Abstract
Problem solving in animals is often studied by measuring an animal’s ability to solve man-made puzzles, such as puzzle feeders. Outside of scientific studies, puzzle feeders are also presented to captive animals housed in zoos, aquaria, and sanctuaries as a form of enrichment. Footage of these interactions is commonly posted on social media accounts to increase public engagement with the institution. However, because these puzzle feeders are not presented to animals for research purposes, the problem-solving abilities of numerous species may not be recorded in the scientific literature. Therefore, we searched through three social media platforms (Facebook, Instagram and X) for footage of captive animals solving problems to determine if social media could be a useful tool for behavioural scientists. For each video, we recorded what species was featured, what the puzzle was, how it was solved, and whether this was the first documentation of problem solving for the species (to the best of our knowledge). We found 111 records of successful problem solving across 74 species, including mammals (
1. Introduction
Problem-solving ability is a growing area of animal behaviour and cognition research, largely because it is expected to be important for an animal’s survival (Cole & Quinn, 2012). Problem solving is defined as an animal’s ability to move itself or an object to overcome a barrier and access a reward (Rowell et al., 2021). Animals can solve problems innovatively (using a new behaviour or an existing behaviour in a new context; Reader & Laland, 2003), by using a learned behaviour (Anderson, 1993) or accidentally through trial and error (Tecwyn et al., 2012).
While there are often differences in problem-solving abilities documented between species (Benson-Amram et al., 2016), the problem-solving abilities of the majority of species have not been documented. This is partially due to the challenges of conducting behavioural studies on many species of animals (Rowe & Healy, 2014). For example, for wild-living animals, studying problem solving in the wild has logistical difficulties (e.g., expensive to travel and difficult to access habitat; Desai & Potter, 2006) or the nature of the animals themselves may make research difficult (e.g., rare, elusive, occupy large territories; Pritchard et al., 2016). Similarly, it can be expensive and require many resources (e.g., facilities, funding, staff) for researchers to establish captive colonies of animals, particularly large-bodied or long-lived animals, or animals with specialised requirements. Therefore, it is often more practical to study already established captive populations of animals, such as those in zoos.
Properly managed and accredited zoos, aquaria, gardens, and sanctuaries (hereafter referred to as ‘zoos’) allow species of wild animals to be closely observed by people in an environment designed to facilitate natural behaviours (Rabin, 2003). These institutions generally provide their animals with enrichment items or tasks, including puzzle feeders (e.g., a ball to roll around to release pieces of food) and foraging challenges (e.g., finding food hidden throughout enclosure) to prevent boredom and encourage a wider, and more natural, range of behaviours (Meehan & Mench, 2007). Problem-solving behaviours of the animals during these activities is therefore commonly observed by staff members and is easy to record. This often-entertaining footage is shared with the public on social media to increase public engagement and highlight the positive actions the institution is taking to care for its animals. For example, many zoos provide holiday-themed enrichment items to their animals, and post footage of this to encourage visitation over the holiday season (e.g., Cincinnati Zoo & Botanical Garden: www.facebook.com/photo/?fbid=744568294360269&set=a.639373024879797). However, these records have previously not been considered as evidence of problem solving by the scientific community because the footage was not obtained as part of a formal experimental design. Consequently, the scientific literature may have missed many species that can, and do, solve problems.
Therefore, we aimed to gauge whether social media is a viable resource for obtaining evidence of problem solving in captive animals. It is relatively easy to locate problem solving footage on social media, so we searched social media accounts on Facebook, Instagram and X (formerly Twitter) for records of animals successfully solving problems. Our intent was not to do an exhaustive search of all available social media accounts. Rather, we aimed to explore the potential for social media to be an untapped repository of observations of new species being recorded solving puzzles. As this was an exploratory survey, we made no a priori predictions on patterns of problem solving or species occurrences.
2. Methods
We searched social three media platforms (Facebook, Instagram, and X) for accredited zoo accounts or posts tagged with key words including ‘zoo enrichment’, ‘animal enrichment’, and ‘animals solving puzzles’. Due to the large extent of content available, only posts from August 2021 until August 2023 were considered. To broaden our search, we also searched through the last two years of posts on accounts/pages of the top zoos in the world (found using Google searches) and zoos that were prominent on social media.
We watched all videos in these search results. If an animal was recorded solving a problem, we recorded (1) where it was located (if known), (2) the species recorded, (3) the type of problem, (4) how the problem was solved and (5) whether this was the first account of problem solving in this species (to our knowledge), or where the species had previously been described solving a problem (literature search). Videos had to show the problem being solved, not just the animal interacting with a problem, to be considered. We could not extract any information about how long it took to solve the problem, or how the animals interacted with the problem (e.g., latency to approach, total time interacting) as (1) the videos were often edited and made up of multiple sequences clipped together to fit into the platform’s time limits, (2) we did not know how long the animal had access to the puzzle feeder before the video was recorded and (3) we did not know if this was the first instance the puzzle feeders were given to these individuals. We therefore only recorded whether the problem was successfully solved. In addition, we did not record the number of solutions possible in complex puzzles (e.g., puzzle boards with multiple food wells) as we could not necessarily tell this information from all the videos viewed (e.g., sometimes the animal’s body obscured the puzzle). Species in videos were identified by reading the post’s description and/or searching the zoo’s website for a species list. We used Google Scholar to search for previous scientific accounts of problem solving specifically for each species by searching the species name with ‘innovation’ or ‘problem solving’.
3. Results
We collected 111 records of animals solving problems on social media from 41 accredited institutions and multiple accounts of businesses/individuals (Table 1). Details on the location of the animals and a link to the video record can be found in Table A1 in the Appendix. The animals recorded included birds (13 species), mammals (57 species), and reptiles (4 species). Overall, to the best of our knowledge, these records include the first accounts of problem solving in 29 species (39% of records) across these three taxonomic groups (Table 1). Multiple videos showing the presentation of different puzzle types were found for 23 species and these videos therefore often showed different solving behaviours (e.g., pushing, pulling) being used within each species (Table 1).
4. Discussion
We searched through two years of posts on social media platforms to investigate whether social media was a viable resource for observing problem solving in captive animals. We documented records of problem solving in 74 species of mammals, birds, and reptiles using key word searches and by searching through prominent social media accounts, many of which had not been previously recorded. This suggests that there is value in monitoring social media platforms for problem solving studies. As a problem can be solved through trial and error learning (Tecwyn et al., 2012), an animal does not have to solve a problem spontaneously or innovatively to be considered capable of problem solving (Rowell et al., 2021). It is therefore not necessary to have a complete understanding of the individual’s behavioural history to document problem solving, and a recorded observation from an institution, such as a zoo, can still sufficiently demonstrate an individual’s ability to solve a problem. With this knowledge, researchers can then work with zoos to continue investigating problem solving in more detail in the species known to solve problems (e.g., individual variation, or consistency in solving over time). Alternatively, a more rigorous search of social media content could be undertaken through a citizen science project to find other records of problem solving, or to investigate other animal behaviour questions (e.g., whether behaviour towards a puzzle changes over time). Importantly, problem solving in captive animals should not be discounted as “ecologically irrelevant” because, while the problems presented may not be encountered in the wild, they still provide valuable insights on cognitive (e.g., learning, Aplin et al., 2013), behavioural (e.g., motivation, van Horik & Madden, 2016) and mechanical (e.g., range of motion, Rowell et al., 2021) processes that are inherent to the individual or species, as well as the capacity of the species, as a whole, to solve problems.
We found large differences in solving records between taxa, with higher numbers of mammals solving puzzle feeders (
Zoos present animals with foraging tasks to enhance their welfare and wellbeing, and this desktop survey found a large number of puzzle feeder records (
5. Conclusion
Overall, social media appears to be an untapped and useful resource for identifying problem solving in animals, particularly for mammals. This suggests that further work should be done with zoos and their staff to increase the number of species that are exposed to puzzle feeders, to understand the impact of puzzle feeders on animal welfare, and to encourage the posting of videos of ‘less popular’ species, which could further increase the new records of solving successes. This also shows the potential for community education and participation in research using social media. We argue that social media provides researchers with a cheap and easily accessible resource for documenting problem solving in animals and could provide a rich avenue for engaging in citizen science projects in the future.
Corresponding author’s e-mail address: misha.rowell@my.jcu.edu.au
Acknowledgement
The authors acknowledge the owners of the social media accounts referenced in this study.
References
Anderson, J.R. (1993). Problem solving and learning. — Am. Psychol. 10: 35-44.
Aplin, L.M., Sheldon, B.C. & Morand-Ferron, J. (2013). Milk bottles revisited: social learning and individual variation in the blue tit, Cyanistes caeruleus. — Anim. Behav. 85: 1225-1232. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.009.
Benson-Amram, S., Dantzer, B., Stricker, G., Swanson, E.M. & Holekamp, K.E. (2016). Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. — Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113: 2532-2537.
Bouchard, J., Goodyer, W. & Lefebvre, L. (2007). Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons (Columba livia). — Anim. Cogn. 10: 259-266.
Caicoya, A.L., Schaffer, A., Holland, R., von Fersen, L., Colell, M. & Amici, F. (2023). Innovation across 13 ungulate species: problem solvers are less integrated in the social group and less neophobic. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 290: 20222384. DOI:10.1098/rspb.2022.2384.
Cameron, R. & Rogers, L.J. (1999). Hand preference of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus): problem solving and responses in a novel setting. — J. Comp. Psychol. 113: 149-157. DOI:10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.149.
Clark, F.E., Gray, S.I., Bennett, P., Mason, L.J. & Burgess, K.V. (2019). High-Tech and tactile: cognitive enrichment for zoo-housed gorillas. — Front. Psychol. 10: 1574. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01574.
Clavel, M.G.G., Youngblood, M. & Lahti, D. (2020). Relationship between personality and cognitive traits in domestic rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). — bioRxiv. DOI:10.1101/2020.10.12.336024.
Colbert-White, E.N., McCord, E.M., Sharpe, D.I. & Fragaszy, D.M. (2013). String-pulling behaviour in a Harris’s Hawk Parabuteo unicinctus. — Ibis 155: 611-615. DOI:10.1111/ibi.12040.
Cole, E.F. & Quinn, J.L. (2012). Personality and problem-solving performance explain competitive ability in the wild. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 279: 1168-1175. DOI:10.1098/rspb.2011.1539.
Cooper, T.L., Zabinski, C.L., Adams, E.J., Berry, S.M., Pardo-Sanchez, J., Reinhardt, E.M., Roberts, K.M., Watzek, J., Brosnan, S.F., Hill, R.L., Weigel, E.G. & Mendelson III, J.R. (2020). Long-term memory of a complex foraging task in Monitor lizards (Reptilia: Squamata: Varanidae). — J. Herpetol. 54: 378-383. DOI:10.1670/19-122.
Cunningham, C.L., Anderson, J.R. & Mootnick, A.R. (2006). Object manipulation to obtain a food reward in hoolock gibbons, Bunopithecus hoolock. — Anim. Behav. 71: 621-629. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.013.
Dabhelia, M. (2022). Decision-making during problem-solving and innovation in an urban dwelling population of yellow mongooses (Cynictis penicillata). Thesis, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg.
Desai, V. & Potter, R.B. (2006). Doing development research. — Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Dussutour, A., Deneubourg, J.-L., Beshers, S. & Fourcassié, V. (2009). Individual and collective problem-solving in a foraging context in the leaf-cutting ant Atta colombica. — Anim. Cogn. 12: 21-30.
Esch, L., Wöhr, C., Erhard, M. & Krüger, K. (2019). Horses’ (Equus caballus) laterality, stress hormones, and task related behavior in innovative problem-solving. — Animals 9: 265. DOI:10.3390/ani9050265.
Foerder, P., Galloway, M., Barthel, T., Iii, D.E.M. & Reiss, D. (2011). Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant. — PLoS ONE 6: e23251. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0023251.
Gajdon, G.K., Fijn, N. & Huber, L. (2006). Limited spread of innovation in a wild parrot, the kea (Nestor notabilis). — Anim. Cogn. 9: 173-181. DOI:10.1007/s10071-006-0018-7.
Harrington, K.J., Folkertsma, R., Auersperg, A.M.I., Biondi, L. & Lambert, M.L. (2023). Innovative problem solving by wild falcons. — Curr. Biol. 34: 190-195.e3. DOI:10.1016/j.cub.2023.10.061.
Hauser, M.D., Kralik, J. & Botto-Mahan, C. (1999). Problem solving and functional design features: experiments on cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus oedipus. — Anim. Behav. 57: 565-582.
Hopper, L.M., Price, S.A., Freeman, H.D., Lambeth, S.P., Schapiro, S.J. & Kendal, R.L. (2014). Influence of personality, age, sex, and estrous state on chimpanzee problem-solving success. — Anim. Cogn. 17: 835-847. DOI:10.1007/s10071-013-0715-y.
Howard, M. (2018). Socialization and problem-solving in domestic cats (Felis catus). Thesis, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN.
Jønsson, K.A., Fabre, P.H. & Irestedt, M. (2012). Brains, tools, innovation and biogeography in crows and ravens. — BMC Evol. Biol. 12: 72. DOI:10.1186/1471-2148-12-72.
Kittler, K., Kappeler, P.M. & Fichtel, C. (2018). Instrumental problem-solving abilities in three lemur species (Microcebus murinus, Varecia variegata, and Lemur catta). — J. Comp. Psychol. 132: 306-314. DOI:10.1037/com0000113.
Klump, B.C., Martin, J.M., Wild, S., Hörsch, J.K., Major, R.E. & Aplin, L.M. (2021). Innovation and geographic spread of a complex foraging culture in an urban parrot. — Science 373: 456-460. DOI:10.1126/science.abe7808.
Laumer, I.B., Call, J., Bugnyar, T. & Auersperg, A.M.I. (2018). Spontaneous innovation of hook-bending and unbending in orangutans (Pongo abelii). — Sci. Rep. 8: 16518. DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-34607-0.
Lee, J., Lee, C.-W., Kwon, H.-S., Kim, Y.-T., Park, C.-G., Kim, S.-J. & Kang, B.-C. (2008). Changes in food intake and abnormal behavior using a puzzle feeder in newly acquired sub-adult rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): a short term study. — Exp. Anim. 57: 433-437. DOI:10.1538/expanim.57.433.
Meehan, C.L. & Mench, J.A. (2007). The challenge of challenge: can problem solving opportunities enhance animal welfare? — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 102: 246-261. DOI:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.031.
Munteanu, A.M., Starnberger, I., Pašukonis, A., Bugnyar, T., Hödl, W. & Fitch, W.T. (2016). Take the long way home: behaviour of a neotropical frog, Allobates femoralis, in a detour task. — Behav. Process. 126: 71-75. DOI:10.1016/j.beproc.2016.03.009.
O’Connor, A.M., Burton, T.J., Leamey, C.A. & Sawatari, A. (2014). The use of the puzzle box as a means of assessing the efficacy of environmental enrichment. — J. Vis. Exp. 94: 52225. DOI:10.3791/52225.
Padrell, M., Amici, F., Córdoba, M.P. & Llorente, M. (2022). Cognitive enrichment in a social setting: assessing the use of a novel food maze in sanctuary-housed chimpanzees. — Primates 63: 509-524. DOI:10.1007/s10329-022-00996-0.
Parker, S.T. & Gibson, K.R. (1994). ‘Language’ and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative developmental perspectives. — Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Paul, E.S., Harding, E.J. & Mendl, M. (2005). Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. — Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29: 469-491. DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002.
Pesendorfer, M.B., Dickerson, S. & Dragoo, J.W. (2018). Observation of tool use in striped skunks: how community science and social media help document rare natural phenomena. — Ecosphere 9: e02484. DOI:10.1002/ecs2.2484.
Pritchard, D.J., Hurly, T.A., Tello-Ramos, M.C. & Healy, S.D. (2016). Why study cognition in the wild (and how to test it)? — J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 105: 41-55. DOI:10.1002/jeab.195.
Rabin, L.A. (2003). Maintaining behavioural diversity in captivity for conservation: natural behaviour management. — Anim. Welfare 12: 85-94. DOI:10.1017/S0962728600025409.
Reader, S.M. & Laland, K.N. (2003). Animal innovation. — Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Renner, E., Abramo, A.M., Karen Hambright, M. & Phillips, K.A. (2017). Insightful problem solving and emulation in brown capuchin monkeys. — Anim. Cogn. 20: 531-536. DOI:10.1007/s10071-017-1080-z.
Rowe, C. & Healy, S.D. (2014). Measuring variation in cognition. — Behav. Ecol. 25: 1287-1292. DOI:10.1093/beheco/aru090.
Rowell, M.K., Pillay, N. & Rymer, T.L. (2021). Problem solving in animals: proposal for an ontogenetic perspective. — Animals 11: 866. DOI:10.3390/ani11030866.
Sanders, K. & Fernandez, E.J. (2022). Behavioral implications of enrichment for golden lion tamarins: a tool for ex situ conservation. — J. Appl. Anim. Welfare Sci. 25: 214-223. DOI:10.1080/10888705.2020.1809413.
Schmelz, M., Duguid, S., Bohn, M. & Völter, C.J. (2017). Cooperative problem solving in giant otters (Pteronura brasiliensis) and Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinerea). — Anim. Cogn. 20: 1107-1114. DOI:10.1007/s10071-017-1126-2.
Scott, A.E. & Wiebers, T. (1996). Observational learning in sable ferrets (Mustela putorius furo). — J. Ark. Acad. Sci. 50: 143-144.
Smith, B.P. & Litchfield, C.A. (2010). How well do dingoes, Canis dingo, perform on the detour task? — Anim. Behav. 80: 155-162. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.017.
Snaddon, J.L., Turner, E.C. & Foster, W.A. (2008). Children’s perceptions of rainforest biodiversity: which animals have the lion’s share of environmental awareness? — PLoS ONE 3: e2579. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0002579.
Sol, D., Timmermans, S. & Lefebvre, L. (2002). Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds. — Animal Behav. 63: 495-502. DOI:10.1006/anbe.2001.1953.
Spring, S.E., Clifford, J.O. & Tomkoi, D.L. (1997). Effect of environmental enrichment devices on behaviors of single- and group-housed squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). — J. Am. Ass. Lab. Anim. Sci. 36: 72-75.
Stanton, L., Davis, E., Johnson, S., Gilbert, A. & Benson-Amram, S. (2017). Adaptation of the Aesop’s Fable paradigm for use with raccoons (Procyon lotor): considerations for future application in non-avian and non-primate species. — Anim. Cogn. 20: 1147-1152. DOI:10.1007/s10071-017-1129-z.
Swaisgood, R.R., White, A.M., Zhou, X., Zhang, H., Zhang, G., Wei, R., Hare, V.J., Tepper, E.M. & Lindburg, D.G. (2001). A quantitative assessment of the efficacy of an environmental enrichment programme for giant pandas. — Anim. Behav. 61: 447-457. DOI:10.1006/anbe.2000.1610.
Tecwyn, E.C., Thorpe, S.K. & Chappell, J. (2012). What cognitive strategies do orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) use to solve a trial-unique puzzle-tube task incorporating multiple obstacles? — Anim. Cogn. 15: 121-133.
Thornton, A. & Samson, J. (2012). Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats. — Anim. Behav. 83: 1459-1468. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.018.
Topál, J., Miklósi, Á. & Csányi, V. (1997). Dog–human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog. — Anthrozoös 10: 214-224. DOI:10.2752/089279397787000987.
van Horik, J.O. & Madden, J.R. (2016). A problem with problem solving: motivational traits, but not cognition, predict success on novel operant foraging tasks. — Anim. Behav. 114: 189-198. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.02.006.
van Zeeland, Y.R.A., Schoemaker, N.J., Ravesteijn, M.M., Mol, M. & Lumeij, J.T. (2013). Efficacy of foraging enrichments to increase foraging time in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus erithacus). — Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 149: 87-102. DOI:10.1016/j.applanim.2013.09.005.
Varracchio, C., Gatto, E., Bertolucci, C. & Lucon-Xiccato, T. (2024). Do captive fish need cognitive enrichment? A test with a puzzle feeder in guppies. — Ethology 130: e13442. DOI:10.1111/eth.13442.
Wagman, J.D., Lukas, K.E., Dennis, P.M., Willis, M.A., Carroscia, J., Gindlesperger, C. & Schook, M.W. (2018). A work-for-food enrichment program increases exploration and decreases stereotypies in four species of bears. — Zoo Biol. 37: 3-15. DOI:10.1002/zoo.21391.
Worsley, M.A. (2013). Problem-solving capabilities as a test of predator adaptability: a comparison of bobcats (Lynx rufus) and Canada lynx (Lynx canadensis). — Thesis, University of Reno, Reno, NV.