then tested in intra-sexual competition trials where alarm cues were absent.
2.2. Competition experiment
Preliminary trials revealed that the presence of alarm cues inhibits any competitive interaction between P. taeniatus. Theoretically, the absence of alarm cues during trials may represent a predator-free environment and thereby alter the behavioural response. However, following the risk allocation hypothesis, not only current risk but also the proportion of time at high risk is what shifts individual decision-making to a focus on anti-predator responses (Lima & Bednekoff, 1999; Ferrari et al., 2009). This hypothesis has been confirmed in many empirical studies on fish, ranging from the cyprinid Pimephales promelas (Meuthen et al., 2019a,b) to P. taeniatus (Meuthen et al., 2016). Here, we staged contests between siblings of the same sex that were raised in different treatments; hence individual contests were always between an alarm cue-exposed fish and a control fish. Because alarm cue exposure did not affect individual morphology in our fish at the age of testing as outlined in Meuthen et al. (2018a), we size-matched fish to a <5% total length difference so as to mitigate size-based contest resolution (sensu O’Connor et al., 2015). As there was substantial between-family variation in body size, only by using siblings we were able to obtain fish similar in body size. Moreover by using siblings, we were able to control for genetic effects that may lead to differences in competitiveness. To ensure that fish competed with each other in a context of sexual reproduction and to minimise inter-individual variation in behaviour arising from differences in social status (‘winner/loser effects’), we isolated individual fish for one week prior to trials and stimulated them (by placing smaller tanks in front of the isolation tanks) daily first for 15 minutes with a fish of the same sex and thereafter for 15 minutes with a fish from the opposite sex (see e.g. Thünken et al., 2014). Then, fish were transferred to the experimental set-up, which consisted of a 20 × 30 × 20 cm (length × width × height) tank that was separated into two equally sized compartments (20 × 15 × 20 cm each) by a retractable grey opaque plastic sheet and acclimated overnight. Afterwards, we removed the sheets via a pulley-rope system and allowed fish to freely interact with each other; we recorded interactions between competitors for a period of up to three hours (QuickCam 9000, Logitech, Suzhou, China). The time until winners and losers were determined differed between pairs (median, interquartile range (IQR): 731.0, IQR 393.5–1354.0 s), but the treatment of the winning fish did not affect this duration (independent Wilcoxon test,
2.3. Data analysis
A naïve observer scored individual fish behaviour until 5 minutes after the competition was resolved (as indicated by one fish assuming typical loser behaviour where they turn black, fold their fins and behave submissively by not retaliating towards the other fish, see Enquist & Jakobsson, 1986; Barlow, 2000). Agonistic behaviours were scored as displays or attacks as defined in previous studies of cichlids (Jakobsson et al., 1979; Enquist et al., 1990; Barlow, 2000). Afterwards, we calculated the number of displays and attacks per minute because as opposed to analysing raw behaviours, this allowed us to statistically compare behavioural frequencies between contests of unequal length. Moreover, we identified winner and loser identity (with the ‘loser’ being the individual that turns black, folds its fins, behaves submissively and does not retaliate against further attacks by the ‘winner’ for a period of 5 minutes).
2.3.1. Statistical analysis
Male and female competitions did not differ in the frequency of lateral displays (independent Wilcoxon test,

(a) Displays and (b) attacks per minute (mean ± SE) during P. taeniatus intra-sexual competition. Competing fish were raised either under perceived predator presence (alarm cue-exposed) or in a control environment (distilled water). Shown is the frequency of attacks (butts, nips, tail-beats, approaches) for alarm cue-exposed (dashed bars) and control (open bars) winners and losers, respectively. Asterisks denote statistically relevant differences and the topmost line in each figure indicates the significance of the interaction. **
Citation: Behaviour 156, 15 (2019) ; 10.1163/1568539X-00003577
3. Results
Treatment did not significantly affect the proportion of fights won (alarm cue-exposed fish won 14 times in 31 staged competitions, exact binomial test,
4. Discussion
To determine the consequences of anti-predator phenotypic plasticity in animal contests, we staged fights between same-sexed pairs of adult size-matched sibling P. taeniatus that were obtained from different environments. Within each dyad, one fish was exposed to high perceived risk (i.e., alarm cues) during development and the other fish to a control developmental environment. In contrast to the expected costs associated with anti-predator plasticity, the alarm cue developmental environment did not directly cause competitive disadvantages that influenced the outcome of intra-sexual contests. However, fish that grew up in the presence of alarm cues differed from control fish in their contest behaviour. This effect was especially pronounced in alarm cue-exposed fish that lost: they displayed less agonistic behaviour than winners from both treatments but also lower aggression than losers of the control group. In contrast, fish from the control treatment behaved the same independent of whether they won or lost.
Our finding that alarm cue-exposed losers displayed and attacked their opponents less than winners is consistent with the war-of-attrition model, which predicts that the loser of a contest is the individual that is unable to match the display and attack frequency of its opponent (Maynard-Smith, 1974), a pattern that has also been shown in many empirical studies (Rand & Rand, 1976; Jakobsson et al., 1979; Mosler, 1985; Hofmann & Schildberger, 2001; Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 2011). This is related to the energetic and injury-related costs associated with performing displays and attacks, which makes the maximum frequency of these behaviours a reliable indicator of individual quality and strength. The cost of these signals are further increased in predatory environments because conspicuous signallers (Zuk & Kolluru, 1998) and exhausted individuals (Yachi, 1995) are more prone to attacks by predators. Consequently, exposure to alarm cues during development appears to have modulated the perceived costs of fighting in accordance with the threat-sensitivity hypothesis (Helfman, 1989). In predatory environments, low-quality fish that eventually emerge as losers are unable to compensate for the increase in conspicuousness to predators that is associated with an increased display frequency (Zuk & Kolluru, 1998). This is because the lower energy reserves of low-quality individuals cause them to easily become exhausted during a contest. In a predatory environment, where exhausted prey are at high risk to be eaten (Yachi, 1995), such low-quality individuals face substantial predation risk when participating in a prolonged contest. Hence, the aim of low-quality losers should be to minimise competition-related costs by reduced investment into aggressive behaviour frequency and a focus on early disengagement. Alternatively, the high cost associated with fighting in a predatory environment forces losers to spend more energy on accurate sensory perception during the fight. This should cause a more accurate assessment about their own subpar and their opponent’s superior fighting capabilities, leading to a quick fight resolution as soon as minimal differences between individual abilities are detected.
In our control environment, high-quality winners and low-quality losers did not differ in their behavioural frequencies. Following our hypothesis, the absence of predation risk reduces the costs of fighting and thereby allows losing P. taeniatus to take more risks during contests. At low predation risk, low-quality individuals should invest more energy into the fight as an attempt to win even when the probability to do so is low. Moreover, at low risk levels, losers may decide to invest more energy into repeating aggressive behaviours and less into sensory systems. This comes at the cost of a less accurate assessment of their own and their opponent’s fighting capabilities.
At first glance, our results appear contrary to the finding that in the presence of predators, male sticklebacks of poor condition terminally invest into larger sexual ornaments than high-condition males (Candolin, 1999). However, we propose that the consequences of a predatory developmental environment differ from those of acute predator presence during trials (Candolin, 1999). At the same time, the results of our experiment match previous studies suggesting that losers display fewer agonistic behaviours during intra-sexual competition only when wild-caught fish, which likely experienced some degree of predation, are studied (Roy & Bhat, 2015) while this does not appear to be the case for lab-reared fish that have never experienced predation (Leiser et al., 2004). This may reflect how previous exposure to predation risk increases the perceived costs of fighting and thereby modulates loser behaviour. Hence, future animal contest research should consider incorporating previous exposure to predation risk in their study design as this may improve our knowledge about the evolution of animal signals.
Alternatively, our results can be interpreted in a signal honesty context. Animal signals do not evolve to make information transfer accurate, but instead in a direction that maximises fitness benefits for the signaller (Dawkins & Krebs, 1978; Krebs & Dawkins, 1984). Likewise, this should lead to the evolution of an optimal level of deceit in displays during animal contests (Bond, 1989). By exaggerating their quality when fighting against an opponent of similar or higher quality, dishonest individuals can prolong and escalate the fight and eventually win at a cost of increased exhaustion and damage. When two individuals of similar quality compete, dishonest exaggerated signalling by only one individual may appear to be able to decide a fight. However, in these circumstances, both individuals will evolve to signal dishonestly (Maynard-Smith & Parker, 1976), which escalates the fight and eventually decides the outcome by conventional means at the cost of both individuals being more exhausted. However, in a predatory environment, the costs of dishonest signalling are exacerbated. Conspicuous signallers and exhausted prey are subject to higher predation risk (see above), which should select against dishonest signalling. High-quality individuals have greater energy reserves and more muscle mass, which allows them to escape from predators even after a fight. Instead, low-quality individuals cannot do so and hence are unable to compensate for the increase in predation risk caused by dishonestly exaggerated signalling. Consistent with this alternative hypothesis, we found that alarm cue-exposed losers (which are low-quality fish) displayed and attacked less often than winners (which are high-quality fish). However, in control fish, losers (low-quality fish) did not show different frequencies of displays and attacks from winners (high-quality fish); control losers displayed almost the same frequencies of behaviours as winners, hinting at dishonestly exaggerated behavioural frequencies. Alternatively, the reduced frequencies of contest behaviour in alarm cue-exposed losers constitute a more honest signal as they are incapable of winning and thus have nothing to gain by an increased investment into exaggerated signalling. In other words, alarm cue-exposed losers did more of what is expected of losers whereas this was not the case for control fish. Nevertheless, signal honesty appears to be maintained only in alarm cue-exposed losers, suggesting that predation may be an evolutionary driver of signal honesty due to the elevated predation risk associated with signalling.
Corresponding author’s e-mail address: dmeuthen@evolution.uni-bonn.de
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Ethan Clotfelter and two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments which substantially improved the manuscript. This research was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) (BA 2885/5-1, ME 4974/1-1, ME 4974/2-1, TH 1615/1-1, TH 1615/3-1).
References
Archer, J. & Huntingford, F. (1994). Game theory models and escalation of animal fights. — In: The dynamics of aggression: biological and social processes in dyads and groups (Potegal, M. & Knutson, J.F., eds). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Upper Saddle River, NJ, p. 3-32.
Baldauf, S.A., Kullmann, H., Bakker, T.C.M. & Thünken, T. (2011). Female nuptial coloration and its adaptive significance in a mutual mate choice system. — Behav. Ecol. 22: 478-485.
Barlow, G.W. (2000). The cichlid fishes. — Perseus Publishing, Cambridge.
Bond, A.B. (1989). Toward a resolution of the paradox of aggressive displays: I. Optimal deceit in the communication of fighting ability. — Ethology 81: 29-46.
Bradbury, J.W. & Vehrencamp, S.L. (2011). Conflict resolution. — In: Principles of animal communication (Bradbury, J.W. & Vehrencamp, S.L., eds). Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA, p. 421-465.
Candolin, U. (1999). The relationship between signal quality and physical condition: is sexual signalling honest in the three-spined stickleback? — Anim. Behav. 58: 1261-1267.
Chivers, D.P. & Smith, R.J.F. (1994a). Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, acquire predator recognition when alarm substance is associated with the sight of unfamiliar fish. — Anim. Behav. 48: 597-605.
Chivers, D.P. & Smith, R.J.F. (1994b). The role of experience and chemical alarm signaling in predator recognition by fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas. — J. Fish Biol. 44: 273-285.
Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J.R. (1978). Animal signals: information or manipulation. — In: Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach (Krebs, J.R. & Davies, N.B., eds). Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, p. 282-309.
Earley, R.L. & Hsu, Y. (2013). Contest behaviour in fishes. — In: Animal contests (Hardy, C.W. & Briffa, M., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 199-227.
Enquist, M. & Jakobsson, S. (1986). Decision-making and assessment in the fighting behavior of Nannacara anomala (Cichlidae, Pisces). — Ethology 72: 143-153.
Enquist, M., Leimar, O., Ljungberg, T., Mallner, Y. & Segerdahl, N. (1990). A test of the sequential assessment game: fighting in the cichlid fish Nannacara anomala. — Anim. Behav. 40: 1-14.
Ferrari, M.C.O., Sih, A. & Chivers, D.P. (2009). The paradox of risk allocation: a review and prospectus. — Anim. Behav. 78: 579-585.
Ferrari, M.C.O., Warren, D.T., McCormick, M.I. & Chivers, D.P. (2019). The cost of carryover effects in a changing environment: context-dependent benefits of a behavioural phenotype in a coral reef fish. — Anim. Behav. 149: 1-5.
Field, S.A. & Briffa, M. (2013). Human contests: evolutionary theory and the analysis of interstate war. — In: Animal contests (Hardy, C.W. & Briffa, M., eds). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 321-334.
Helfman, G.S. (1989). Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish-trumpetfish interactions. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: 47-58.
Hofmann, H.A. & Schildberger, K. (2001). Assessment of strength and willingness to fight during aggressive encounters in crickets. — Anim. Behav. 62: 337-348.
Houston, A.I., McNamara, J.M. & Hutchinson, J.M.C. (1993). General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation. — Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 341: 375-397.
Hunt, J. & Hosken, D.J. (2014). Genotype-by-environment interactions and sexual selection. — Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester.
Imre, I., Di Rocco, R.T., Brown, G.E. & Johnson, N.S. (2016). Habituation of adult sea lamprey repeatedly exposed to damage-released alarm and predator cues. — Environ. Biol. Fish. 99: 613-620.
Ingleby, F.C., Hunt, J. & Hosken, D.J. (2010). The role of genotype-by-environment interactions in sexual selection. — J. Evol. Biol. 23: 2031-2045.
Jakobsson, S., Radesäter, T. & Järvi, T. (1979). On the fighting behaviour of Nannacara anomala (Pisces, Cichlidae) ♂♂. — Z. Tierpsychol. 49: 210-220.
Kishida, O., Trussell, G.C., Mougi, A. & Nishimura, K. (2010). Evolutionary ecology of inducible morphological plasticity in predator-prey interaction: toward the practical links with population ecology. — Popul. Ecol. 52: 37-46.
Krebs, J.R. & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: mind reading and manipulation. — In: Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach, 2nd edn. (Krebs, J.R. & Davies, N.B., eds). Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, p. 380-402.
Laforsch, C., Beccara, L. & Tollrian, R. (2006). Inducible defenses: the relevance of chemical alarm cues in Daphnia. — Limnol. Oceanogr. 51: 1466-1472.
Leiser, J.K., Gagliardi, J.L. & Itzkowitz, M. (2004). Does size matter? Assessment and fighting in small and large size-matched pairs of adult male convict cichlids. — J. Fish Biol. 64: 1339-1350.
Lima, S.L. & Bednekoff, P.A. (1999). Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. — Am. Nat. 153: 649-659.
Maynard-Smith, J. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. — J. Theor. Biol. 47: 209-221.
Maynard-Smith, J. & Parker, G.A. (1976). Logic of asymmetric contests. — Anim. Behav. 24: 159-175.
McCullough, E.L., Miller, C.W. & Emlen, D.J. (2016). Why sexually selected weapons are not ornaments. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 31: 742-751.
Meuthen, D., Baldauf, S.A., Bakker, T.C.M. & Thünken, T. (2016). Predator-induced neophobia in juvenile cichlids. — Oecologia 181: 947-958.
Meuthen, D., Baldauf, S.A., Bakker, T.C.M. & Thünken, T. (2018a). Neglected patterns of variation in phenotypic plasticity: age- and sex-specific antipredator plasticity in a cichlid fish. — Am. Nat. 191: 475-490.
Meuthen, D., Flege, P., Brandt, R. & Thünken, T. (2018b). The location of damage-released alarm cues in a cichlid fish. — Evol. Ecol. Res. 19: 529-546.
Meuthen, D., Ferrari, M.C.O., Lane, T. & Chivers, D.P. (2019a). High background risk induces risk allocation rather than generalized neophobia in the fathead minnow. — Behav. Ecol. 30: 364-371.
Meuthen, D., Ferrari, M.C.O., Lane, T. & Chivers, D.P. (2019b). Plasticity of boldness: high perceived risk eliminates a relationship between boldness and body size in fathead minnows. — Anim. Behav. 147: 25-32.
Mosler, H.J. (1985). Making the decision to continue the fight or to flee: an analysis of contests between male Haplochromis burtoni (Pisces). — Behaviour 92: 129-145.
O’Connor, C.M., Reddon, A.R., Ligocki, I.Y., Hellmann, J.K., Garvy, K.A., Marsh-Rollo, S.E., Hamilton, I.M. & Balshine, S. (2015). Motivation but not body size influences territorial contest dynamics in a wild cichlid fish. — Anim. Behav. 107: 19-29.
Palaoro, A.V., Velasque, M., Santos, S. & Briffa, M. (2017). How does environment influence fighting? The effects of tidal flow on resource value and fighting costs in sea anemones. — Biol. Lett. 13: 20170011.
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., Heisterkamp, S., Van Willigen, B. & EISPACK authors, R-core (2018). nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effect models. — R package version 3.1-131.1, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
R Core Team (2016). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. — R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
Rand, W.M. & Rand, A.S. (1976). Agonistic behavior in nesting iguanas: a stochastic analysis of dispute settlement dominated by the minimization of energy cost. — Z. Tierpsychol. 40: 279-299.
Relyea, R.A. (2002). Competitor-induced plasticity in tadpoles: consequences, cues, and connections to predator-induced plasticity. — Ecol. Monogr. 72: 523-540.
Roy, T. & Bhat, A. (2015). Can outcomes of dyadic interactions be consistent across contexts among wild zebrafish? — Roy. Soc. Open Sci. 2: 150282.
Royle, N.J., Lindström, J. & Metcalfe, N.B. (2005). A poor start in life negatively affects dominance status in adulthood independent of body size in green swordtails Xiphophorus helleri. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 272: 1917-1922.
Sih, A. (1980). Optimal behavior: can foragers balance two conflicting demands? — Science 210: 1041-1043.
Stabell, O.B. & Lwin, M.S. (1997). Predator-induced phenotypic changes in crucian carp are caused by chemical signals from conspecifics. — Environ. Biol. Fishes 49: 145-149.
Thünken, T., Baldauf, S.A., Kullmann, H., Schuld, J., Hesse, S. & Bakker, T.C.M. (2011). Size-related inbreeding preference and competitiveness in male Pelvicachromis taeniatus (Cichlidae). — Behav. Ecol. 22: 358-362.
Thünken, T., Bakker, T.C.M. & Baldauf, S.A. (2014). “Armpit effect” in an African cichlid fish: self-referent kin recognition in mating decisions of male Pelvicachromis taeniatus. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 68: 99-104.
Tibbetts, E.A. (2014). The evolution of honest communication: integrating social and physiological costs of ornamentation. — Integr. Comp. Biol. 54: 578-590.
Yachi, S. (1995). How can honest signalling evolve? The role of handicap principle. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 262: 283-288.
Zuk, M. & Kolluru, G.R. (1998). Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. — Q. Rev. Biol. 73: 415-438.