Abstract
It is widely thought that (i) Proto-Indo-European had *-mon-stem nominals formed by internal derivation from neuter *-men-stems and that (ii) these *-mon-stems were characterized by “amphikinetic” inflection, thus stressed full-grade of the root in their strong case forms (*R(é)-mon-). This paper challenges the latter claim, arguing that these forms instead had stem-final stress (*R(e)-món-). I adduce prosodic evidence in support of this alternative reconstruction from Lithuanian, Greek, Hittite, and above all Vedic Sanskrit, where the attested reflexes of these *-mon-stems consistently show stem-final stress. I then propose a new account of their root full-grade, which on this new reconstruction is phonologically irregular, since it surfaces in a pretonic syllable. I contend that this full-grade was synchronically transferred from their neuter *-men-stem bases. In this respect, internally derived *-mon-stems are shown to pattern morphophonologically with other reconstructible non-primary derivatives, which similarly acquire their root vocalism from their derivational bases.
1 Introduction
This paper is concerned with the reconstructible phonology of Proto-Indo-European (PIE) *-mon-stem nominals and their development into the IE daughter languages. More precisely, it focuses on the properties of those PIE *-mon-stems that are widely thought to be formed by internal derivation (ID)—viz., derivation marked only by changes in prosodic properties (i.e., word stress and/or ablaut), no overt (“external”) affixation—from PIE neuter *-men-stem nouns.1 The derivational relationship between these categories would under this view be reflected in attested nominal pairs like (1):2
(1)
*-men-stem (n.nom/acc.sg)
*-mon-stem
a.
Ved. bráhma
‘formulation’
Ved. brahmā́nam
‘formulator; priest’
(m.acc.sg)
b.
Ved. dhárma
‘foundation’
Ved. dharmā́nam
‘support(er)’
(m.acc.sg)
c.
Gk.
θῆμα ‘tomb’
Gk.
θημών ‘heap’
(m.nom.sg)
d.
Gk.
μνῆμα ‘remembrance’
Gk.
μνήμων ‘mindful’
(m/f.nom.sg)
e.
Lat. augmen
‘addition’
Lith. augmuõ
‘sprout’
(m.nom.sg)
Ved. ojmā́nam
‘strength’
(m.acc.sg)
Similarly, there is broad agreement in this literature about the inflectional patterns of PIE *-mon-stems of the type in (1). Under the widely accepted Erlangen Model (EM) of IE athematic nominal inflection, these *-mon-stems exhibited “amphikinetic” (AK) inflectional paradigms—i.e., stressed full-grade of the root and *o-grade of the suffix in the strong cases, and zero-grade of the root and suffix and stressed inflectional endings in the weak cases.3, 4
In this paper, I propose a new phonological reconstruction of PIE internally derived *-mon-stems like (1) (henceforth, ID *-mon-stems). The proposed reconstruction partially aligns with the AK inflection posited by EM, especially in the weak cases (see 7.2 below for discussion). The main claim advanced in this paper, however, deals with the strong case forms of PIE ID *-mon-stems. I argue that these nominals were characterized by stress on the stem-final syllable, in this respect diverging from the AK pattern hypothesized by EM.5 The contrast between these two reconstructions of the strong cases is represented for the PIE ancestor of Vedic dharmán- ‘support(er)’ from (1b) above in (2) below, where it can be observed that only the proposed reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems directly accounts for the consistent stem-final stress observed in Ved. dharmán- and other Vedic nouns belonging to this class (cf. section 3 below).6 I contend below that the reflexes of this class in the other IE daughter languages likewise support reconstructing stem-final stress for PIE itself and, in turn, that this fact provides decisive evidence against the AK reconstruction and in favor of the alternative proposal.
(2) Two reconstructions of the strong cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems:
a.
Amphikinetic
b.
Proposed
Vedic
nom.sg
*[dhér-mōn]
*[dher-m̄́on]
>
dharmā́
‘support(er)’
acc.sg
*[dhér-mon-ṃ]
*[dher-món-ṃ]
>
dharmā́ṇam
"
nom.pl
*[dhér-mon-es]
*[dher-món-es]
>
dharmā́ṇas
‘support(er)s’
The rest of this article is structured as follows. I begin in section 2 with a brief discussion of the nature of the prosodic evidence for ID *-mon-stems. The next several sections constitute a survey of the reflexes of PIE ID *-mon-stems in the four IE languages that provide evidence for their inherited stress patterns: Vedic Sanskrit in section 3; Lithuanian in section 4; Greek in section 5; and Anatolian in section 6. This survey culminates in section 7 with an overall assessment of this evidence for the reconstruction of the strong case forms of ID *-mon-stems. Having briefly discussed the evidence for the weak cases, I present a new prosodic reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stem whereby these nominals were characterized by paradigmatic root full-grade; in the strong cases, the stem-final stress pattern seen in (2b); and in most of the weak cases, the same zero-grade of the suffix (with */m/-deletion; see 7.2 below) and stressed inflectional endings expected on the AK reconstruction (e.g., gen.sg *[-n-é/ós]). Section 8 considers a possible theoretical objection to this new reconstruction—specifically, its full-grade root, which is phonologically unexpected in a pretonic syllable. I propose that this root vocalism was transferred from their derivational bases, neuter *-men-stems. In this respect, ID *-mon-stems are argued to parallel other types of non-primary derivatives, which as already observed Schindler (1975c:260) often exhibit phonological properties that appear to have been transferred from their derivational bases. Finally, I conclude in section 9 with some brief remarks on the morphophonology of PIE ID *-mon-stems and of PIE non-primary derivatives generally. Since previous analyses mismatch the phonological reconstruction in (2b), I discuss the properties of ID *-mon-stems that future analyses of this category must account for.
2 Evidence for word stress in ID *-mon-stems
As noted already in section 1, this paper is concerned specifically with the word-prosody of the subset of PIE *-mon-stems that stand in a derivational relationship with neuter *-men-stems—i.e., with ID *-mon-stems. In 2.1 I discuss two types of evidence from the IE daughter languages that inform the prosodic reconstruction of this PIE category. The remainder of this section deals with *-mon-stems that I argue are uninformative in this respect: compound *-mon-stems in 2.2; and PIE *h2ék̑mon- ‘stone; heaven’ in 2.3.
2.1 Sources for reconstructing word stress in ID *-mon-stems
The first type of IE evidence that bears upon the prosodic reconstructions of PIE ID *-mon-stems is the stress patterns observed in direct reflexes of this category—i.e., reflexes of *-mon-stems attested beside cognate neuter *-men-stems that (i) historically (and in some cases potentially also synchronically) served as their derivational bases, or (ii) whose bases are plausibly reconstructed for PIE by comparison. Examples of the former type were cited in (1). An example of the latter type is TB klyomo ‘noble’ (= TA klyom < *k̑leu-món-); the cognate neuter *-men-stem is not found in Tocharian, but is attested in YAv. sraoman- ‘hearing’ (< *k̑léu-men-) and with further suffixation in Indic and Germanic, Ved. śrómata- and OHG hliumunt ‘(good) reputation’ (< *k̑léu-mn̥-to-), on which basis it is possible to reconstruct a derivationally related pair PIE *k̑leu-món- n *k̑léu-men- ( *k̑leu- ‘hear’; cf. NIL: 425, LIV2: 334–335).
Also relevant for the prosodic reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems are deverbal and denominal *-mon-stems, which are generally held to have arisen by reanalysis of ID *-mon-stems.7 These formations are attested in a number of IE language branches (at least Anatolian, Celtic, Indo-Iranian, Italic, and Tocharian), which suggests that this reanalysis may have occurred already in PIE itself (cf. Yates 2020b:253–254). Given the poverty of word equations across IE language branches, however, it is also conceivable that it occurred independently in each of these branches (cf. Weiss 2017:387 n. 53).8 Which of these scenarios is correct is for present purposes immaterial; in either case, the stress patterns seen in these deverbal and denominal *-mon-stems would necessarily be based on the ID *-mon-stems from which the suffix was resegmented, and so indirectly testify to the prosodic properties of this category.
2.2 The prosody of compound *-mon-stems
In addition to the direct and indirect reflexes of ID *-mon-stems discussed in 2.1 above, there is robust evidence in the IE languages—especially in Greek (see further 5.2 below)—for exocentric compound adjectives (i.e., bahuvrīhis; BV s) with stem-final *-mon-. The second members (2Ms) of these compounds are in general segmentally identical to ID *-mon-stems and could thus in principle be analyzed as such. However, the standard view of these compounds is that the 2M is not an ID *-mon-stem per se, but rather a neuter *-men-stem stem that undergoes a morphophonological change as part of the compounding process, whence stem-final *-mon- (see, e.g., Brugmann 1906:233, 239, Wackernagel & Debrunner 1954:762, Risch 1974:52). Evidence for this view comes from the fact that, in both Greek and Vedic, there are numerous *-mon-stem BV s in which the 2M corresponds to an attested primary neuter *-men-stem stem, but is not independently attested (i.e., as a non-compound *-mon-stem)—e.g., (3). From such cases it can be inferred that neuter *-men-stems—not ID *-mon-stems—function as the input to compounding in *-mon-stem BV s.
(3) |
nom/acc.sg *[-mṇ] |
BV 2M *-mon- |
ID *-mon- |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a. |
Ved. |
kár-ma |
‘deed’ |
viśvá-karmāṇam |
‘having all deeds’ |
xkar-mán- |
||
b. |
Ved. |
ján-ma |
‘birth’ |
bhrā́jaj-janmānas |
‘having a brilliant birth’ |
xjan-mán- |
||
c. |
Ved. |
mán-ma |
‘thought’ |
dur-mánmānam |
‘having bad thoughts’ |
xman-mán- |
||
d. |
Gk. |
|
‘affair’ |
|
‘having many affairs’ |
x |
||
e. |
Gk. |
|
‘word’ |
|
‘having bad/evil words’ |
x |
||
f. |
Gk. |
|
‘form’ |
|
‘having different forms’ |
x |
According to EM, the apparent change in the shape of the stem seen in these compounds (n *-men- 2M *-mon-) is the result of ID—more specifically, of “a productive derivational pattern in Proto-Indo-European by which possessive compounds could be derived from proterokinetic neuters by introducing amphikinetic inflection” (Stüber 1998:144; cf. Widmer 2004:69–70, Schneider 2010:66, Fellner & Grestenberger 2016, i.a.).9 On this analysis, BV *-mon-stems are created by what is at least formally the same derivational process as ID *-mon-stems, in which case their prosodic properties might be relevant to the reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems.
Yet while the EM analysis of BV compounds has been widely adopted (e.g., Fortson 2010:137, Weiss 2020:282), its empirical basis is questionable. As pointed out by Kiparsky (2010:169–170) and by Lundquist (2021a:155–156), the only AK property that is observed in the attested IE reflexes of such BV s is *o-vocalism of the stem-final suffix in the strong cases;10 there is no evidence for AK stress mobility, either within the 2M of the compound (which also shows no trace of root or suffixal ablaut) or in the compound as a whole (assuming that “amphikinesis” in a compound would be equivalent to edge-to-edge mobility, i.e., word-initial stress in the strong cases and stressed inflectional endings in the weak).11 In Vedic, BV *-mon-stems are instead governed by the same principles of stress assignment as other BV s: the general rule is that the first member (1M) is stressed on the same syllable as when it occurs as an independent word.12 This rule is illustrated in (4). The contrast between (4d) and (4e) in particular shows that the position of stress depends on the accentual properties of the 1M of the compound.
(4)
1M
+
2M
BV *-mon-
a.
śúci-
‘brilliant’
ján-man-
‘birth’
śúci-janman-
‘having a brilliant birth’
b.
satyá-
‘true’
mán-man-
‘thought’
satyá-manman-
‘having true thoughts’
c.
áśman-
‘stone’
hán-man-
‘blow’
áśma-hanman-
‘having blows like stones’
d.
śatá
‘hundred’
yā́-man-
‘course’
śatá-yāman-
‘having a hundred courses’
e.
sahásra-
‘thousand’
yā́-man-
‘course’
sahásra-yāman-
‘having a thousand courses’
In Greek, on the other hand, all BV *-mon-stems exhibit “recessive accentuation” (RA): stress—phonetically realized as a high tone—is assigned to the leftmost mora within the “Law of Limitation” (LoL), a right edge stress window that developed in Proto-Greek; stress thus surfaces on the final vocalic mora of the word’s antepenultimate syllable if the final syllable is light (modulo final consonant extrametricality), otherwise on the initial vocalic mora of the penultimate syllable.13 For BV *-mon-stems with primary neuter *-men-stems as their 2M, RA is descriptively identical to root stress of the 2M in most case forms, including the m/f.nom.sg citation form, as evident in (3d–f) above. That root stress in these forms is the result of RA, however, is evident from paradigmatic forms like the n.nom/acc.sg (e.g.,
A historical explanation for the prosody of these compounds was outlined already by Wheeler (1885:39–55), who observed that Greek words regularly exhibit RA when maintaining their PIE stress patterns (generally reconstructed on the basis of Vedic) would have resulted in stress falling closer to the left edge of the word than was permitted by the LoL. If this generalization is correct, then the RA pattern seen in Greek BV *-mon-stems (and in most other BV stem types) can be derived from a Vedic-like PIE grammar in which BV s had 1M stress:15 since inherited 1M stress would consistently fall beyond the LoL in BV *-mon-stems, the emergence of RA in these compounds after the innovation of the LoL in Proto-Greek would be historically expected (cf. Lundquist 2016, 2017:70–71, 2021a).
The most economical account of the Greek and Vedic data is therefore that in PIE BV *-mon-stems were regularly stressed on their 1M—more precisely, on the same syllable of the 1M that bears stress when it occurs independently (cf. Brugmann 1905–1906:65, Lundquist & Yates 2018:2127–2128, i.a.).16 This situation is directly continued in Vedic, and is also reflected in Greek modulo the LoL. For present purposes, the major take-away from this discussion is that BV *-mon-stems are uninformative with respect to the position of stress in ID *-mon-stems. Even if it is the case that the 2M of these PIE compounds is formally equivalent to an ID *-mon-stem as assumed by EM (which is not assured; cf. n. 10 above), since the 1M determined their stress pattern as a whole, their reflexes provide no evidence as to whether the 2M had AK stress mobility, stem-final stress, or any other stress pattern as an independent word.
2.3 The relationship between PIE *h2ék̑mon- and ID *-mon-stems
In most previous scholarship, PIE *h2ék̑mon- ‘stone; heaven’ has been grouped together with ID *-mon-stems,17 and its numerous IE reflexes—e.g., (5a–f) below—used to reconstruct a single inflectional pattern associated with all PIE primary *-mon-stem nominals, viz., AK inflection (e.g., Gotō 2013:39–40, Meier-Brügger & Fritz 2021:223). Yet while the segmental identity of their stem-final elements (*[-mōn] in nom.sg, *[-mon-] in acc.sg and nom.pl) may suggest a connection at some historical stage, the comparative IE evidence does not support the assumption of a unified morphological category in PIE itself.
(5) Reflexes of PIE *h2ék̑mon- (cf. NIL: 287):
a. Vedic: acc.sg áśmānam, ins.sg áśnā / áśmanā ‘stone’18
b. Younger Avestan: acc.sg asmanəm, gen.sg ašnō ‘stone; heaven’19
c. Old Persian: acc.sg asmānam ‘heaven’
d. Greek: nom.sg
ἄκμων , gen.sgἄκμονος ‘anvil; heaven (Hesych.)’e. Old Lithuanian: nom.sg ãkmuo/akmuõ, gen.sg ãkmenes/akmenès ‘stone’
f. Latvian: nom.sg akmens, gen.sg akmens ‘stone’
The central morphological problem with treating *h2ék̑mon- as an ID *-mon-stem is the absence of a corresponding neuter *-men-stem from which it could be internally derived.20 This absence is not likely to be accidental. A more probable cause is that neuter *-men-stems were derived primarily from verbal roots (Fortson 2010:123, Lundquist & Yates 2018:2110, Weiss 2020:333–334, i.a.), but there is no known PIE verbal root from which such a *-men-stem could plausibly be derived, and in turn, a *-mon-stem via ID. The only established PIE root that is formally compatible with *h2ék̑mon- is *h2ek̑- ‘sharp, pointed’, which is the source of various IE nominal forms (e.g., Lith. aštrùs, Lat. ācer, OIr. aicher, TB akwatse ‘sharp’; Lat. acus ‘needle’; Gk.
In view of this evidence for a morphological distinction between *h2ék̑mon- and ID *-mon-stems in PIE, it would be unsurprising if they also exhibited different prosodic behavior, and in fact, this is precisely what is observed in the IE languages. The clearest evidence comes from Vedic and (Old) Lithuanian where—as will be seen in sections 3 and 4—the reflexes of ID *-mon-stems show a single stress pattern but those of *h2ék̑mon- show a different pattern. In Vedic, ID *-mon-stems like brahmán- ‘formulator; priest’ in (1a) consistently have stem-final stress in their strong cases, whereas áśman- ‘stone’ in (5a) has word-initial stress. Similarly in Lithuanian, nouns with nom.sg in -muo regularly show intraparadigmatic stress mobility, e.g., nom.sg raumuõ ‘muscle’ vs. dat raũmeniui; but while ‘stone’ conforms to this pattern in standard Lithuanian (nom.sg akmuõ vs. dat.sg ãkmeniui), in Old Lithuanian (Daukša) it shows almost exclusively fixed initial stress, e.g., nom.sg ãkmuo, gen.sg ãkmenes (cf. Senn 1966:139, Illič-Svityč 1979:49; see 4.2 below for further discussion).
The most straightforward explanation for these facts is that *h2ék̑mon- was prosodically distinct from ID *-mon-stems already in PIE, a difference which persisted into the attested IE languages. The fixed initial stress pattern observed in the Old Lithuanian and Vedic reflexes *h2ék̑mon- is matched by Greek
(6) |
PIE |
Greek |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
nom.sg |
*[h2ákmōn] |
> |
|
‘anvil’ |
|
acc.sg |
*[h2ákmon-ṃ] |
> |
|
” |
|
nom.pl |
*[h2ákmon-es] |
> |
|
‘anvils’ |
In contrast, I argue in section 7 below that ID *-mon-stems were in PIE characterized by stem-final stress in their strong cases just like their Vedic reflexes. It is impossible to exclude that PIE *h2ék̑mon- and ID *-mon-stems ultimately derive historically from a single prosodically uniform category, but if so, I take this category and its prosodic properties to be matters of internal reconstruction (i.e., pre-PIE).
3 ID *-mon-stems in Vedic Sanskrit
The most robust direct evidence for the reconstructible prosodic patterns of PIE ID *-mon-stems comes from Vedic Sanskrit. At least nine reflexes of primary neuter *-men-stem nouns are attested in the R̥gveda (RV) beside cognate *-mon-stems—in Vedic, masculine nouns—which are segmentally identical but differ prosodically. The existence of such pairs is economically explained under the view, widely accepted since Schindler (1975a:63–64), that historically they reflect the input and output of ID—i.e., that *-mon-stems derive from neuter *-men-stems by changing the prosodic properties of the latter (see further section 9 below). In addition, there are at least two Vedic masculine nouns that can be plausibly assumed to continue PIE ID *-mon-stems on the basis of comparative or historical evidence. As will become clear presently, the Vedic reflexes of these PIE ID *-mon-stems have stem-final stress without exception.
I begin with the *-mon-stems synchronically paired with primary neuter *-men-stems. This set can be divided into two groups: a first group in (7) in which a transparent semantic relationship obtains between neuter *-men-stem and *-mon-stem; and a second group in (8) which are related to one another formally in the same way but semantically do not conform to this pattern.
(7) |
nom/acc.sg *[-mṇ] |
acc.sg *[-món-ṃ] |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a. |
dā́ma |
‘gift’ |
dāmā́nam |
‘giver; giving’ |
||
b. |
dhárma |
‘support; ordinance’ |
dharmā́ṇam |
‘supporter; issuer of ordinances’ |
||
c. |
bráhma |
‘sacred formulation’ |
brahmā́ṇam |
‘formulator; priest’ |
||
d. |
sádma |
‘seat’ |
sadmā́nam |
‘sitter’ |
(8) |
nom/acc.sg *[-mṇ] |
acc.sg *[-món-ṃ] |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a. |
óma* |
‘aid’ |
omā́nam |
‘aid’ |
||
b. |
várima* |
‘expanse’ |
varimā́ṇam |
‘expanse’ |
||
c. |
várṣma* |
‘height’ |
varṣmā́ṇam |
‘height’ |
||
d. |
svā́dma |
‘sweetness’ |
svādmā́nam |
‘sweetness’ |
||
e. |
bhū́ma |
‘earth’ |
bhūmā́ṇam |
‘abundance’ |
In the first group, the ID *-mon-stem stem is generally a masculine agent noun, which denotes an entity that habitually/characteristically produces or employs the neuter deverbal (7a–c) result noun or (7d) instrument noun from which it is derived. Thus in (7c), for instance, m brahmán- ‘formulator; priest’ is the one who produces the n bráhman- ‘ritual formulation’ in his primary duty, the performance of the ritual. All of the *-mon-stems in (7) function as agent nouns in the RV; (7a) is also attested with event/result noun readings (‘giving, gift’; e.g., RV IV.42.2c).24 The fact that the meanings of the *-mon-stems are essentially predictable on the basis of their corresponding *-men-stems suggests that the historical relationship between them may be synchronically maintained, with the latter continuing to serve as the derivational base for the former.
This semantic relationship is not seen in the second group, however. In (8a–d), the neuter base and masculine derivative appear to be synonymous.25 In (8e), the relationship is opaque, presumably due to lexicalization of the neuter *-men-stem in the meaning ‘earth’; this change is confirmed by the semantic divergence seen also between this noun and the the Vedic root bhū- ‘become’, which historically was its base (viz., PIE *bhuhx-; cf. NIL: 47, LIV2: 98–99).
A third and final group consists of Vedic masculine *-mon-stem nouns for which a cognate neuter *-men-stem is not directly attested but is in all likelihood reconstructible, and which thus may reflect PIE ID *-mon-stems. This group includes at least two nouns attested in the RV, which are given in (9) along with evidence that supports the reconstruction of the n *-men-stem from which they were derived historically:26
(9) |
Evidence for n *-men-stem |
acc.sg *[-món-ṃ] |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a. |
Lat. augmen |
‘increase’ |
ojmā́nam |
‘strength’ |
||
TB auki |
‘increase’ |
|||||
b. |
Ved. dárīman- |
‘splitting’ |
darmā́ṇam |
‘splitter’ |
In the case of (9a), neuter *-men-stems derived from the same root (< PIE *h2eug-; see NIL: 328–329, LIV2: 274–275) are found in Latin and Tocharian.27 They support the reconstruction of a neuter *-men-stem *h2éug-men- from which *h2eug-món- (> Ved. ojmā́nam, Lith. augmuõ ‘sprout’; on the latter see 4.2 below) was formed by ID.28
For (9b) the neuter event noun Ved. dárīman- ‘splitting’ provides evidence for an erstwhile *dár-man-—i.e., the expected neuter *-men-stem from which the *-mon-stem reflected in Ved. darmā́nam was historically derived. This noun belongs to a small, idiosyncratic class of Vedic neuter nouns with stem-final -īman-. Just eight stems of this type are attested in the RV, collectively occurring twenty-seven times, only in the loc.sg (–īman-i or, less commonly, endingless -īman) or ins.pl (–īma-bhis). Per Wackernagel & Debrunner (1954:763) the core of this class consists of neuter *-men-stems built to PIE roots of the shape *(s)TeRH with irregular lengthening of the vocalic reflex of the root-final laryngeal (i.e., -ī-man- for expected *-i-man-)29—e.g., Ved. sávī-man- ‘impelling’ < PIE *séuh1-men-, stárī-man- ‘strewing’ < *stérh3-men-, hávī-man- ‘invoking’ < *g̑héuhx-men-.30 From this core were then built several new -īman-stems beside existing *-men-stems, clear examples of which are bhárīman- ‘support’ beside bhár-man- ‘id.’ (< *bhér-men-; cf. Gk.
The forms in (7–9) above exhaust the direct evidence for PIE ID *-mon-stems in Vedic. Prosodically, the Vedic reflexes of this category are effectively uniform: they exhibit paradigmatic full-grade of the root (in historical terms) and stem-final stress in their strong case forms.32 Both of these properties fit precisely with the reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems proposed in (2) above, but their stress pattern does not match the traditional AK reconstruction, which predicts stem-initial rather than stem-final stress in these forms.
Vedic also provides some indirect evidence for the reconstruction of PIE *-mon-stems. Vedic has a substantial class of masculine nouns that appear to be primary derivatives formed with a suffix *-mon-. These nouns are derived especially from adjectival roots that encode property concepts, e.g.: prathi-mán- ‘breadth’ (cf. pṛth-ú– ‘broad’), jari-mán- ‘old age’ (cf. jár-ant- ‘old’), mahi-mán- ‘greatness’ (cf. máhi- ‘big, great’), drāgh-mán- ‘length’ (cf. dīrghá- ‘long’).33 Just like the Vedic reflexes of ID *-mon-stems, these are consistently stressed on the stem-final syllable in their strong case forms.34 As discussed in 2.1 above, it is generally assumed that nominals formed with an independent suffix *-mon- arose by a reanalysis of ID *-mon-stems. The fact that independent *-mon-stems in Vedic bear stem-final stress can be understood only if the ID *-mon-stems from which the suffix *-mon- was resegmented also had stem-final stress at whatever stage this reanalysis occurred; accordingly, they constitute evidence that ID *-mon-stems had stem-final stress already in the prehistory of Vedic, or else even in PIE itself (cf. 2.1 above). In this respect, their indirect testimony is wholly consistent with the direct evidence for ID *-mon-stems in Vedic.
4 ID *-mon-stems in Lithuanian and Balto-Slavic
The Lithuanian evidence for PIE ID *-mon-stems is necessarily indirect, since prehistorically they fell together with neuter *-men-stems and animate *-men-stems into a single category, here referred to as the -muo-class after their characteristic nom.sg form.35 Section 4.1 discusses evidence for the diachronic merger of these categories, as well as the synchronic formal properties of the resulting class in derivation and inflection. The prosody of Lithuanian nouns in -muo is then treated in section 4.2, where I argue that ID *-mon-stems with stem-final stress play a pivotal role in explaining the word stress patterns observed in this class.
4.1 The synchronic and diachronic morphology of the Lithuanian -muo-class
The Lithuanian -muo-class consists of masculine nouns. Synchronically, this class is productive in deverbal derivation (cf. Skardžius 1943:293–297, Bammesberger 1973:118–122), especially in the formation of technical and scientific terms. Some representative examples of -muo-class nouns derived in this way are given in (10).
(10) |
Verbal base (3sg.act) |
–muo-noun (m.nom.sg) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a. |
atitìkti |
‘correspond to’ |
atitikmuõ |
‘correspondence’ |
||
b. |
dúoti |
‘give’ |
duomuõ |
‘(logical) given’ |
||
c. |
júosti |
‘engird’ |
juosmuõ |
‘waist(band)’ |
||
d. |
reĩkti |
‘be necessary’ |
reikmuõ |
‘need’ |
||
e. |
sèkti |
‘follow’ |
sekmuõ |
‘consequence’ |
||
f. |
sṗ́eti |
‘assume’ |
spėmuõ |
‘assumption’ |
A few other nouns in the -muo-class are more likely to be deradical. One example is raumuõ ‘muscle’, which may be derived from a root raud- ‘red’ (< PIE *h1reudh-; NIL: 580–584) at the base of the adjectives raũdas ‘reddish-brown’ and rùdas ‘red-haired’ (cf. Smoczyński 2018:1068–1070, 1110). Another is ašmuõ ‘cutting edge’, derived from the same root aš- ‘sharp’ (< PIE *h2ek̑-; NIL: 287–300) as aštrùs ‘sharp’ and ašnìs ‘scythe blade; sprout’ (cf. Smoczyński 2018:51–52).
Some of the strongest evidence for the diachronic confluence of *-mon-stems, neuter *-men-stems, and animate *-men-stems in the -muo-class comes from their intraparadigmatic suffixal allomorphy. The inflection of this class is exemplified in (11):
(11) Inflection of Lithuanian -muo-class (3b raumuõ ‘muscle’):
singular
dual
plural
nom
raumuõ
raũmeniu
raũmenys
voc
raumeniẽ
raũmeniu
raũmenys
gen
raumeñs
–
raumen̨ũ
dat
raũmeniui
raumenìm
raumenìms
acc
raũmen̨i
raũmeniu
raũmenis
ins
raũmeniu
raumenim̃
raumenimìs
loc
raumenyjè
–
raumenysè
Segmentally, the nom.sg in -muo is standardly taken to reflect the nom.sg of PIE *-mon-stems (< *[-mōn]; see, e.g., Senn 1966:87, Stang 1966:47, Olander 2015:83–84). On the other hand, the suffixal e-vowel seen in the other case forms can be attributed to the influence of (i) neuter *-men-stems, which had *e-grade in their oblique cases (e.g., ins.sg Ved. bráh-man-ā ‘with the sacred formulation’ < *-men-eh1); (ii) animate *-men-stems, which had *e-grade in at least the acc.sg (e.g., Gk.
The confluence of these three historically distinct categories is also supported by comparative-historical evidence. Lithuanian nouns in the -muo-class form word equations with nominals belonging to each of these categories in IE languages in which they remain distinct. Thus, for instance, Lith. piemuõ ‘shepherd boy’ matches Gk.
(12) Word equations between Lith. -muo-class nouns and *-mon- or n *-men-stems in IE:
Lithuanian
PIE *-mon-
PIE n *-men-
root
a.
akmuõ ‘stone’
Gk.
ἄκμων ‘anvil’–
–
b.
augmuõ ‘sprout’
Ved. ojmā́nam ‘strength’
Lat. augmen ‘increase’
*h2eug-
c.
dėmuõ ‘addition’
Gk.
θημών ‘heap’Ved. dhā́man– ‘domain’
*dheh1-
d.
juosmuõ ‘waist(band)’
–
Gk.
ζῶμα ‘girdle’*yeh3s-
e.
melmuõ ‘loins’
–
Ved. márman-‘vulnerable spot’
*mel-?
f.
sekmuõ ‘consequence’
–
Ved. sákman- ‘fellowship’
*sekw-
g.
ṡ́emenys ‘linseed’ (pl)
–
Lat. sēmen ‘seed’
*seh1-
h.
sraumuõ ‘stream’
–
Gk.
ῥεῦμα ‘stream’*sreu-
i.
st(u)omuõ ‘stature’
Gk.
στήμων ‘warp’Ved. sthā́man– ‘station’
*steh2-
j.
šelmuõ ‘roof ridge’
–
Ved. śárman- ‘shelter’
*k̑el-
k.
želmuõ ‘shoot’
Ved. harimā́ṇam ‘jaundice’
–
*g̑helh3-
4.2 The synchronic and diachronic prosody of the Lithuanian -muo-class
The question of principal interest for this study is how word stress in the Lithuanian -muo-class is to be explained historically. In standard Lithuanian -muo-class nouns as a rule belong to accent paradigm (AP) 3, exhibiting intraparadigmatic stress mobility of the type illustrated in (11) above. A handful of -muo-class nouns are also attested with AP 1 forms (i.e., fixed initial stress). In Old Lithuanian (Daukša), the word for ‘stone’ in (12a) is—as noted in 2.3 above—overwhelmingly AP 1 (e.g., nom.sg ãkmuo); the (plurale tantum) word for ‘linseed’ in (12g) is mostly AP 1 (e.g., dat.pl ṡ́emenims); and ‘shepherd boy’ is occasionally AP 1 (e.g., gen.sg píemenes).36 Senn (1966:138–139) states that ṡ́emenys retains AP 1 in the literary language, and adds that the same holds for ṙ́emuo ‘heartburn’. Finally, Illič-Svityč (1979:55) reports that st(u)omuõ ‘stature’ in (12i) is attested in the dictionary of Mielcke (Mielcke 1800) as AP 1, and that the same stress pattern occurs in the eastern Aukštaitis dialect of the Pušalotas region. This distribution of forms suggests that for each of these nouns AP 1 is older, replaced by the AP 3 pattern that is regular in the productive -muo-class.37
What must be accounted for, then, from a diachronic perspective is how AP 3 came to be the regular stress pattern in the -muo-class. Regarding the prehistory of AP 3 there is widespread agreement: Lithuanian nominals with this stress pattern had stem-final stress (traditionally “oxytone”) prior to the set of innovations that gave rise to the phenomenon of “Balto-Slavic accentual mobility”—i.e., the innovative stress mobility patterns that are characteristic of the Baltic and Slavic languages.38 In contrast, inherited nominals with fixed root stress either preserve this stress pattern, hence AP 1 inflection, or undergo Saussure’s Law (de Saussure 1896; cf. Olander 2009:109–117, Jasanoff 2017:38–39), resulting in mobile AP 2 inflection. The emergence of generalized AP 3 in the -muo-class can thus be explained only if stem-final stress was well-represented among the historical inputs to this class.
It was established already in 4.1 above that ID *-mon-stems, neuter *-men-stems, and animate *-men-stems were among these inputs. To judge from the comparative evidence in (12) above, the most common of these were neuter *-men-stems. For this reason, it is attractive to consider scenarios in which neuter *-men-stems were a direct source of stem-final stress, as these would effectively solve the problem of AP 3 inflection in the Lithuanian -muo-class: the stress pattern best represented among the historical membership of the class was generalized to the rest. I discuss two such scenarios in 4.2.1 and 4.2.2 below. Having argued that neither is tenable, I then consider the possibility that the emergence of AP 3 inflection in the -muo-class owes crucially to animate *-men-stems (4.2.3) or ID *-mon-stems (4.2.4).
4.2.1 AP 3 inflection is due to neuter *-men-stems? The “proterokinetic” hypothesis
One scenario in which neuter *-men-stems facilitate the development of AP 3 in the Lithuanian *-muo-class would take as its starting point Schindler’s (1975c:263–264) widely accepted (pre-)PIE reconstruction of neuter *-men-stems with PK inflection: nom/acc.sg R(é)-mṇ, gen.sg R(∅)-mén-s, dat.sg R(∅)-mén-ei, etc. It would then be necessary to assume just two developments in the prehistory of Balto-Slavic: (i) analogical leveling of root full-grade from the strong cases to weak; (ii) analogical leveling of suffixal stress from the weak cases to the strong. If Schindler’s reconstruction is correct, then the first step would have strong empirical support. There is no evidence for intraparadigmatic *é/∅-root ablaut among the members of the Lithuanian -muo-class in (12) that align etymologically with other neuter *-men-stems nor, for that matter, in any other members of this class. Likewise, deverbal neuter *-men-stems elsewhere in the IE family reflect only paradigmatic full-grade of the root;39 thus, e.g., Lat. augmen ‘increase’ in (12b) (< PIE *h2éug-men-) has a gen.sg augminis, Ved. dhā́man– ‘domain’ in (12c) (< PIE *dhéh1-men-; cf. Av. dāman- ‘creation’) has a gen.sg dhā́manas (cf. YAv. dām̨an), Gk.
The more serious problems with this scenario arise in the second step. There are two basic issues. First, if it is the case that neuter *-men-stems retained PK mobility going into Balto-Slavic, it would be a striking archaism against the other ancient IE languages, which otherwise point uniformly to fixed root stress already in PIE. In Vedic, all deverbal neuter *-men-stems exhibit fixed root stress (cf. Wackernagel & Debrunner 1954:755–759). In Greek, all deverbal neuter *-men-stems exhibit RA,41 which presupposes an earlier Vedic-like situation. The Hittite evidence is more limited, but the neuter *-men-stem NINDAšaraman- ‘ration bread’ clearly shows root stress in the strong cases (nom/acc.sg šarāman [sráː-man], pl šarāma [sráː-ma]) and deletion of the suffixal vowel in the weak cases (e.g., dat/loc.sg šaramni, gen.sg šaramnaš), which suggest that these forms also had root stress (i.e., [sráː-mn-i], [sráː-mn-as]). This is corroborated by an Old Script plene spelling of the root vowel dat/loc.pl sarāmaš ([sráː-m(n)-as]; KBo 20.27 rev. 10).42
Thus for Balto-Slavic to have inherited neuter *-men-stems with suffixal stress requires that the apparent convergence between these other IE languages is accidental, the result of independent but parallel levelings of root stress from the strong cases to the weak in each branch. One might argue that this is a trivial (hence repeatable) innovation, but this would have awkward consequences for the second issue, the assumption that Balto-Slavic has leveled suffixal stress from the weak cases to the strong. If there were indeed certain structural factors at work in the grammar of PIE that facilitated the generalization of root stress in neuter *-men-stems in the prehistory of Indo-Iranian, Greek, and Anatolian, then it would be especially unlikely that the leveling went in the opposite direction in Balto-Slavic.
It is unlikely, then, that Balto-Slavic is an outlier in this way. The comparative data discussed above suggests instead that already in PIE neuter *-men-stems had stressed full-grade of the root in both their strong and weak cases (cf. Lundquist & Yates 2018:2110): nom/acc.sg R(é)-mṇ, gen.sg R(é)-men-(e/o)s, dat.sg R(é)-men-ei, nom/acc.pl R(é)-mōn).43 If this reconstruction is correct, PIE neuter *-men-stem should simply have maintained fixed root stress (= AP 1 inflection) into (Old) Lithuanian, just like OLith. ãkmuo ‘stone’ (< *h2ék̑mon- per 2.3 above). Consequently, neuter *-men-stems would not have contributed to the development of word stress in the Lithuanian -muo-class; rather, this class must have developed AP 3 on the basis of its other members, and erstwhile neuter *-men-stems owe their AP 3 inflection (in place of historically expected AP 1) to their eventual incorporation into this class.
4.2.2 AP 3 inflection is due to neuter *-men-stems? The “hysterokinetic” hypothesis
Jasanoff (2017:166–168) has raised the possibility of an alternative scenario in which neuter *-men-stems play a crucial role in the development of AP 3 inflection in the Lithuanian -muo-class.44 Following Nussbaum (1986:118–123), he assumes that in PIE the nom/acc.pl (“collective”) of neuter *-men-stems could be formed in one of two ways, AK *R(é)-mōn or “hysterokinetic” (HK) *R(∅)-mḗn. In his view, the latter is continued in Slavic, where neuter *-men-stems have nom/acc.sg forms in Proto-Slavic (PSl.) -m̨e (e.g., OCS sěm̨e ‘seed’, vrěm̨e ‘time’) that cannot reflect PIE *-mṇ,45 which would have yielded PSl. x-mĭ. Jasanoff argues that inherited *-mṇ was remade after PIE nom/acc.pl *-mḗn, whence PSl. *-m̨e.46 Supposing that this change took place already in Proto-Balto-Slavic (PBS),47 one could envision a scenario whereby the stem-final stress of the new n.nom/acc.sg in *-mḗn was leveled through the paradigm of (some) neuter *-men-stems, creating new weak case forms with stem-final stress (gen.sg *-mén-e/os, dat.sg *-mén-ei, etc.).48
This scenario encounters morphological problems, however. The basic issue is the lack of comparative support for n.nom/acc.pl forms in *-mḗn (cf. Harðarson 1987b:94). Outside of Balto-Slavic, the only possible trace of such forms is in Indo-Iranian where, according to Jasanoff (2017:168 n. 101) and Nussbaum (apud Jasanoff 1989:138 n. 10), they are directly reflected in Avestan n.nom/acc.pl -m̨an (or -m̨am with assimilation), e.g., OAv. dām̨an ‘bonds’, haxəm̨am ‘retinues’. Jasanoff (1989:138) argues that it is advantageous to derive these forms from *-mḗn rather than the standardly reconstructed *-mōn because, in his view, word-final *n was lost already in PIE after *ō but not after *ē (cf. n. 6 above), thus only from *-mḗn can OAv. -m̨an/–m̨am be lautgesetzlich. Yet the assumption that *-mḗn would yield -m̨an/–m̨am is problematic, since the nasal-less nom.sg of OAv. airiiə̄mā ‘hospitality’ (< *-mḗn) suggests that word-final *n was also lost in this environment, likely already in Proto-Indo-Iranian (PIIr.; cf. Ved. aryamā́ ‘Aryaman’).49 Therefore regardless of whether the starting point for OAv. -m̨an/–m̨am is *-mḗn or *-mō(n), it is necessary to assume that the word(/suffix)-final *n was analogically restored in Indo-Iranian from a paradigm cell in which it was preserved—i.e., singular weak cases with PIIr. *-man-V or gen.pl *-man-aHam.
A still more serious objection to deriving OAv. -m̨an/–m̨am from *-mḗn is prosodic. While there is no way to determine how the reflexes of neuter *-men-stems were stressed in Avestan, there is no reason to suspect that it was different from Vedic, where the cognate class shows only root stress in the nom/acc.pl—e.g., Ved. kármāṇi ‘deeds’, sádmāni ‘seats’, dā́māni ‘cords’ (cf. OAv. dām̨an above). A pre-form *-mḗn with final stress cannot account for the root stress pattern in these Vedic forms. Root stress is expected, however, if they derive from PIE *R(é)-mōn via the series of developments in (13) (cf. Harðarson 1987b:96–97):
(13) Chronological development of nom/acc.pl of PIE neuter *-men-stems into Vedic:
a. PIE *R(é)-mōn > pre-PIIr. *R(á)-mā
Phonological loss of word-final *n after *ō in PIE (per Jasanoff 1989) or after *ā in pre-PIIr.
b. pre-PIIr. *R(á)-mā >> PIIr. *R(á)-mān
Analogical restoration of suffix-final *n from elsewhere in the paradigm in PIIr.50
c. PIIr. *R(á)-mān >> PInd. *R(á)-mānH > Ved. R(á)-māni
Recharacterization with the n.nom/acc.pl marker *-H (< PIE *-h2) in Proto-Indic (PInd.).51
Since OAv. -m̨an/–m̨am can equally continue *-mḗn or *-mōn and Ved. -māni can only continue the latter, the most economical account of the Indo-Iranian data is that both reflect *-mōn.
The Slavic evidence for n.nom/acc.pl *-mḗn is likewise equivocal. Kim (2019b) provides a critical assessment of various accounts of the historically unexpected nom/acc.sg *-m̨e found in Slavic neuter *-men-stems, including that of Jasanoff (2017). He argues in favor of an explanation first proposed by Vondrák (1905:215) and subsequently endorsed by a number of scholars (e.g., Kortlandt 1983:176, Szemerényi 1996:170–171, Rasmussen apud Olander 2015:85): inherited nom/acc.sg *-mṇ was replaced in PBS by *-men, which was analogically leveled from the weak cases in *-men-V; PBS *-men then developed regularly into PSl. *-m̨e.52
Thus neither Slavic nor Avestan provides compelling evidence for an inherited nom/acc.pl *-mḗn in neuter *-men-stems. Accordingly, I reject the reconstruction of such forms for PIE. An upshot of this conclusion is that PIE neuter *-men-stems can be reconstructed with a single exponent of nom/acc.pl, schematically *R(é)-mōn, thereby eliminating the need to explain why this category should have two n.nom/acc.pl forms without any apparent functional difference. However, it also implies that *-mḗn did not factor into the prehistory of Lithuanian neuter *-men-stems (as in the scenario outlined above), in which case it is necessary to look elsewhere for the source of AP 3 inflection in this class.
4.2.3 AP 3 inflection is due to animate *-men-stems?
A different potential source of AP 3 in the -muo-class is animate *-men-stems. It is uncontroversial that this class had suffixal stress in their strong case forms in PIE, as is the case in Greek (e.g., nom.sg
The main issue with attributing AP 3 inflection in the -muo-class to animate *-men-stems alone is that there is just a single clear reflex of this category in Lithuanian, piemuõ ‘shepherd boy’, which cannot on its own have constituted a sufficient basis for generalizing AP 3 in the -muõ-class at the expense of the AP 1 inflection that is historically expected for the much more numerous neuter *-men-stems (per above). There is also an issue particular to this lexical item—namely, that it is attested with AP 1 inflection in Old Lithuanian (cf. n. 37 above). However these forms are to be explained, they suggest that piemuõ adopted AP 3 inflection within the history of Lithuanian; it thus can hardly have played a role in establishing AP 3 as the regular prosodic pattern in the -muo-class.
4.2.4 AP 3 inflection is due to ID *-mon-stems
The last remaining historical input to the Lithuanian -muo-class was PIE *-mon-stems. I propose that it was mainly PIE nominals stems of this type that served as the historical basis for the generalization of AP 3 inflection in the -muo-class. If ID *-mon-stems like (12) were inherited into Balto-Slavic with the same stem-final stress pattern consistently observed in Vedic (e.g., acc.sg -mā́n-am, gen.sg -mán-as; see section 3 above), they would naturally have developed AP 3 inflection in Lithuanian. Taken together with any other animate *-men-stems (viz., besides piemuõ) that may have been inherited into Lithuanian, these ID *-mon-stems could have provided a robust enough core of nominals with AP 3 inflection that this stress pattern was extended to neuter *-men-stems when they fell together with the other *-mVn-stems into the -muo-class.
For comparison, consider what would have happened if PIE ID *-mon-stems were instead inherited into Balto-Slavic with the AK stress pattern reconstructed by EM. The outcome of such forms would in all likelihood have been the same AP 1 inflection as observed in OLith. ãkmuo (cf. 4.2.1) above). In this scenario, the emergence of AP 3 inflection in the Lithuanian -muo-class would be surprising, since nearly all of the historical inputs to this class should have developed AP 1 inflection. In contrast, the proposal outlined above offers a plausible pathway to AP 3 in the -muo-class by making ID *-mon-stems a direct source of this stress pattern.
5 ID *-mon-stems in Greek
The Greek evidence for the reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems is difficult to interpret. Like Vedic, Greek maintains the inherited distinction between *-mon-stems, animate *-men-stems, and neuter *-men-stems, preserving all three as separate classes. Greek *-mon-stems, however, exhibit a much greater diversity of formal properties. Morphosyntactically, they may be nouns or adjectives. Some are probably formed from primary or non-primary neuter *-men-stems by ID, whereas others appear to be derived from verbal or nominal bases with a suffix *-mon-. As for their prosodic properties, some have fixed stem-final stress throughout their inflectional paradigm, while others are subject to RA (for the term see 2.2 above).
Morphosyntax and prosody are correlated in Greek *-mon-stems. A first-order division can be made between, on the one hand, *-mon-stem nouns, which show a mixture of stem-final stress and RA, and on the other, *-mon-stem adjectives, which uniformly exhibit RA (cf. Schwyzer 1939:522).54 I begin by examining the nouns in 5.1, then proceed to the adjectives in 5.2. Finally, 5.3 provides an overall assessment of this evidence. I argue on Greek-internal grounds that RA in some *-mon-stem nouns and all *-mon-stem adjectives is innovative, a diachronic replacement for the stem-final stress pattern that is to some extent preserved in nouns of this type.
5.1 The phonology and morphology of Greek *-mon-stem nouns
I organize my discussion of Greek *-mon-stem nouns along morphological lines, based on their likely historical and/or synchronic derivational bases. Greek nouns that may directly continue ID *-mon-stems are treated in 5.1.1, followed by nouns that were formed by suffixation of *-mon- to verbal or nominal stems in 5.1.2. A residual set of archaic-looking *-mon-stems are examined in 5.1.3.
5.1.1 Greek *-mon-stem nouns derived from primary neuter *-men-stems
Greek *-mon-stem nouns provide some direct evidence for the prosodic reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems. This evidence consists of a small set of *-mon-stem nouns attested beside cognate primary neuter *-men-stems from which they may have been formed historically by ID. These derivationally related pairs are given in (14), where it can be observed that the *-mon-stems exhibit an even mixture of stress patterns: (14a–c) have RA, which descriptively yields root stress except in the gen.pl (-
(14) Greek *-mon-stem nouns attested beside cognate primary neuter *-men-stem nouns:
nom/acc.sg *[-mṇ]
nom.sg *[-mōn]
a.
Gk.
τέρμα ‘end, boundary’
τέρμων ‘boundary’
b.
Gk.
στῆμα ‘stamen; shaft of penis’
στήμων ‘warp’
c.
Gk.
γνῶμα ‘judgment’
γνώμων ‘judge, interpreter’
d.
Gk.
θῆμα ‘tomb’
θημών ‘heap’
e.
Gk.
χεῖμα ‘winter (weather)’
χειμών ‘winter (weather), storm’
f.
Gk.
κεῦθμα *‘hiding place’
κευθμών ‘hiding place’
The implications of this evidence for IE reconstruction are not immediately clear. Since the result of RA in (14a–c) is generally root stress, it is plausible to view RA in these nouns as the regular diachronic outcome in Greek of prehistoric root stress and thus to take them as evidence for reconstructing PIE ID *-mon-stems with this pattern. On the other hand, (14d–f) point to stem-final stress as the inherited pattern.
A closer examination of these forms yields little to suggest that one stress pattern or the other is innovative. All six *-mon-stems in (14) occur in archaic or classical Greek, and both groups include nouns that are attested already in early Greek epic:
In sum, neither of the two stress patterns in (14)—i.e., RA in (14a–c) or stem-final stress in (14d–f)—has an obviously stronger claim to diachronic priority. The situation is clearer, however, in Greek’s indirect reflexes of PIE *-mon-stems, which are the subject of 5.1.2.
5.1.2 Greek *-mon-stem nouns derived from verbal or nominal stems with a suffix *-mon-
Greek also has *-mon-stems that lack corresponding neuter *-men-stems. Most of these appear to be derived from attested verbal or nominal stems with an independent suffix *-mon-stem, and as such, indirectly testify to the stress patterns of PIE ID *-mon-stems (cf. 2.1 above). Greek *-mon-stem nouns formed in this way are listed in (15) along with their derivational bases.59 That these *-mon-stem nouns are non-primary derivatives is evident from the fact that, in each case, overt stem-forming morphology contained in the base also surfaces in the derived noun.
(15) Greek nouns derived from verbal or nominal stems with a suffix *-mon-:
base
nom.sg *[-mōn]
a.
Gk.
κήδομαι ‘care for’
κηδεμών ‘attendant’
b.
Gk.
ἡγέομαι ‘lead’
ἡγεμών ‘leader’
c.
Gk.
ἀγρέω ‘seize’
ἀγρεμών ‘hunter’
d.
Gk.
ἰχνεύω ‘track’
ἰχνεύμων ‘(type of weasel/wasp)’
e.
Gk.
ἀρτάω ‘hang’
ἀρτέμων ‘foresail’
f.
Gk.
δαιτύς ‘meal’
δαιτυμών ‘diner’
g.
Gk.
ἀκρός ‘top; end’
ἀκρεμών ‘branch’
h.
Gk.
θηλή ‘teat’
θηλαμών ‘wet nurse’
In the *-mon-stem nouns in (15) stem-final stress is the predominant pattern. It is the only stress pattern attested for denominal *-mon-stem nouns—i.e.,
Yet however RA in these two forms is to be explained, it is evident that that stem-final stress is the better established pattern in non-primary *-mon-stem nouns, especially in the oldest layer of Greek. These nouns accordingly constitute a prima facie argument for reconstructing stem-final stress in ID *-mon-stems.
5.1.3 Other Greek *-mon-stem nouns
Not all Greek *-mon-stems fit neatly into the morphological types discussed in 5.1.1 or 5.1.2. Here I briefly discuss three such *-mon-stems that, in view of their early Greek attestation and possible IE comparanda, have a credible claim to inheritance:
The first,
The second,
Finally,
Questions thus remain concerning the morphological background of
5.2 The phonology and morphology of Greek *-mon-stem adjectives
Greek *-mon-stem adjectives as a class differ from *-mon-stem nouns in at least two general ways. The first is phonological: all Greek *-mon-stem adjectives have RA, in contrast to the mixture of RA and stem-final stress seen in *-mon-stem nouns. They also differ morphologically, in that relative to the nouns the adjectives more often appear to be inner-Greek formations, derived by productive processes in the language. Consistent with this view is the fact that (virtually) all *-mon-stem adjectives have clear derivational bases—in some cases, more than one possible base—with which they stand in a transparent semantic relationship,68 and that a non-trivial number of these adjectives are hapax forms.69 Conversely, *-mon-stem adjectives with exact cognates in other IE languages are almost entirely lacking, and even *-mon-stem adjectives paired with reconstructible primary neuter *-men-stems—i.e., the type that bear directly on the reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems—are relatively rare. In the remainder of this section, I lay out the evidence for Greek *-mon-stem adjectives, focusing especially on their morphological structure, which often admits more than one analysis; I then return to the issue of their prosody in 5.3 below.
5.2.1 Greek *-mon-stem adjectives beside neuter *-men-stems
A first group of *-mon-stem adjectives are attested beside neuter *-men-stem nouns from which they could in principle be formed by ID. Examples of this type are given in (16):70
(16) Greek *-mon-stem adjectives with corresponding neuter *-men-stem nouns:
nom.sg *[-mṇ]
nom.sg *[-mōn]
a.
Gk.
μνῆμα ‘remembrance’
μνήμων ‘mindful’
b.
Gk.
ἧμα ‘projectile’
ἥμων ‘projectile-throwing’
c.
Gk.
αἷμα ‘blood’
αἵμων ‘bloody’
d.
Gk.
πῆμα ‘misery’
πήμων ‘baneful’
e.
Gk.
νόημα ‘thought’
νοήμων ‘thoughtful’
f.
Gk.
δήλημα ‘harm’
δηλήμων ‘harmful’
g.
Gk.
πένθημα ‘mourning’
πενθήμων ‘mournful’
h.
Gk.
μείδημα ‘smile’
μειδᾱ́μων ‘smiling’
i.
Gk.
ἄνθημα ‘blossom’
ἀνθήμων ‘blooming’
j.
Gk.
τέχνημα ‘work of art’
τεχνήμων ‘artful’
k.
Gk.
βλάστημα ‘offspring’
βλαστήμων ‘sprouting’
Of these *-mon-stem adjectives at least
It is uncertain, however, whether the *-mon-stems in (16) are really related to their corresponding neuter *-men-stems in the way represented in these derivations. A well-known distributional property of *-mon-stem adjectives is that BV compounds are much more common by type than non-compounds, and as a consequence, there are numerous *-mon-stem adjectives that occur only as the 2M of BV compounds, where they function as the compositional form of neuter *-men-stems (cf. 2.2 above). Thus, e.g., beside the neuter *-men-stems
5.2.2 Greek deverbal and denominal *-mon-stem adjectives formed with a suffix *-mon-
Also found in Greek are *-mon-stem adjectives without corresponding neuter *-men-stems. Two of these,
The other candidate for a primary derivative,
On this view,
(17) Greek adjectives derived from verbal or nominal stems with a suffix *-mon-:
base
nom.sg *[-mōn]
a.
Gk.
τλῆναι ‘endure’
τλήμων ‘enduring’
b.
Gk.
δαῆναι ‘learn’
δαήμων ‘experienced’
c.
Gk.
αἰδέομαι ‘be ashamed’
αἰδήμων ‘modest’
d.
Gk.
ἐλεέω ‘show mercy’
ἐλεήμων ‘merciful’
e.
Gk.
ἀλάομαι ‘wander’
ἀλήμων ‘wandering; inconstant’
f.
Gk.
μάχη ‘battle’
μαχήμων ‘warlike’
g.
Gk.
παιγνίη ‘play, game’
παιγνιήμων ‘playful’
With the exception of
5.2.3 Greek deverbal and denominal *-mon-stem adjectives formed with a suffix -ημον -
Finally, Greek has a number of *-mon-stems adjectives that appear to be derived from simple thematic verbs or nouns but unexpectedly show a presuffixal long vowel (nom.sg -
(18) Greek adjectives derived from verbal or nominal stems with a suffix -
ημον -:
base
nom.sg *[-mōn]
a.
Gk.
ἀλιτεῖν ‘sin, transgress’
ἀλιτήμων ‘wicked’
b.
Gk.
δείδω ‘fear’
δειδήμων ‘fearful’
c.
Gk.
ζῆλος ‘jealousy’
ζηλήμων ‘jealous’
d.
Gk.
ἔθω ‘be accustomed’
ἐθήμων ‘accustomed’
e.
Gk.
(
ἐ )θέλω ‘want, wish’
θελήμων ‘voluntary’
f.
Gk.
ψεύδω ‘lie’
ψευδήμων ‘lying, false’
At least three *-mon-stem adjectives derived in this way occur in Homer:
Significantly, the examples in (18) raise the possibility of an alternative analysis of many of the adjectives discussed above—namely, that they are formed with the same non-primary suffix -
This type of analysis cannot of course reflect the original situation: as noted above, there must have been an original core of *-mon-stem nominals formed by ID or by suffixation of *-mon- from which -
It is not crucial for present purposes whether the strong form of this hypothesis is correct. What is clear from the discussion above is that the majority of Greek *-mon-stem adjectives are created within Greek or its shallow prehistory, and that many of these can be attributed to one of two innovative morphological processes: (i) back-formation from compounds; or (ii) suffixation of -
5.3 The synchronic and diachronic prosody of Greek *-mon-stems
The preceding two sections examined the Greek evidence for *-mon-stem nouns (5.1) and adjectives (5.2), which exhibit a prosodic split: nouns have a mixture of suffixal stress and RA, while adjectives uniformly have RA. This section is concerned with the diachronic development of this prosodic distribution. I consider three possible explanations below, all of which depart from the standard assumption that the morphosyntactic distinction between nouns and adjectives is a Greek innovation, as there is no evidence for such a distinction in Vedic Sanskrit or elsewhere in IE.
5.3.1 Explaining word stress in Greek *-mon-stems: the “amphikinetic” hypothesis
The first possibility is that *-mon-stem nominals were inherited into Greek with AK stress mobility (as in (2b) above), then generalized root(/initial) stress at an early prehistoric stage. On this scenario, the more frequent RA pattern would be explained straightforwardly: in *-mon-stems formed by ID from primary neuter *-men-stems, root stress and RA are formally equivalent except in the gen.pl (cf. 5.1.1 above). After the emergence of the right edge stress window in Proto-Greek (“Law of Limitation;” see Probert 2012; Gunkel 2014b), the transition from root stress to RA would be essentially trivial. What requires a special explanation on this account are the cases of stem-final stress in Greek *-mon-stem nouns, which were argued in 5.1 to support the reconstruction of this pattern for PIE ID *-mon-stem. One such explanation is offered by Buck & Petersen (1945:216), who observe that stem-final stress is especially well-represented in Greek *-mon-stem agent nouns, which include (nearly) all of the forms in (15) and, from a historical perspective, likely also
This proposal encounters at least two problems, however. One is that Greek agent nouns do not uniformly exhibit stem-final stress—for instance, the numerous agent nouns formed with the suffix -
5.3.2 Explaining word stress in Greek *-mon-stems: the modified “amphikinetic” hypothesis
A second possibility is that Greek inherited *-mon-stems with nom.sg * ′–mōn but acc.sg *-món-ṃ and nom.pl *-món-es due to the operation of the so-called “*kwetwóres-Regel” over an erstwhile AK paradigm (Rix 1985; cf. Klingenschmitt 1994:389–390 n. 131). The resulting stress alternation was then variously leveled: some Greek *-mon-stems generalized the root stress of the nom/acc.sg, others the stem-final stress of the acc.sg and nom.pl.
This approach—recently endorsed, e.g., by Neri (2017:112 n. 114)—is difficult to falsify: if one starts from a primary nominal paradigm that has a combination of root, suffix, and ending stress, then one can in principle explain any attested stress pattern by paradigm leveling. Yet positing such analogical leveling is by no means cost-free, and in this case, it is necessary to explain why the root-suffix-ending stress alternation, which should have arisen in every AK paradigm via the “*kwetwóres-Regel,” was eliminated in every one of these paradigms in every single daughter language. Even the two Vedic nominal paradigms that uniquely retain AK-type edge-to-edge stress mobility exhibit only root stress in their strong cases (acc.sg púmāṃs-am ‘man’; nom.pl pánthās ‘paths’), never the peninitial stress expected on this hypothesis (xpumā́ṃs-am < *-ós-ṃ, xpanthā́s < *-óh2-es). More generally, too, there are good reasons to be skeptical of the “*kwetwóres-Regel.” Rasmussen (2001) has criticized the putative evidence for this rule and argued that it should be abandoned.89 To the empirical problems raised by Rasmussen can be added a theoretical consideration—namely, that no plausible phonetic motivation for the change has ever been proposed (cf. Yates 2019b:217 n. 23).
Yet even if these issues were left aside and the “*kwetwóres-Regel” were accepted as a PIE-level sound change, there are certain facts about Greek *-mon-stems that it does not neatly account for. The most problematic set of nominals once again includes
5.3.3 A new historical account of word stress in Greek *-mon-stems
The third possibility is that *-mon-stem nominals were inherited into Greek with stem-final stress (at least in their strong case forms, as in (2b) above). On this scenario, stem-final stress in the non-agentive *-mon-stem nouns noted above is instead a conservative feature, a direct reflex of PIE ID *-mon-stems with stem-final stress in (14a–c), and an indirect reflex of the same in
If this account of the diachrony is correct, then it is the RA pattern observed in a smaller set of Greek *-mon-stem nouns and consistently in the more numerous Greek *-mon-stem adjectives that remains to be explained.90 For the latter, I propose that a three step change has occurred. In the first step, new *-mon-stem adjectives were abstracted from existing BV compounds. Per Risch (1974:52–53) a likely example of a *-mon-stem adjective created at this prehistoric stage is
(19) Greek non-compound adjectives back-formed from compounds exhibit the same stress pattern as the compound:
a. BV compound *-mon-stem adjective (RA) >> *-mon-stem adjective (RA)
b. BV compound *-es-stem adjective (stem-final stress) >> *-es-stem adjective (stem-final stress)
In the next step, the suffix -
At this point, Greek still had some *-mon-stem adjectives with stem-final stress—on the one hand, the reflexes of PIE ID *-mon-stems, which likely include
The remaining forms to be addressed, then, are the *-mon-stem nouns with RA:
Yet if instead
It was suggested already in 5.1 that
I therefore contend that is easier to account for the mixed prosodic behavior of *-mon-stems in Greek starting from an inherited situation in which this stem class uniformly had stem-final stress (in the strong cases) than one in which it had root stress. This claim may seem counter-intuitive, since *-mon-stems with stem-final stress are considerably less common by type than those with RA. However, much of the apparent synchronic robustness of RA is likely to be a recent historical development, owing above all to the inner-Greek productivity of the innovative suffix -
A consequence of this analysis is that the complicated Greek evidence for word stress in *-mon-stems aligns historically with what is directly observable in Vedic (section 3), as well as with what was argued above to be the best starting point for explaining the prosodic properties of the Lithuanian -muo-class (section 4). I return to the implications of this agreement in section 7 after treating the Anatolian evidence for *-mon-stems in section 6 below.
6 ID *-mon-stems in Anatolian
The Anatolian evidence for ID *-mon-stems is sparse. Since Yates (2020b) has recently provided a comprehensive treatment of the direct and indirect reflexes of ID *-mon-stems in Anatolian, I present here just a brief discussion of the most important piece of evidence: Hitt. išḫiman- ‘bond, cord’.97 This noun derives historically from the PIE root *sh2ei- ‘bind’, which is also the source of the Hittite radical ḫi-verb išḫ(a)i- ‘bind’.98 That Hitt. išḫiman- is animate gender—thus the reflex of a *-mon-stem rather than a neuter *-men-stem—is assured, as unambiguously animate strong case forms are securely attested in Old Script texts: nom.sg iš-ḫi-ma-a-aš (KBo 17.15 rev.! 11); nom.pl KUŠiš-ḫi-ma-a-ne-eš (KBo 17.15 rev. 10); and likely acc.pl [KUŠiš-]ḫi-ma-a-nu-uš (KBo 17.15 rev.! 7). Oblique case forms are more limited; only abl and ins are attested and both occur first in Middle Script: abl iš-ḫi-ma-na-az (KUB 36.55 ii 16); ins iš-ḫi-ma-ni-it (KUB 17.60 obv. 3).
All of these forms are phonologically expected if the Hittite noun continues *sh2i-món-—i.e., a primary *-mon-stem with zero-grade of the root and stressed suffixal *ó-vocalism of the type proposed above for Gk.
The pre-form *sh2i-món- may hold special interest for the historical morphology of IE *-mon-stems. On the one hand, this form could have been created within Anatolian, since the verbal root *sh2ei- is preserved in Hittite (išḫ(a)i- ‘bind’) and elsewhere in the Anatolian languages (e.g., reduplicated CLuw. ḫišḫi(ya)–/HLuw. hi-sà-hi- ‘id.’).100 It is thus compatible with the view discussed in 2.1 above that an independent suffix *-mon- developed separately in the prehistory of the IE languages in which it is attested (Anatolian, Celtic, Indo-Iranian, Italic, and Tocharian). On this analysis, Anatolian would have inherited ID *-mon-stems with stem-final stress; from these, a suffix *-mon- was then resegmented and used to form (at least) primary *-mon-stems with stem-final stress, including the congenitor of Hitt. išḫiman- ‘bond, cord’.
Yet there is comparative evidence that suggests a different diachronic scenario. Strikingly, potential reflexes of *sh2i-món- (or of *sih2-món- with laryngeal metathesis) are found in at least three other IE branches: OIr. sim ‘chain’ (or sím; the quantity is unknown); Germanic words for ‘rope’, including OSax. sīmo, OE sīma, and ON sími; and Ved. sīmán- ‘boundary; hairline’ (see KEWA III: 475, Frisk 1960:724–725, Beekes 2010:489–490).101 The Vedic form is of particular significance, as it corroborates the stem-final stress pattern observed in Hitt. išḫiman-. This convergent evidence for *sh2i-món- across four IE branches makes a strong case—indeed, probably a stronger case than for any other primary deverbal *-mon-stem—that it should be reconstructed for PIE, and so too, for the PIE status of the derivational process by which it was formed. If *sh2i-món- is reconstructed for this stage, it would imply that already in PIE itself the independent suffix *-mon- was resegmented from ID *-mon-stems with stem-final stress and used to form (at least) primary derivatives that likewise had stem-final stress, such as *sh2i-món- (thus Yates 2020b:257–258; cf. 2.1 above).
For present purposes, it does not matter which of these scenarios is correct. In either case, the stem-final stress pattern of Hitt. išḫiman- ([is
7 Reassessing the phonology of PIE ID *-mon-stems
The preceding four sections have systematically examined the direct and indirect reflexes of PIE ID *-mon-stems in those IE languages that provide evidence for their inherited stress patterns. The principal question of interest in this survey was whether ID *-mon-stems had root stress in their strong cases, as expected on EM’s AK reconstruction, or stem-final stress, as proposed in section 1 above. In 7.1, I bring together the results of this survey in order to answer this question and thereby establish the reconstructible prosodic properties of ID *-mon-stems in their strong case forms. In 7.2 the evidence for the reconstruction of the weak cases is briefly reassessed, and on this basis, some revisions to the AK reconstruction are proposed. Finally, in 7.3 I combine the findings of the 7.1 and 7.2 in order to present a new prosodic reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems.
7.1 Reconstructing the strong cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems
With respect to word stress in the strong cases of ID *-mon-stems, the evidence of Anatolian, Lithuanian, and Vedic all point in the same direction—namely, toward stem-final stress. In Anatolian (section 6), there is at least one *-mon-stem whose stress pattern can be identified, Hitt. išḫiman- ‘bond, cord’, and it clearly exhibits stem-final stress, as evident from Old Script plene spellings like nom.pl KUŠiš-ḫi-ma-a-ne-eš (KBo 17.15 rev. 10). In Lithuanian (section 4), direct evidence for the stress patterns of ID *-mon-stems is obscured by their historical merger with neuter and animate *-men-stems, but the most plausible explanation for regular stress mobility (AP 3) in the resulting category (i.e., the muo-class) is that it spread analogically from ID *-mon-stems (e.g., Lith. augmuõ ‘sprout’ < PIE *h2eug-món-; cf. Ved. oj-mán- in (12b) above), where it is the expected historical outcome of prehistoric stem-final stress. Finally, Vedic has direct reflexes of ID *-mon-stems, most securely attested beside the primary neuter stems from which they were (historically) derived (e.g., m Ved. dā-mán- ‘giver’ n dā́-man– ‘gift’ in (7a) above), as well as indirect reflexes of this category, primary derivatives of adjectival roots formed with a suffix *-mon- (e.g., prathi-mán- ‘breadth’ << *pḷth2-món– PIE *pleth2-; cf. 5.1 above); both types consistently exhibit stem-final stress.
The only language that complicates this picture is Greek (section 5): some *-mon-stems have stem-final stress, but more (by type) exhibit RA, which is mostly formal equivalent to and thus easily explained diachronically as a reflex of root stress. In 5.3 I made the case on purely Greek-internal grounds that stem-final stress is nevertheless an archaism in this stem class. I proposed a new explanation for the development of RA in Greek *-mon-stems, at the core of which are two well-established observations about the diachrony of the language generally and about *-mon-stems in particular: (i) that there is a tendency for words with inherited non-RA stress patterns to adopt RA over time (Probert 2006a,b; cf. 5.3 above); and (ii) that at least some (and perhaps many) non-compound *-mon-stem adjectives are historically back-formed from BV compounds (Risch 1974:52–53; cf. 5.2 above).
A consequence of (i) is that RA is the lectio facilior in Greek—i.e., in the general case, RA is more likely to be innovative than stem-final stress or other non-RA patterns. It is therefore unsurprising that no plausible explanation for stem-final stress in Greek *-mon-stems as an innovation has yet been put forward (cf. 5.3 above). As for (ii), I suggested that such “de-compositional” *-mon-stem adjectives were created with RA (just like the corresponding BV *-mon-stem compounds); the innovative adjectival suffix -
This particular explanation of the Greek facts (and the premises it rests upon) are of course open to challenge, and I concede that, when considered independently, the mixture of stem-final stress and RA seen in Greek *-mon-stems is hardly probative evidence for reconstructing stem-final stress in PIE. Still, Greek does not provide compelling evidence for reconstructing root stress either. Thus when the comparative IE evidence for ID *-mon-stems is taken into account, there can be little doubt that the strong case forms of nouns belonging to this category had stem-final stress in PIE (not root stress per EM). This stem-final stress pattern was maintained in Vedic and into the prehistory of Lithuanian, indirectly reflected in Anatolian, and partially preserved in Greek.
The final point that must be addressed in order to complete this new reconstruction of the strong cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems is their root vocalism. In this respect, the traditional AK analysis of this category and the alternative proposed in section 1 converge, both reconstructing full-grade of the root in this context. This agreement is not surprising, since the comparative evidence is unequivocal: all direct reflexes of ID *-mon-stems—i.e., *-mon-stems which are attested beside primary neuter *-men-stems in the same IE language or whose corresponding primary neuter *-men-stems are reconstructible for PIE—show full-grade of the root in their strong case forms.102 The strong case forms of PIE ID *-mon-stems can therefore be reconstructed schematically as in (20) ([CeC] = full-grade root; cf. (2b) above):
(20) Strong case forms of ID *-mon-stems in PIE:
PIE
nom.sg
*[CeC-m̄́on]
acc.sg
*[CeC-món-ṃ]
nom.pl
*[CeC-món-es]
7.2 Reconstructing the weak cases of ID *-mon-stems
In the preceding sections, I have generally left aside the issue of the reconstruction of the weak case forms of PIE ID *-mon-stems, primarily because in the weak cases—unlike in the strong cases—the AK reconstruction hypothesized by EM is in my view on the right track.103 Nevertheless, there are some differences between AK inflection and the reconstruction of the weak case forms that—as I will argue below—the comparative evidence supports. My proposal for the reconstruction of the PIE weak case forms is given in (21b) beside the AK reconstruction in (21a):104
(21) Two reconstructions of the weak cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems:
a.
Amphikinetic
b.
Proposed
ins.sg
*[CC-n-éh]
*[CeC-n-éh]
dat.sg
*[CC-n-éi]
*[CeC-n-éi]
abl/gen.sg
*[CC-n-é/ós]
*[CeC-n-é/ós]
loc.sg
*[CC-mén]
*[CeC-mén]
dat.pl
*[CC-n-ós]
*[CeC-n-ós]
gen.pl
*[CC-n-óhom]
*[CeC-n-óhom]
As evident in (21), I tentatively reconstruct for PIE essentially the same desinential patterns: when the inflectional ending was vowel-initial, zero-grade of the suffix (with deletion of suffixal */m/; see below) and stressed inflectional endings (e.g., gen.sg *[-n-é/ós]); and in the “endingless locative,” stressed full-grade of the suffix (loc.sg *[-mén]). The Vedic reflexes of ID *-mon-stems support this reconstruction of the locative—e.g., loc.sg Ved. brahmáṇi ‘at the priest’ (cf. acc.sg brahmā́ṇam in (7c) above), which appears to continue *[-mén] recharacterized by the synchronic loc.sg ending -i. Vedic also provides evidence for zero-grade of the *m-less suffix and stressed inflectional endings. Traces of this pattern are attested (e.g., ins.sg Ved. dānā́ ‘with giving’; cf. acc.sg dāmā́nam in (7a) above) beside a much more common pattern whereby the suffixal vowel surfaces and receives stress (e.g., ins.sg ománā ‘with aid’; cf. acc.sg omā́nam in (8a) above). Since */m/-deletion appears to be reconstructible for PIE (see, e.g., Mayrhofer 1986:159),105 it seems likely that the former Vedic pattern reflects the PIE situation, while the latter emerged in (the prehistory of) Vedic after this */m/-deletion process was lost.
In a different respect, however, I contest the AK reconstruction of the weak cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems. I propose that these forms were characterized by full-grade of the root rather than the zero-grade expected if ID *-mon-stems exhibited AK inflection in PIE. It was observed in 7.1 above that all assured direct reflexes of ID *-mon-stems in the IE languages continue full-grade of the root in their strong case forms. While the survey of these IE reflexes conducted in sections 3–6 above focused on their strong case forms, a complementary observation is relevant here: none of these IE forms shows any evidence for intraparadigmatic root ablaut. In other words, these forms all appear to reflect paradigmatic full-grade of the root, including in the weak cases. This point is effectively conceded by Stüber (1998:147), who attributes it to intraparadigmatic leveling from the strong cases.106 Since leveling of root ablaut is a common phenomenon in the IE languages, such a change could plausibly have occurred independently in each of these languages. Nevertheless, given the absence of evidence for zero-grade in the weak (or strong) cases, it is more economical to assume that the weak cases of ID *-mon-stem should be reconstructed as in (21b) with full-grade of the root already in PIE. It is therefore a virtue of the analysis of the morphophonology of ID *-mon-stems developed in §8 below that it straightforwardly accounts for this weak root full-grade, which it explains via the same mechanism as the root full-grade in the strong cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems.
7.3 A new prosodic reconstruction of PIE *-mon-stems
The comparative evidence for the prosodic reconstruction of the strong and weak cases of PIE ID *-mon-stems was reevaluated in 7.1 and 7.2 respectively. I have argued that this evidence supports reconstructing for PIE ID *-mon-stems the (partial) inflectional paradigm in (22), which deviates from the traditional AK reconstruction in two principal ways: (i) stem-final stress in the strong cases (vs. root stress); (ii) paradigmatic full-grade of the root (vs. zero-grade in weak cases).107
(22) A new reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems:
sg
pl
nom
*[CeC-m̄́on]
*[CeC-món-es]
acc
*[CeC-món-ṃ]
ins
*[CeC-n-éh]
dat
*[CeC-n-éi]
*[CeC-n-ós]
abl
*[CeC-n-é/ós]
gen
*[CeC-n-é/ós]
*[CeC-n-óhom]
loc
*[CeC-mén]
A reviewer raises the possibility that the AK inflection hypothesized by EM for ID *-mon-stems could still be reconciled with the IE evidence that I have argued supports the reconstruction of the paradigm in (22) for PIE. One way to bridge this gap would be to assume that the AK paradigm reconstructed under EM obtained in pre-PIE and was then transformed via a series of analogical innovations into the PIE paradigm in (22), which in turn was inherited into the IE languages along the lines described in sections 3–6 above.108
On this view, the relevant analogical innovations would need to occur just once (viz., between pre-PIE and PIE), and indeed, such changes are conceivable. The paradigmatic full-grade in (22) could have been leveled from the weak cases, as already noted in 7.2 above. As for the stress pattern of the strong cases in (22), one possibility is that it spread analogically from the endingless locative, the lone site of stem-final stress per EM (see, e.g., Schindler 1975c:262, Meier-Brügger & Fritz 2021:222–223). An alternative possibility, raised by the same reviewer, is that the weak cases with stressed inflectional endings somehow caused stress to shift to the stem-final syllable in the strong cases. One could imagine a scenario whereby stress on the weak inflectional endings was reanalyzed in phonological terms as peninitial stress, and on this basis, generalized throughout the paradigm, yielding strong case forms with the innovative stress pattern in (22), which is peninitial in phonological terms and stem-final in morphological terms.109
Strictly speaking, it is impossible to rule out such scenarios, since the relevant changes would be situated in pre-PIE and so by definition separated from the actual facts of the IE languages (see Lundquist & Yates 2018:2135 for further discussion). Still, insofar as the IE data bear on the plausibility of pre-PIE changes, they are not supportive. If it were the case that the phonologically-driven analogy outlined above was a general mechanism of prosodic change in PIE (or in languages like Vedic and Hittite that broadly preserve its prosodic system intact), one might expect to find evidence for a similar spread of peninitial stress in other categories. Thus, for instance, root nouns with stressed inflectional endings in their weak cases could have developed a phonologically uniform, peninitially-stressed paradigm by shifting stress in the strong cases from the root onto inflectional endings. No IE language shows any trace of this development, however (i.e., no attested reflexes of nom.pl x[C(e)C-és], acc.sg x[C(e)C-ṃ́] with peninitial/ending stress).
Similarly, there is little basis for the assumption that the endingless locative of ID *-mon-stems was privileged by IE speakers in such a way that the stem-final stress pattern uniquely associated with this case-form would have replaced earlier root stress in the more numerous (viz., at least three) strong case forms of ID *-mon-stems. In this respect, it is perhaps notable that the Vedic evidence points in the opposite direction: among the direct and indirect reflexes of ID *-mon-stems discussed in section 3 above, not a single stem attests an “endingless locative” in the RV, and even loc.sg forms recharacterized with the ending -i appear to be quite rare (brahmáṇi cited above is an isolated RVic example). The marginal role of the “endingless locative” in Vedic is hardly probative as to its pre-PIE status, given the time-depth separating these two linguistic stages, but at minimum it does not recommend a scenario in which the inflectional paradigm of ID *-mon-stems was re-built on the basis of this single case-form.
I am therefore skeptical of the idea that PIE ID *-mon-stems exhibited AK inflection at any point in their deeper prehistory. It should be emphasized, however, that this is not the main claim of this paper, which is concerned specifically with PIE. I have argued that at this historical stage ID *-mon-stems were characterized by the inflectional pattern in (22) rather than by AK inflection, which would require that the convergent IE evidence for stem-final stress in the strong case forms of ID *-mon-stems be the result of independent parallel innovations. The analogical changes that could give rise to this pattern were just discussed: if they are indeed unlikely as a one-time event, then it is especially improbable that they unfolded in the same way in the separate prehistories of each of these IE languages. In my view, then, there is no viable way to bridge the gap between an AK paradigm for ID *-mon-stems in PIE and the IE data; yet however one assesses the plausibility of this scenario, it is clear that the IE evidence for stem-final stress is most economically explained by reconstructing this stress pattern for PIE, as in (22).
8 The morphophonology of ID *-mon-stems
In the preceding sections it was argued on empirical grounds that PIE ID *-mon-stems should be reconstructed as in (22), with paradigmatic full-grade of the root; stressed *ó-vocalism in their stem-final syllable in the strong cases; and zero-grade suffix and stressed inflectional endings in most of the weak cases. In this section, I discuss a possible theoretical objection to this reconstruction—namely, that in pretonic syllables only zero-grade of the root should occur. The basis for this objection is outlined in 8.1. Having concluded that this full-grade root is indeed phonologically irregular and thus unexpected in a primary derivative, in 8.2 I propose a new explanation for its appearance in PIE ID *-mon-stems. This proposal depends crucially on the fact that PIE ID *-mon-stems are non-primary derivatives, which as observed already by Schindler (1975c:260) may contain phonological properties that are proper to their derivational bases.
8.1 Pretonic root vocalism in ID *-mon-stems and PIE primary derivatives
Descriptively, the traditional AK reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems and the new reconstruction in (22) partially agree on the root vocalism of this category: both reconstruct full-grade of the root in the strong cases, and thus diverge only in the weak, where the former reconstructs zero-grade and the latter full-grade. There is a fundamental morphophonological difference between these reconstructions, however. A correlation between full-grade and word stress is a well-established feature of PIE (see, e.g., Lundquist & Yates 2018:2133–2134 with references). This correlation is maintained on the AK reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems, where the ablaut of the root varies with the position of stress: the root appears in full-grade when it bears stress in the strong cases (e.g., acc.sg *[CéC-mon-m̥]); and in zero-grade when the following inflectional ending bears stress in the weak cases (gen.sg *[CC-n-é/ós]). In contrast, no such correlation obtains in the new reconstruction: the root always exhibits full-grade but never bears stress, which falls instead on the following stem-final vowel in the strong cases (acc.sg *[CeC-món-m̥]) and on the following inflectional endings in the weak cases (gen.sg *[CeC-n-é/ós]).
The invariant pretonic full-grade in the new reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems is especially striking, since there is particularly strong evidence that PIE morphemes regularly appeared in their zero-grade allomorphs in this phonological context. Notably, this generalization is directly encoded in EM, which systematically excludes root *e- and *o-grades in immediately pretonic syllables from the four inflectional classes that it reconstructs for primary athematic nominals. HK nominals are reconstructed with zero-grade of the root and suffixal stress in their strong cases (e.g., nom.sg PIE *dh3-tḗr > Gk.
The same generalization holds in primary derivatives that stand outside the scope of EM proper. Thus, e.g., the ending-stressed plural forms of PIE root presents regularly show zero-grade of the root (e.g., 3pl.prs PIE *h1s-énti > Ved. sánti, Osc. sent, Goth. sind ‘are’), which contrasts with the full-grade root of their root-stressed singular forms (3sg.prs PIE *h1és-ti > Ved. ásti, Hitt. ēšzi, Osc. est, Goth. ist ‘is’). Likewise, suffix-stressed primary thematic derivatives like *-to-adjectives regularly have zero-grade of the root (e.g., anim.nom.sg PIE *k̑lu-tó-s > Ved. śrutás, Gk.
The broad take-away from this brief discussion is that there is robust evidence in the IE languages that pretonic non-zero-grades were phonologically dispreferred in PIE. The pretonic root full-grade reconstructed for ID *-mon-stems in (22) is thus indeed phonologically irregular, a fact which could in principle constitute grounds for rejecting the reconstruction altogether: despite the empirical evidence for stem-final stress in the strong cases, the root full-grade must point to erstwhile root stress, as is the case in EM’s AK reconstruction. In the next section (8.2), however, I argue that this assessment is unwarranted. I contend that it fails to take into account a crucial morphological difference between the forms cited above and PIE ID *-mon-stems—namely, that the former are primary derivatives, but the latter are non-primary derivatives.
8.2 Pretonic root vocalism in ID *-mon-stems and PIE non-primary derivatives
In this section I propose a new account of the phonologically irregular pretonic full-grade root vocalism of PIE ID *-mon-stems as reconstructed in (22). The inspiration for this proposal is a significant but generally overlooked observation of Schindler (1975c:260): “Es besteht dabei generell die Möglichkeit, dass spezifische Ablautstufen der zugrundeliegenden Primärbildungen auch in den sekundären Ableitungen erscheinen.” Schindler does not specify whether his statement is intended as a synchronic claim about the prosodic properties of PIE primary derivatives, or else a diachronic one, the result of einzelsprachlich analogical developments. Whatever the case, it is clear that his observation to some extent holds already for PIE itself. I provide in (23) some examples of non-primary derivation involving overt affixation, the outputs of which are generally thought to be reconstructible for PIE:
(23) PIE “external” non-primary derivatives and their IE reflexes:
a.
PIE
*(h1)rot-eh2-
‘wheel’
*(h1)rot-h2-ó-
‘wheeled’
>
Lat. rota
‘wheel’
Ved. rátha-, YAv. ra
θ a-‘chariot’
b.
PIE
*sokw-eh2-
‘accompaniment’
*sokw-h2-ói-
‘comrade’
>
– ( Gk.
ὀπάων ‘comrade’)
Ved. sákhā(y)-
‘friend’
c.
PIE
*wét-es-
‘year’
*wet-s-ó-
‘having a year’
>
Gk.
ἔτος ‘year’
Ved. vatsá-
‘calf’
d.
PIE
*pék̑-os-
‘wool’
*pek̑-s-ó-
‘having wool’
>
Gk.
πέκος Lat. pecus‘wool’‘herd’
Lat. pexus
‘wooly’
e.
PIE
*léuks-men-
‘light’
*leuks-m̸n-ó–
‘having light’
>
Lat. lūmen
‘light’
YAv. raoxšna-
‘bright’
The derivation of Ved. rátha- in (23a) can be fairly said to reflect the communis opinio, appearing in recent handbooks (e.g., Weiss 2020:126, 320, Meier-Brügger & Fritz 2021:126; cf. NIL: 575–578) and etymological dictionaries (e.g., EWA IIII: 429–430, de Vaan 2008:527). Likewise secure is the derivation of Ved. sákhā(y)- in (23b), which was first proposed by Schindler (1969:164 n. 65) and since widely accepted (e.g., EWA II: 684–685, Beekes 2010:112–113, 1089).112 The frequently cited derivation of Ved. vatśa- in (23c) is supported by the majority of scholars (e.g., Stüber 2002:31, 187–188, Schaffner 2004:292–293, Meissner 2005:153 n. 82, 165).113 Finally, the derivations of Lat. pexus in (23d) and YAv. raoxšna- in (23e) reflect traditional analyses (Schmidt 1895:101–102, Ernout & Meillet 2001:491), which have recently been endorsed respectively by Höfler (2017b:307–308) and Nussbaum (2010:270).114
Each of the examples in (23) exhibit two properties that are of particular interest here. In each case (i) the non-primary derivative is characterized by a non-zero-grade root allomorph in an immediately pretonic syllable: pretonic *o-grade in (23) and pretonic full-grade in (23c–e). From a purely phonological perspective, the root vocalism of these non-primary derivatives is irregular (per 8.1 above). However, in each case (ii) it also has the same root vocalism as its derivational base, which at least for (23c–e) provides a phonological environment in which it is stressed and thus regular.115 These examples thus fit the general pattern observed by Schindler (1975c:260): ablaut grades proper to a derived base also surface in its further derivatives. Furthermore, they suggest that this generalization holds even when the transfer of root ablaut grades from base to derivative results in non-zero-grade root allomorphs that are irregular in their new phonological context.
Significantly, examples of such base-derivative transfer effects are not confined to non-primary derivatives formed by overt affixation. The same phenomenon occurs in “
(24) PIE
τόμος - andτομός -type nominals and their IE reflexes:
a.
PIE
*tómh1-o-
‘slice’
*tomh1-ó-
‘cutting, cutter’
>
Gk.
τόμος ‘slice’
Gk.
τομός ‘cutting’
b.
PIE
*wólh1-o-
‘choice’
*wolh1-ó-
‘choosing, chooser’
>
Ved. vára-
‘choice’
Ved. vará-
‘suitor’
c.
PIE
*k̑óuhx-o-
‘swell’
*k̑ouhx-ó-
‘swelling’
>
– ( Sp. cueva
‘cave’)
Lat. cavus
‘hollow’
Just as in the “external” non-primary derivatives in (23), the internally derived
The non-primary derivatives in (23) and (24) thus open the door for a principled explanation of the root vocalism of PIE ID *-mon-stems. I propose that the pretonic root full-grade of PIE ID *-mon-stems was transferred from their neuter *-men-stem bases, where this full-grade was stressed (cf. 4.2.2 above). This proposal is consistent with Schindler’s (1975c:260) observation regarding base-derivative transfer of ablaut grades, but I explicitly suggest that the transfer of root vocalism was a synchronic phenomenon in PIE ID *-mon-stems, a part of the morphophonological process by which they were derived from neuter *-men-stems. I represent this process schematically in (25), where the attested IE forms can be traced back directly to PIE ID *-mon-stems with base-derivative transfer of root vocalism from their corresponding neuter *-men-stems, not to the pseudo-forms x[dhr̥-món-m̥], x[h2uɡ-m̄́on], and x[dhəh-m̄́on] with phonologically regular pretonic zero-grade of the root (the transferred root vowel is underlined):
(25) Synchronic transfer of root vocalism from PIE neuter *-men-stems to ID *-mon-stem stems:
PIE *[dhér-mn̥]
PIE *[dher-món-m̥]
>
Ved. dharmā́ṇam
( x[dhr̥-món-m̥])
PIE *[h2áuɡ-mn̥]
PIE *[h2auɡ-m̄́on]
>
Lith. augmuõ
( x[h2uɡ-m̄́on])
PIE *[dhéh-mn̥]
PIE *[dheh-m̄́on]
>
Gk.
θημών ( x[dhəh-m̄́on])
On this proposal, PIE ID *-mon-stems would exhibit what are commonly referred to as cyclic effects (or perhaps more intuitively, as “synchronic analogy” by Kiparsky 2015b:3): a phonological property is transferred from a base to its derivative, resulting in the opaque under- or overapplication of an active phonological process in the latter. Specifically, ID *-mon-stems would show underapplication of the PIE vowel deletion process that is responsible for the phonological distribution discussed in 8.1—i.e., that roots and suffixes regularly exhibit zero-grade in pretonic position.118 Such cyclic effects are cross-linguistically common and admit a range of theoretical analyses.119 I make no strong claim here as to which of these analyses is to be preferred. For present purposes, it is sufficient that the base-derivative transfer effect proposed for ID *-mon-stems has solid cross-linguistic parallels and that there are well-established formal mechanisms that can account for their pretonic full-grade while still predicting regular pretonic zero-grade in primary derivatives.
9 Discussion and conclusions
In this paper I have proposed a new prosodic reconstruction of PIE *-mon-stem nominals that were formed by internal derivation from neuter *-men-stems. Above all, I have argued that these nominals were characterized by full-grade of the root and stressed *ó-vocalism of the stem-final syllable in their strong case forms. I repeat this new reconstruction in (26) (= (22) above; [CeC] = full-grade root):
(26) A new reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems:
sg
pl
nom
*[CeC-m̄́on]
*[CeC-món-es]
acc
*[CeC-món-ṃ]
ins
*[CeC-n-éh]
dat
*[CeC-n-éi]
*[CeC-n-ós]
abl
*[CeC-n-é/ós]
gen
*[CeC-n-é/ós]
*[CeC-n-óhom]
loc
*[CeC-mén]
I have argued (in section 7) that the reconstruction in (26) better accounts for the word-prosodic patterns attested in the IE languages than the traditional “amphikinetic” inflection reconstructed by the Erlangen Model. In particular, only this new reconstruction plausibly explains why the direct and indirect reflexes of this category in Vedic Sanskrit consistently show stem-final stress (cf. section 3), a pattern that is matched by the available evidence in Anatolian (cf. section 6). Moreover, the reconstruction in (26) offers the best starting point for explaining the development of AP 3 inflection in the Lithuanian -muo-class (cf. section 4), as well as the mixture of stem-final stress and RA in the direct and indirect reflexes of ID *-mon-stems in Greek (cf. section 5 above). Having established this new reconstruction on empirical grounds, I then addressed (in section 8) a theoretical objection that it may encounter—namely, that a pretonic root full-grade is phonologically irregular in PIE. I argued that PIE ID *-mon-stems acquire this root vocalism from their neuter *-men-stem bases, which had stressed full-grade of the root. In this respect, it was demonstrated that ID *-mon-stems align formally with other securely reconstructible PIE non-primary formations, which likewise exhibit exceptional pretonic root full-grades (or *o-grades) that are phonologically regular in the context of their derivational bases.
One consequence of this new prosodic reconstruction is that the traditional EM analysis of the derivational relationship between neuter *-men-stems and ID *-mon-stems can no longer be maintained. EM analyzes ID as a process whereby an athematic base undergoes a change in its inflectional class membership, switching from one primary inflectional class to a different primary inflectional class. On this view, the derivation of ID *-mon-stems from neuter *-men-stems would involve a switch from PK inflection to AK inflection. However, this analysis is unsatisfactory in at least two respects. The first and most obvious is that the output of this process did not exhibit AK inflection, but instead had strong case forms with stem-final stress as in (26) above.
Yet there is also a deeper conceptual issue. By analyzing ID as involving a switch between primary inflectional classes, EM fundamentally claims that internal derivatives should have the same formal properties as primary derivatives. In other words, there should be nothing on the surface that distinguished, e.g., between (i) a PK *u-stem adjective formed by ID from an AS noun and (ii) a PK *u-stem adjective derived from a (verbal) root, and thus no phonological reason to suspect that (i) is a non-primary derivative but (ii) a primary derivative.120 Yet this type of analysis precisely fails to capture the fact that ID *-mon-stems pattern morphophonologically with other non-primary derivatives, exhibiting phonologically irregular ablaut grades that are proper to their “zugrundeliegenden Primärbildungen” (Schindler 1975c:260).
In 8.2 I advanced a proposal that explicitly accounts for this fact: the root vocalism of PIE neuter *-men-stems was transferred to ID *-mon-stems as a regular part of the synchronic process whereby the latter were derived from the former. A full analysis of the morphophonology of this process is beyond the scope of this paper; for future work in this vein, however, I note that if such an analysis is to be viable, it must account not only for fact that ID *-mon-stems have the same root vocalism as their neuter *-men-stem bases, but also for two prosodic differences between these categories that emerge from this process: (i) a rightward shift in word stress from the root in the base to the stem-final syllable in the derivative; and (ii) the discrepancy in the vocalism of the stem-final syllable (e.g., acc.sg *[-mn̥] vs. *[-món-m̥]).121
The new prosodic reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems in (26) also raises fresh questions about PIE morphophonology—in particular, of non-primary derivatives. One such question concerns the base-derivative transfer effect proposed to account for ID *-mon-stems: is transfer of root vocalism from base to derivative a more general property of synchronic PIE non-primary derivation? It was shown in 8.2 that there are clear examples of PIE non-primary derivatives formed by overt affixation that at least descriptively exhibit this transfer, as well as at least one other category of ID formations,
Acknowledgments
This paper benefited tremendously from the critical feedback of many friends and colleagues, including the members of the Indo-European & Modern Linguistic Theory Research group (especially Ryan Sandell, Jesse Lundquist, and Dieter Gunkel); the participants in the “Indo-European Word Prosody” seminar that I taught at UCLA in the spring of 2020 (especially John Clayton and Teigo Onishi); and the audience of the 31st Annual UCLA Indo-European Conference. I am also very grateful to Nelson Goering, Olav Hackstein, Stephanie Jamison, Jay Jasanoff, Ron Kim, Craig Melchert, Sergio Neri, Thomas Olander, Philomen Probert, Brent Vine, and Michael Weiss, all of whom generously offered their expert advice on various points discussed here, as well as to two anonymous reviewers and the editors at IEL. Finally, I am thankful to the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation, which provided a research fellowship that allowed this paper to be brought to completion.
Abbreviations
ALEW2 |
Hock, Wolfgang, Rainer Fecht, Anna H. Feulner, Eugen Hill, & Dagmar S. Wodtko (eds.). 2019. Altlitauisches etymologisches Wörterbuch. Version 2.0. |
CHD |
Güterbock, Hans G., Harry A. Hoffner, & Theo van den Hout (eds.). 1989. The Hittite dictionary of the Oriental Institute of Chicago. Chicago: The Oriental Institute of the University of Chicago. |
DELG |
Chantraine, Pierre. 1968–80 [1999]. Dictionnaire étymologique de la langue grecque. Histoire des mots2. Paris: Klincksieck. |
DMic |
Aura Jorro, Francisco. 1985–93. Diccionario micénico. 2 vols. Madrid: Consejo superior de investigaciones científicas. |
EWA |
Mayrhofer, Manfred. 1986–2001. Etymologisches Wörterbuch des Altindoarischen. 3 vols. Heidelberg: Winter. |
KEWA |
Mayrhofer, Manfred. 1956–64. Kurzgefasstes etymologisches Wörterbuch des Altindischen. 4 vols. Heidelberg: Winter. |
LIV2 |
Rix, Helmut & Martin J. Kümmel (ed.). 2001. Lexikon der indogermanischen Verben. Die Wurzeln und ihre Primärstammbildungen2. Wiesbaden: Reichert. |
LSJ |
Liddell, Henry G., Robert Scott, Henry Stuart Jones, Roderick McKenzie, Peter G.W. Glare, & Anne A. Thompson. 1996. Greek-English lexicon with a revised supplement9. Oxford: Clarendon Press. |
NIL |
Wodtko, Dagmar S., Britta Sofie Irslinger, & Carolin Schneider (ed.). 2008. Nomina im indogermanischen Lexikon. Heidelberg: Winter. |
TLG |
Pantelia, Maria (ed.). 1972. Thesaurus Linguae Graecae. University of California, Irvine. |
See Schindler (1975a:63–64), Widmer (2004:69), Rau (2009:134), Fortson (2010:122–123), Nussbaum (2014b:244, 248), Weiss (2020:281–282), i.a.
In Vedic *-mon-stems can be distinguished from animate *-men-stems only in the acc.sg, nom.pl, and nom/acc.du, where the former have a suffixal long vowel reflecting PIE *o (via Brugmann’s Law; Brugmann 1876) vs. a short vowel in the latter reflecting PIE *e. For this reason I cite the acc.sg in (1) and in subsequent Vedic examples below.
First codified by Rix (1976/1992:121–124), EM represents the culmination of earlier research by Pedersen (1926, 1933), Kuiper (1942), and especially Schindler (1967, 1969, 1972, 1975b,c, 1994). Overviews of EM can be found in most recent IE handbooks, e.g., Tichy (2004:75–81), Clackson (2007:79–89), Fortson (2010:119–123), Weiss (2020:276–282), Meier-Brügger & Fritz (2021:207–224). In this paper, I assume that EM’s reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems as an AK class represents a claim about their inflection in PIE, here defined as the last stage of the proto-language that is ancestral to all of the IE languages (viz., including the Anatolian branch). This is not the only possible interpretation of EM’s reconstruction, however, which instead may be understood as a claim about their inflection at a deeper (i.e., pre-PIE) stage; see 7.3 below for further discussion.
Differently, Tremblay (1996:128–132) reconstructs “anakinetic” inflection—i.e., stem-final stress in the strong cases, root stress in the weak. Among other issues, this hypothesis fails to account for the Vedic evidence for stressed inflectional endings in the weak cases of ID *-mon-stems, which are an inherited feature both under the AK reconstruction and under the new prosodic reconstruction advanced here (see 7.2 for discussion).
I provisionally limit my claim to the anim.nom.sg, acc.sg, and nom.pl due to uncertainties about whether the anim.acc.pl was a strong case in PIE (as in Greek, Hittite, and probably Balto-Slavic) or a weak case (as in Indo-Iranian).
The nom.sg of the PIE *-mon-stems in (2) and in subsequent examples below is reconstructed here with a word-final nasal. It is commonly assumed that already in PIE *n was lost in word-final position after *ō (Schindler 1974:5, Harðarson 1987a:118–121, Jasanoff 1989:138, 2002:35; cf. Mayrhofer 1986:159, Byrd 2015:21), given its absence in this environment in several IE branches (Anatolian, Balto-Slavic, Celtic, Indo-Iranian, Italic). However, it is also possible that it occurred independently in the prehistories of these branches, especially since most of them (viz., all except Italic and Celtic) also show loss of word-final *n after *ē (cf. the cautious formulations of Melchert 1983:10 n. 25 and Weiss 2020:331 n. 49). I assume the latter, but which of these historical scenarios is correct does not materially affect the analysis of the *-mon-stems developed here.
See Watkins (1962:181–185), Melchert (1983:23), McCone (1995:4), Stüber (1998:145–146), Remmer (2002–2003:173–181), i.a.
While Weiss (2017:387 n. 53) rightly observes that across-branch word equations for non-primary derivatives formed with an independent suffix *-mon- are lacking, I discuss examples below (in 5.1, 5.2, and section 6) of apparent primary derivatives formed with this suffix that are supported by (near) word equations (cf. Stüber 1998:145–146). See also Yates (2020b) for fuller discussion of this material, including arguments that an independent suffix *-mon- is reconstructible for PIE.
In fact, Schindler (1975c:263) did not restrict this process to just neuter or just PK nominals (he cites Gk.
While the reflexes of BV *-mon-stems consistently exhibit *o-vocalism, the evidence for *o-vocalism in other BV s analyzed by EM as AK is less compelling; see now Lundquist (2021a), who raises doubts about the PIE status of the suffixal *o-grade in BV s whose 2Ms exhibit “hysterokinetic” inflection as independent words (e.g., Gk.
The claim of EM is often formulated in an imprecise way, such that it is unclear whether AK inflection is a property of the compound as a whole (cf. Kiparsky 2010:169–170, Lundquist 2021a) or just of the 2M, and if the latter, whether the AK stress pattern obtains only at a level prior to compounding or on the surface. Stüber’s (1998:44) formulation above is merely representative in this respect; the ambiguity is present already in Schindler’s (1975c:263) seminal discussion. Jasanoff (2017:20–21) is somewhat more precise in attributing AK inflection just to the 2M.
The term used by Pānin̥i is prakṛtyā ‘according to its natural place’ (6.2.1; cf. Wackernagel 1905:291, Lundquist 2016:10–11, 2017:94–95). While there are exceptions to this rule in BV *-mon-stems, these exceptions are of the same kind as found in BV s generally (e.g., in BV s with 1M a(n)-, su-, or dus-; see the discussion in Wackernagel 1905:291–302 and n. 16 below).
On “recessive accentuation” and the “Law of Limitation” generally see Probert (2006b:128–144) and Gunkel (2014a,b), and for a survey of theoretical analyses Probert (2010). On the origin of the “Law of Limitation” see Probert (2012).
The n.nom/acc.sg forms in particular show that these BV s do not bear a lexical accent on the root, since accented adjectives always show the same non-RA stress pattern in the n.nom/acc.sg as, e.g., in the m.nom.sg (
The only Greek BV s that do not regularly exhibit RA are compound *-es-stem adjectives, on which see n. 91 below.
Brugmann (1905–1906:65) saw a trace of 1M stress in Germanic compounds like OE fyðer-fēte ‘four-footed’ (cf. Ved. cátur-pad- ‘id.’), which would add further support for reconstructing a Vedic-like situation for PIE. Note, however, that I do not exclude the possibility that certain PIE BV s had 2M member stress—in particular, BV s in which the 1M was not a lexical word (cf. Lundquist 2017:97–110 on *h1su-). Such cases merit further investigation.
On the seemingly divergent meanings of this lexical item in IE and in Greek see Beckwith 1998 with references.
In Vedic and the other Indo-Iranian languages, animate *-mon-stems can be distinguished from animate *-men-stems only in the acc.sg, nom.pl, and nom/acc.du; I therefore preferentially cite these case-forms whenever they are attested.
On the unexpected short vowel in the stem-final syllable of asmanəm see de Vaan (2003:130).
It is in principle possible that Lith. ašmuõ ‘cutting edge’ (attested mostly in nom.pl ãšmenys) and/or Latv. asmens ‘id.’ are reflexes of a neuter *-men-stem to *h2ek̑-. Yet if Lith. akmuõ and Latv. akmens show the regular outcome of *k̑m (with conditioned depalatalization of *k̑; see Matasović 2005:368, Derksen 2015:47), then Lith. ašmuõ and Latv. asmens ‘id.’ are better explained as inner-Baltic formations; see Pardini (2011:30), who argues for independent formations in each language from their respective reflexes of the root *h2ek̑- (cf. 4.1 below).
See NIL: 288–300 for a much more extensive list of derivatives of *h2ek̑- with discussion and references.
NIL: 290–291 n. 7: “Gew[öhnlich] zu dieser W[urzel] gestellt, doch ist für *h2ák̑-mon- eine bereits grundsprachlich lexikalisierte Bedeutung ‘Stein’ deutlicher faßbar, als es bei anderen Nominalbildungen von *h2ak̑-… der Fall ist.”
A separate question is whether in PIE the weak cases of *h2ek̑mon- had stressed inflectional endings—i.e., exhibited AK stress mobility, as traditionally assumed (e.g., NIL: 291)—or else root stress, as the combined evidence of Vedic, Greek, and Old Lithuanian suggests. Standardly cited as evidence for erstwhile AK mobility are weak case forms like gen.sg Ved. áśnas and YAv. ašnō, where the deletion of the vowel in the stem-final syllable—and, as a consequence, of the preceding */m/ in the resulting /.Cmn/ cluster—are widely assumed to be conditioned by stress shifting to the inflectional ending (see, e.g., Schmidt 1895:87–159, Mayrhofer 1986:159, Kiparsky 2010:149, Nussbaum 2010). However, the empirical basis for this assumption is questionable. In Indo-Iranian, at least, deletion of the stem-final vowel occurs also in certain categories attested with fixed root stress such as *-tor-stem agent nouns (e.g., Ved. hótre ‘for the offerer(/priest)’ = YAv. zao
The word n dhárman- in (7b) has a number of senses in the RV (see Brereton 2004), which are also reflected in the derived agent noun m dharmán-; the latter can thus mean both ‘supporter’ (e.g., RV X.92.2b) and ‘one with the power to issue ordinances’ (e.g., IX.97.23c), which correspond respectively the meanings of its base ‘support’ (e.g., X.50.6c) and ‘ordinance’ (e.g., VII.89.5c; cf. Jamison & Brereton 2014:996, 1340, 1458, 1542).
In (8a), n óman- ‘aid’ is attested in ins.pl ómabhis (hapax in RV, 5.43.13b). In (8b–c), n váriman- and várṣman– are attested only in loc.sg: váriman (5× in RV, e.g., IV.54.4c); várṣman (5× in RV, e.g., III.5.9b).
A possible third example is Ved. hemán- ‘impulsion’ (ins.sg hemánā, RV IX.97.1a), generally connected with OAv. zaēman- ‘activity’ (n.acc.sg zāemā, Y 44.5); see Wackernagel & Debrunner (1954:759), EWA II: 819.
On the development of neuter *-men-stems in Tocharian see Adams (2015:178). TA okäm is almost certainly an exact match for TB auki, but the meaning is opaque (perhaps ‘circumspect’; see Adams 2013:137).
Ved. ojmán- must have retained a synchronic connection with the root uj- ‘strong’ (cf. ój-as- ‘strength’), since the root-final j is not the phonologically regular outcome of *g in this context (xogmán– is expected; see Wackernagel & Debrunner 1954:754–755 for discussion).
The long vowel (ī ) is unexpected, perhaps attributable to metrical lengthening (see Wackernagel & Debrunner 1954:763 with references).
For the PIE roots see LIV2: 180–181, 538, 599–600. The last is reconstructed by LIV2 as a “State II” root *g̑hweh-, so the neuter *-men-stem with apparent “State I” shape is perhaps better viewed as an inner-Indic creation. Its formation would thus parallel Ved. párī-man- ‘fullness’ (RV IX.71.3d), whose PIE root is manifestly “State II” *pleh- (cf. LIV2: 482–483).
The application of Brugmann’s Law (cf. n. 2 above) in n.nom/acc.sg Ved. dā́ru ‘(piece of) wood’ (< PIE *dóru; cf. Gk.
The only exception is bhū-mán- in (8e) with the same exceptional zero-grade seen in its neuter base. This property can be attributed to the verbal root underlying this ID pair, which is often reconstructed as *bhuhx- with non-ablauting zero-grade of the root (e.g., Jasanoff 2003:112 n. 252).
On the basis of this usage, Rau (2009:72–74) includes *-mon- as part of the “Caland system” in PIE. Examples like (8b–e) and (9a) are perhaps better understood synchronically as part of the same productive pattern of derivation from adjectival roots; see Wackernagel & Debrunner (1954:754–755) and Kiparsky (to appear) for discussion.
Macdonell (1910:128) lists just four exceptions: Ved. áś-man-, ó-man-, jé-man-, bhás-man-. Other than áś-man- ‘stone’ discussed in 2.3 above, none of these occurs in a strong case form that unambiguously identifies it as a *-mon-stem, and at least one is better analyzed as a neuter (ómabhis, RV V.43.13b; cf. n. 25 above); see further Wackernagel & Debrunner (1954:757, 765–766).
I assume the merger of these categories was a post-Proto-Baltic development in view of the Old Prussian evidence discussed in n. 47 below. It must in any case be a post-Proto-Baltic-Slavic development, since the inherited distinction between animate and neuter *-mVn-stems is preserved in Slavic. The animate *-mVn-stems could in principle offer insight into the reconstruction of PIE ID *-mon-stems, since in some Slavic languages they show a nom/acc.sg ending that appears to derive from this source (e.g., OCS -my < *-mōn). It is difficult, however, to draw any conclusions from Slavic animate *-mVn-stems as a class, since they do not seem to pattern together prosodically (see Pronk 2009:107–112 for discussion), nor from individual lexemes in this class, since I am not aware of any with ID *-mon-stems cognate elsewhere in IE. I therefore discuss the Slavic evidence no further here.
For the attestation of ṡ́emenys see Senn (1966:138–139) and ALEW2, s.v. ṡ́eti; for akmuõ and piemuõ see Illič-Svityč (1979:49, 59) and ALEW2, s.vv.
It is notable that, with the exception of akmuõ, most or perhaps all of these -muo-class nouns with AP 1 in Old Lithuanian have the phonological environment for Hirt’s Law (Hirt 1929), whereby stress was retracted to an immediately preceding syllable if that syllable ended in a consonantal laryngeal (see Olander 2009:149–150 with references). A root-final laryngeal is well-established for ṡ́emenys (< *seh- ‘press; sow’) and st(u)omuõ (< *steh- ‘stand’), and plausibly assumed for ṙ́emuo, although it lacks a secure PIE root etymology (see Smoczyński 2018:1081, 1154, 1318–1319). To account for piemuõ it is proposed by Rasmussen (1989:32–33, 272) that PIE *poh2i-mén- metathesized to *poih2-mén-, whence initial stress by Hirt’s Law.
Thus, e.g., de Saussure (1896:533), Pedersen (1933:24–26), Illič-Svityč (1979:65–69), Derksen (1996:25), Kortlandt (2006:349), Olander (2009:202), Jasanoff (2017:109), Kim (2018:1982), Petit (2018:237). For a full review of literature on the phenomenon of “Balto-Slavic accentual mobility” see Olander (2009:14–52) (cf. Jasanoff 2017:108–116, Olander 2018, Kim 2019a).
A principled exception is the zero-grade in Ved. bhū́man– ‘earth’; see n. 32 above. Note that by restricting my claim to deverbal neuter *-men-stems I exclude the inherited word for ‘name’ (> Lat. nōmen, Ved. nā́man–, Gk.
Both Melchert (2007/8:184–186) and Nussbaum (2010:276 n. 244) suggest that PIE had at least a few “*o/e-acrostatic” deverbal neuter *-men-stems, but see Vine (2019) for critical discussion and alternative explanations of the relevant forms. More generally, see Nussbaum (2010:271–272) for a very different view of the prosody of neuter *-men-stems.
RA is more generally a property of all Greek third declension polysyllabic neuter nouns (Probert 2003:84).
See CHD Š: 239 for attestations. Per Gertz (1982:29) nom/acc.pl Hitt. šarāma ‘ration breads’ is best explained as a direct reflex of a PIE form with final *-mōn and thus supports its reconstruction as such in neuter *-men-stems (as argued in 4.2.2 below).
The reconstruction of the nom/acc.pl of PIE neuter *-men-stems is discussed further in 4.2.2 below. It is commonly assumed (following Schindler 1975b:9) that the *-s allomorph of the gen.sg ending serves as a diagnostic of erstwhile PK inflection, and thus that neuter *-men-stems should be assigned to this inflectional class, since some of their Avestan reflexes (e.g., OAv. casmə̄ṇg ‘of sight’, YAv. dām̨an ‘of the creation’ < *-men-s) appear to show this ending (Gotō 2013:42, Meier-Brügger & Fritz 2021:215, i.a.). Yet since this allomorph also occurs in “acrostatic” nominals (e.g., PIE *négw-t-s > Hitt. nekuz (mēḫur) ‘(time) of evening’; cf. NIL: 504–505) and in other nominals standardly reconstructed with fixed initial stress (e.g., PIE *bhréh2ter-s > Ved. bhrātur, OE broþor ‘of the brother’; cf. NIL: 38, Fortson 2010:116), its occurrence in neuter *-men-stems is also perfectly consistent with the reconstruction of fixed root stress in this PIE category. For further discussion of the distribution of gen.sg *-s see Kiparsky (2010:149–153) and Jasanoff (2017:27–28).
Jasanoff (2017:166–168) focuses on the inherited word for ‘name’, which in my view should not be taken as representative for the properties of PIE neuter *-men-stems in general (see n. 40 above). The question considered here, then, is whether an account along the lines of what he proposes for ‘name’ can be extended to ordinary deverbal neuter *-men-stems. While I argue it cannot, this does not entail rejecting his analysis of ‘name’, which potentially explains why this lexical item—unlike deverbal neuter *-men-stems (see n. 52 below)—has a mobile paradigm (AP c) securely reconstructible for PSl. (cf. Pronk 2009:110–112).
This claim has been challenged by Olander (2015:85–86), who argues that PIE *-mṇ regularly yielded PSl. -m̨e. If correct, it would obviate the need to assume an HK collective in Slavic just like the analogical account advanced by Kim (2019b) and others discussed below.
As discussed especially by Jasanoff (2002) (cf. Nussbaum 2014a:292–294), there are good IE parallels for n.nom/acc.sg forms being replaced by n.nom/acc.pl forms. This case, however, would not involve a wholesale replacement, since Slavic neuter *-men-stems regularly reflect full-grade of the root (e.g., OCS sěm̨e < *séh1-, vrěm̨e < *wért-), not the zero-grade proper to HK inflection.
A PBS date for this change is suggested by n.nom/acc.sg OPr. semen, which could continue *-mḗn, but can also reflect the form *-men argued to underlie PSl. *-m̨e below. The Old Prussian evidence is significant because it alone of the Baltic languages appears to preserve a trace of the inherited distinction between animate and neuter *-mVn-stems. The occurrence of nom.sg semen without final -s in the Elbing Vocabulary ensures that it continues a neuter (see Petit 2010:141–153).
The scenario proposed by Jasanoff (2017:168) differs somewhat, but likewise produces a Proto(-Balto)-Slavic paradigm with nom/acc.sg *-mḗn, weak *-mén-.
As expected on this analysis, other Vedic animate nouns formed with the suffix *-en- are similarly nasal-less, e.g., nom.sg vṛ́ṣā ‘bull’, ukṣā́ ‘ox’ (< PIE *-ḗn).
A few Vedic neuter *-men-stems have nom/acc.pl forms in -mā or -ma (e.g., dhármā̆ ‘foundations’). The former could in principle reflect PIE *-mō(n) without analogically restored nasal, but I follow Harðarson’s (1987b:97) derivation of both forms from an innovative *-mṇ-h2. Structurally, *-mṇ-h2 (or *-mṇ-H in PInd. terms) could have been built on the model of neuter *-u-stems, where nom/acc.pl = n.nom/acc.sg + pluralizing *-h2 (e.g., n.nom/acc.sg Ved. vásu : pl vásū ‘good(s)’ < *-u : *-u-h2). See also n. 51 below for the possibility that *-h2 was used already in PIIr. to recharacterize certain n.nom/acc.pl forms that have lost this marker by sound change.
The creation of the recharacterized desinence -mān-H is perhaps datable to PIIr., since Avestan also has neuter *-men-stems with nom/acc.pl in -mānı̄̆ (OAv. afšmānī ‘damages’, YAv. cinmāni ‘desires’). Yet if so, it would be necessary to explain how the congenitors of the OAv. forms in -m̨an/–m̨am either escaped recharacterization or else subsequently lost *H. I tentatively assume that the same recharacterization occurred independently in Indic and, incompletely, in Iranian (cf. Harðarson 1987b:96), but this assumption has no consequences for the point at hand.
Unlike Jasanoff’s (2017:166–168) proposal, this analysis does not predict stress mobility in ordinary Slavic neuter *-men-stems (see n. 44 above on ‘name’). It is difficult to evaluate these differing predictions, since the prosodic facts in this class are complicated and often contradictory across the Slavic languages (for thorough discussions of the evidence see Snoj 1993 and Pronk 2009). This issue calls for further research.
Leveling of the strong stem vocalism explains, e.g., the apparent full-grade in gen.sg OLith. dukterès (< PIE *dhugh2tr-é/ós; cf. Gk.
In some cases, it is difficult to determine whether a *-mon-stem nominal is a noun or properly an adjective used substantively (on this phenomenon in general in Greek see, e.g., Probert 2006b:156). For the *-mon-stems discussed below, I follow the classification of LSJ except where otherwise noted.
The nouns in (14d–f) with stem-final stress appear to have generalized the stem-final long vowel of the nom.sg throughout the paradigm. This analogical leveling did not affect all *-mon-stem nouns with stem-final stress, however; the non-primary *-mon-stem agent nouns discussed below have a short vowel in these case-forms (gen.sg -
An alternative possibility is that
For the etymologies see, e.g., Beekes (2010:1469, 1482–1483, 1619–1620), NIL: 100, 162. On the non-vocalization of root-final *h2 in Gk.
Buck & Petersen (1945:232) actually list a neuter *-men-stem
I follow Schwyzer’s (1939:522) view that *-mon-stems derived from contract verbs in -
On the derivations in (15f–g) cf. Weiss (2017:386), but note that
Chantraine (1933:173) views both words as archaisms in Greek. While
A PN
See Weiss (2017:386 n. 51), who suggests that the Latin PN Tellumō may reflect the same pre-form.
Beekes (2010:1446–1447) assumes that Hesychius preserves an archaism, seeing in
For the connection between
For discussion see Jasanoff (2003:104–107), who similarly posits an analogical spread of *deh2i- in these forms.
Gk.
There are no well-attested counter-examples to this generalization among the non-compound *-mon-stem adjectives cited by Stratton (1899:126–134), Schwyzer (1939:522) or Buck & Petersen (1945:216–220). Among less frequent stems a possible example is
According to the TLG,
In classifying
The initial *h1 in *h1yeh1- ‘throw’ (reconstructed with *H in LIV2: 225) is in accordance with Bozzone’s Law (Bozzone 2013).
See Beekes (2010:38–39, 1186). An etymological relationship between Gk.
On the development of -
Consistent with this view is the absence of clear word equations across IE branches for non-primary neuter *-men-stems. The mechanism(s) for extending neuter *-men- from primary into non-primary derivation within the IE languages are discussed already by Brugmann (1906:236–237).
A rare exception to this generalization is
A possible third example is
The Vedic noun is attested in the ins.sg vidmánā (6× in RV) and, used infinitivally, in the dat.sg vidmáne (RV I.164.6b, X.88.18d). The absence of strong case forms precludes definitive identification of vidmán- as a *-mon-stem, but given the poverty of animate *-men-stems it is the likeliest analysis.
The full-grade occurs in
For more detailed discussion of this rule see Vine 1998:66–70 and Höfler 2017a with references.
In classifying
In (17f–g) the feminine suffix of the base surfaces in its full-grade form (*-eh2-) in the derived *-mon-stem adjectives
The others occur first in classical Greek:
On the issue of vowel length see n. 59 above. It is generally accepted that a similar resegmentation took place in neuter *-men-stems, resulting in a productive suffix -
Haspelmath (1995) refers to this type of morphological change as “affix telescoping.”
More sporadically, new *-mon-stem adjectives may also have been produced within the historical period by back-formation from compounds. Two likely examples of this kind were discussed above,
See Schwyzer (1939:522–523, 732) for general discussion of the development of verbs in -
There is, however, one Greek noun in -
Notably, Rasmussen’s (2001:27) conclusion that “the accented *ó of *me-món-e is simply the reflex of original, accented /o/” is consistent with Jasanoff’s (2018) recent analysis of the development of the PNIE perfect.
This conclusion was also reached by Bonfante (1930), who proposed that the forms with RA result from a phonological process that shifted stress from a final heavy syllable onto a penultimate heavy syllable. Much of his evidence for this process is better explained in other ways, however; see Probert (2006a:129–131) for discussion.
Some exceptions to this rule are discussed by Lundquist (2016, 2017:61–110, 2021b), who argues partially on this basis that stem-final stress is a Greek innovation in *-es-stem BV compounds, a replacement for inherited 1M stress (cf. Tucker 1990:62, Meissner 2005:200, Blanc 2018:304–305).
Synchronically, -
The first attestation of
Nussbaum’s (2014b:254–255) proposal addresses cases in which *-mon-stems are paired with (virtually) synonymous cognate primary neuter *-men-stems—thus Gk.
Probert’s evidence for this change comes from thematic adjectives—e.g., *-no-, *-mo-, *-ro-, and *-to-stems (2006b:289–284)—and from deverbative *-eh2-stems (Probert 2006a). For further discussion of why default stress often emerges in lexicalized words—and conversely, why non-default stress patterns are more stable in synchronically derived words—see Yates (2015:176–178) and in more detail Sandell (2015:192–214).
For diverse evidence that RA is the default stress pattern in Greek see Probert (2006b:128–144).
The other Anatolian *-mon-stems discussed by Yates (2020b) are either non-probative with respect to their inherited stress pattern (Hitt. arkamman- ‘tribute’) or perhaps point to stem-final stress (Lyd. sadmẽ- ‘seal’ < *seh1i-món- with vowel weakening in the unstressed root syllable).
Thus LIV2: 544; for the morphological reconstruction of Hitt. išḫ(a)i-, however, I follow Jasanoff (2018:144–145), who derives it from an *h2e-conjugation aorist PIE *sh2ói–/*sh2éi- (with early prehistoric replacement of the original weak stem by zero-grade *sh2i–′; for the pattern see Melchert 2013). Kloekhorst (2008:391–393) likewise reconstructs *sh2ói–/*sh2i–′ ablaut, but with a different morphological analysis.
In New Script texts a few aberrant forms of the noun are also attested (e.g., acc.sg [i]šḫimenan), on which basis Oettinger (1982:167–168, 173–175, 2003:146–147) reconstructs an animate HK *-men-stem as the source of išḫiman-. The principal issue with this analysis is that it fails to account for the older and more abundant evidence for [áː] (< *ó) in the derivational suffix; for discussion and further arguments in support of *sh2i-món- see Yates (2020b:252–256).
On the reduplicative pattern see Yates & Zukoff (2018).
Ved. sīmán- refers to a part of the head, perhaps the crest or the hairline, in its earliest occurrence (acc.sg sīmā́nam, AVŚ IX.8.13); this can be understood as a specialized sense of the meaning ‘boundary’ found in later Vedic texts.
A single Vedic exception, bhū-mán- ‘abundance’ in (8e), is discussed in n. 32 above.
A secondary consideration is that the reconstruction of the weak case forms is problematized by a number of outstanding morphological and phonological questions at the PIE level, which lie beyond the scope of this study to resolve (see, e.g., nn. 104 and 105 below). The conclusions presented here should accordingly be regarded as provisional, pending a separate treatment of these forms and the relevant questions elsewhere.
I include in (21) all weak cases with inflectional endings whose PIE reconstruction can be regarded as relatively secure (cf. Lundquist & Yates 2018:2182–2183). I therefore exclude the non-singular weak cases that may have been marked by consonant-initial endings, which have questionable PIE status, especially in view of their absence in Anatolian; in this vein see Jasanoff (2008:140–141), who argues persuasively for reconstructing dat.pl PIE *-os (> Hitt. -aš) rather than *-bh(y)os (> Ved. -bhyas, Lat. -bus) or *-mos (> Lith. -mus, OCS -mŭ). It should be noted, however, that such consonant-initial weak forms would be of significant prosodic interest, since these constitute another locus in which Kiparsky’s (2010)’s analysis of ID *-mon-stems (discussed in n. 107 below) predicts a non-AK stress pattern. The historical development of these consonant-initial endings calls for further research.
The reconstruction of this process is supported by forms in a variety of IE languages (see Schmidt 1895:87–159, Nussbaum 2010, Pronk 2014, i.a.), which notably include at least one word equation: Ved. budhná- ‘deep’ = Lat. fundus ‘id.’ < PIE *bhudh-n-ó-, which shows */m/-deletion vis-à-vis its derivational base, the animate *-men-stem PIE *bhudh-mén- (> Gk.
Stüber (1998:147): “[T]he full-grade of the root appears to have been generalized almost universally.” The one example that she adduces of a PIE root zero-grade is Gk.
The proposed reconstruction of the strong and weak cases in (21) are wholly compatible, as Kiparsky (2010:143–149) has demonstrated. I provisionally follow his analysis of stress assignment in these forms: the stem-final vowel bears a lexical accent in the input (*/-món-/; cf. n. 121 below), which explains why it attracts stress in the strong cases; when the stem-final vowel is deleted in the weak cases, this lexical accent then shifts to accented vowel-initial endings (via “secondary mobility;” see further Yates 2020a), which therefore attract stress. Notably, this analysis predicts stem-final stress in the context of consonant-initial weak case endings rather than the stressed inflectional endings expected on the AK reconstruction. Whether such endings are reconstructible for PIE is uncertain (cf. n. 104 above); if reconstructible, though, the relevant Vedic forms (e.g., m.ins.pl -má-bhis, dat.pl -má-bhyas) suggest they would be consistent with Kiparsky’s predictions.
It is possible that some of the scholars who endorse EM’s reconstruction of ID *-mon-stems as an AK class (see n. 1 above) do so as a claim about pre-PIE rather than PIE itself. This view would be in keeping with the original conception of EM’s inflectional classes, which—as emphasized by early researchers like Pedersen (1933:21) and Schindler (1975c:259–261)—are the output of internal reconstruction, intended to serve as a pre-PIE starting point for analyzing the subsequent development of (P)IE athematic nominal inflection (cf. Lundquist & Yates 2018:2134). In this camp would belong, e.g., scholars who would use the “*kwetwóres-Regel” as a mechanism for explaining their IE reflexes with stem-final stress (cf. 5.3.2 above).
Proponents of EM should be reluctant to posit this sort of phonologically-driven analogy as a mechanism for prosodic change in the proto-language, since it is at odds with the core assumption of this model that stress was fundamentally a morphologically-determined property.
On the EM inflectional classes see the literature cited in n. 3 above. The PIE reconstructions of ‘path’, ‘thought’, and ‘giver’ are standard; see, e.g., Fortson (2010:124–125, 234).
The PIE reconstructions of ‘be’, ‘heard (of)’, and ‘bear’ are standard; see, e.g., Lundquist & Yates (2018:2122, 2127, 2133).
On the prosody of PIE *sokw-h2-ói-—and of PIE animate *-oi-stems generally—see Yates (2019b), who argues that stem-final stress is preserved in the BV compound Ved. su-ṣakhā́(y)– ‘having good fellowship’. The same stem-final stress pattern is reconstructed for the simplex by Gotō (2013:29), though without argumentation.
Vine (2009) proposes instead that both Ved. vatsá- and CLuw. ušša/i–/HLuw. usa/i- ‘year’ (< *ut-s-ó-) are decasuative formations (on which process see now Fortson 2020), derived with the same adjectival suffix */-ó-/ but from the gen.sg of the root noun continued by Hitt. witt- ‘year’.
For the neuter *-men-stem base in (23e) I reproduce the segmentation of Nussbaum (2010:270), who appears to derive it from an *s-extended form of the root *leuk- ‘shine’ (see LIV2: 418–419). Alternatively, Höfler (2017b:464–465) argues that YAv. raoxšna- is a *-no-adjective derived from a neuter *-es-stem *léuk-es- (> Ved. rokás-, YAv. raocah- ‘light’), but this analysis is problematized by the weak comparative evidence for *-no- as a non-primary suffix in PIE.
I am aware of no evidence as to whether the bases in (23a–b) were root-stressed (*(h1)rót-eh2-, *sókw-eh2-) or suffix-stressed (*(h1)rot-éh2- and *sokw-éh2-). If the latter, supposing the generalization proposed in this section is on the right track, their pretonic root *o-grade may be a sign of their own non-primary status.
See, e.g., Schaffner (2001:98), Widmer (2004:32), Fortson (2010:122), Nussbaum (2014b:243–251), 2017:237–239, Jasanoff (2017:21–22), Lundquist & Yates 2018:2108–2109, Weiss 2020:287. Differently, Benveniste (1935:172) and Krasukhin (2000:133–134) argue on semantic grounds for the opposite direction of derivation, taking the
The examples in (24a–b) are commonly cited in the literature in n. 116 above (e.g., Jasanoff 2017:21–22). On (24c) see Vine (2006:235–237).
How precisely this PIE deletion process should be analyzed is not crucial here. One possibility is Kiparsky’s (2010; 2018) “Zero-Grade Rule,” but for a different conception see Yates (2019a).
Bermúdez-Otero (2011) and Kiparsky (2015b) provide recent overviews and critical discussion of this literature.
Nussbaum (1998:147 n. 63) explicitly allows for both types of formations in PIE.
Kiparsky (2010) and Keydana (2013) provide two possible analyses of (i); for a provisional analysis of (ii) see Yates (2019c).
Notably, Höfler (2015, 2017b) reconstructs for PIE both this “double zero-grade” pattern and the more familiar type with root-full grade represented by Ved. vatsá- ‘calf’ in (23c) and Lat. pexus ‘wooly’ in (23d) above.
References
Adams, Douglas Q. 2013. AdictionaryofTocharianB. Revised and greatly enlarged. Amsterdam / New York: Rodopi.
Adams, Douglas Q. 2015. TocharianB. Agrammarofsyntaxandword-formation. Innsbruck: Institut für Sprachwissenschaft der Universität Innsbruck.
Bammesberger, Alfred. 1973. AbstraktbildungenindenbaltischenSprachen. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.
Beckwith, Miles C. 1998. The ‘hanging of Hera’ and the meaning of Greek ἄκμων. HarvardStudiesinClassicalPhilology 98: 91–102.
Beekes, Robert. 2010. EtymologicaldictionaryofGreek. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Beekes, Robert S.P. 1969. ThedevelopmentoftheProto-Indo-EuropeanlaryngealsinGreek. The Hague: de Gruyter.
Benveniste, Émile. 1935. Originesdelaformationdesnomsenindo-européen. Paris: Maisonneuve.
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 2011. Cyclicity. In van Oostendorp, Marc & Ewen, Colin J. & Hume, Elizabeth & Rice, Keren (eds.), TheBlackwellcompaniontophonology, 2019–2048. Oxford / Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Blanc, Alain. 2018. Lesadjectifssigmatiquesdugrecancien. Uncasdemétamorphismedérivationnel. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft.
Bonfante, Giuliano. 1930. L’accento delle parole greche a esito spondaico. Studiitalianidifilologiaclassica 8: 265–295.
Bozzone, Chiara. 2013. Initial “yod” in Greek and the etymology of Gk. ἵππος ‘horse’. In Jamison, Stephanie W. & Melchert, H. Craig & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe24thAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 1–26. Bremen: Hempen.
Brereton, Joel P. 2004. dhárman in the R̥gveda. JournalofIndianphilosophy 32: 449–489.
Brugmann, Karl. 1876. Zur Geschichte der stammabstufenden Declinationen. Erste Abhandlung: Die Nomina auf -ar- und -tar-. (Curtius’)StudienzurgriechischenundlateinschenGrammatik9: 361–406.
Brugmann, Karl. 1905–1906. Zur Wortzusammensetzung in den idg. Sprachen. IndogermanischeForschungen 18: 59–76.
Brugmann, Karl. 1906. GrundrißdervergleichendenGrammatikderindogermanischenSprachen2. Band II: LehrevondenWortformenundihremGebrauch. Teil I. Strassburg: Trübner.
Buck, Carl Darling & Petersen, Walter. 1945. AreverseindexofGreeknounsandadjectives. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Byrd, Andrew M. 2015. TheIndo-Europeansyllable. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Byrd, Andrew M. 2018. The phonology of Proto-Indo-European. In Klein, Jared S. & Joseph, Brian D. & Fritz, Matthias (eds.), ThehandbookofcomparativeandhistoricalIndo-Europeanlinguistics, 2056–2079. Berlin / New York: de Gruyter.
Chantraine, Pierre. 1933. Laformationdesnomsengrecancien. Paris: Klincksieck.
Clackson, James. 2007. Indo-Europeanlinguistics. Anintroduction. Cambridge / New York: Cambridge University Press.
de Saussure, Ferdinand. 1896. Accentuation lituanienne. IndogermanischeForschungen 6: 156–177.
de Vaan, Michiel. 2003. TheAvestanvowels. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
de Vaan, Michiel. 2008. EtymologicaldictionaryofLatinandtheotherItaliclanguages. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Derksen, Rick. 1996. MetatonyinBaltic. Amsterdam / Atlanta: Rodopi.
Derksen, Rick. 2015. EtymologicaldictionaryoftheBalticinheritedlexicon. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Ernout, Alfred & Meillet, Alfred. 2001. Dictionnaireétymologiquedelalanguelatine. Histoiredes mots4. Paris: Klincksieck.
Fellner, Hannes & Grestenberger, Laura. 2016. Internal Affairs: Akzent-/Ablautklassen und Interne Derivation. Paper presented at Sanskrit und die Sprachrevolution—200 Jahre Indogermanistik, Berlin, 17–20 May 2016.
Fortson, Benjamin W. 2010. Indo-Europeanlanguageandculture2. Oxford / Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Fortson, Benjamin W. 2020. Towards an assessment of decasuative derivation in Indo-European. Indo-Europeanlinguistics 8: 46–109.
Fraenkel, Ernst. 1952. Zur griechischen Wortbildung. Glotta 32(1/2): 16–33.
Frisk, Hjalmar. 1960. Griechischesetymologischeswörterbuch. Heidelberg: Winter.
Gaisford, Thomas (ed.). 1814. Poetaeminoresgraeci, vol. 1. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Garnier, Romain & Sagot, Benoît. 2017. A shared substrate between Greek and Italic. IndogermanischeForschungen 122: 29–60.
Garrett, Andrew. 2011. Verner’s Law nominal doublets: Bidirectional leveling or accent shift. Paper presented at the 30th Annual East Coast Indo-European Conference, Cambridge, MA, 9 June 2011.
Gertz, Janet E. 1982. Thenominative-accusativeneuterpluralinAnatolian. Dissertation, Yale University.
Gotō, Toshifumi. 2013. OldIndo-AryanmorphologyanditsIndo-Iranianbackground. Vienna: Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften.
Gunkel, Dieter. 2014a. Accentuation. In EncyclopediaofAncientGreeklanguageandlinguistics, Leiden: Brill.
Gunkel, Dieter. 2014b. Law of Limitation. In EncyclopediaofAncientGreeklanguageandlinguistics, Leiden: Brill.
Harðarson, Jón Axel. 1987a. Das uridg. Wort für ‘Frau’. MünchenerStudienzurSprachwissenschaft 48: 115–138.
Harðarson, Jón Axel. 1987b. Zum urindogermanischen Kollektivum. MünchenerStudienzurSprachwissenschaft 48: 71–113.
Haspelmath, Martin. 1995. The growth of affixes in morphological reanalysis. In Booij, Geert & van Marle, Jaap (eds.), YearbookofMorphology 1994, 1–29. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Hirt, Hermann. 1929. IndogermanischeGrammatik. Teil V: DerAkzent. Heidelberg: Winter.
Höfler, Stefan. 2015. Denominale Sekundärderivation im Indogermanischen. Eine Ochsentour. MünchenerStudienzurSprachwissenschaft 69(2): 219–244.
Höfler, Stefan. 2017a. Observations on the palma rule. Pallas 103: 15–23.
Höfler, Stefan. 2017b. Der Stier, der Stärke hat. PossessiveAdjektiveundihreSubstantivierungimIndogermanischen. Dissertation, University of Vienna.
Illič-Svityč, Vladislav M. 1979. NominalaccentuationinBalto-Slavic (Translation by Richard L. Leed and Ronald F. Feinstein of Illič-Svityč 1963, Imennajaakcentuacijavbaltijskomislavjanskom). Moskva: Nauka.
Jamison, Stephanie W. & Brereton, Joel P. 2014. TheRigveda. TheearliestreligiouspoetryofIndia. Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Jasanoff, Jay H. 1989. Old irish bé ‘woman’. Ériu 40: 135–141.
Jasanoff, Jay H. 2002. The nom. sg. of Germanic n-stems. In Wedel, Alfred R. & Busch, Hans-Jörg (eds.), Verbaetlitterae. ExplorationsinGermaniclanguagesandGermanliterature. EssaysinhonorofAlbert L. Lloyd, 31–46. Newark, DE: Linguatext.
Jasanoff, Jay H. 2003. HittiteandtheIndo-Europeanverb. Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Jasanoff, Jay H. 2008. *-bhi, *-bhis, *-ōis: following the trail of the PIE instrumental plural. In Rasmussen, Jens E. & Olander, Thomas (eds.), InternalreconstructioninIndo-European:Results,methods,andproblems. SectionpapersfromtheXVIInternationalConferenceonHistoricalLinguistics,UniversityofCopenhagen,11–15August2003, 137–150. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum.
Jasanoff, Jay H. 2017. TheprehistoryoftheBalto-Slavicaccent. Leiden / New York: Brill.
Jasanoff, Jay H. 2018. What happened to the perfect in Hittite? A contribution to the theory of the h2 e-conjugation. In Rieken, Elisabeth (ed.), 100JahreEntzifferungdesHethitischen:MorphosyntaktischeKategorieninSprachgeschichteundForschung. Akten der Arbeitstagung der Indogermanischen Gesellschaft vom 21. bis 23. September 2015 in Marburg,137–156. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Keydana, Götz. 2013. Accent in thematic nouns. Indo-Europeanlinguistics 1: 107–130.
Kim, Ronald I. 2018. The phonology of Balto-Slavic. In Klein, Jared S. & Joseph, Brian D. & Fritz, Matthias (eds.), ThehandbookofcomparativeandhistoricalIndo-Europeanlinguistics, 1974–1985. Berlin / New York: de Gruyter.
Kim, Ronald I. 2019a. Review of J. Jasanoff (2017), TheprehistoryoftheBalto-Slavicaccent. Scando-Slavica 65(1). 110–123.
Kim, Ronald I. 2019b. The Slavic n-stem neuter ending—in Indo-European perspective. Paper presented at the 14th Annual Meeting of the Slavic Linguistics Society, Potsdam, 13 September 2019.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2010. Compositional vs. paradigmatic approaches to accent and ablaut. In Jamison, Stephanie W. & Melchert, H. Craig & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe21stAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 137–181. Bremen: Hempen.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2015a. New perspectives in historical linguistics. In Bowern, Claire & Evans, Bethwyn (eds.), TheRoutledgehandbookofhistoricallinguistics, 64–102. London / New York: Routledge.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2015b. Stratal OT: A synopsis and FAQs. In Hsiao, Yuchau E. & Wee, Lian-hee (eds.), Capturingphonologicalshades, 2–44. Cambridge / New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2018. Accent and ablaut: Emergent cyclicity. In Goldstein, David M. & Jamison, Stephanie W. & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe28thAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 135–147. Bremen: Hempen.
Kiparsky, Paul. to appear. Accent and ablaut. In Garrett, Andrew & Weiss, Michael (eds.), HandbookofIndo-Europeanstudies, Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Klingenschmitt, Gert. 1994. Das Tocharische in indogermanistischer Sicht. In Schlerath, Bernfried (ed.), Tocharisch. AktenderFachtagungderIndogermanischenGesellschaft,Berlin,September1990, 310–411. Reykjavík: Málvísindastofnun Háskóla Íslands.
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2008. EtymologicaldictionaryoftheHittiteinheritedlexicon. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Kortlandt, Frederik. 1983. On final syllables in Slavic. JournalofIndo-EuropeanStudies 11: 167–185.
Kortlandt, Frederik. 2006. Balto-Slavic accentual mobility. Baltistica 41(3): 349–359.
Krasukhin, Konstantin G. 2000. Archaic features of Indo-European word-formation: The Greek and Old Indic type τόμος—τομός in a PIE perspective. In Clackson, James & Olsen, Birgit A. (eds.), Indo-Europeanwordformation. ProceedingsoftheconferenceheldattheUniversityofCopenhagen,October20th–22nd2000. 119–138. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum.
Kroonen, Guus. 2013. EtymologicaldictionaryofProto-Germanic. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Kuiper, Franciscus B.J. 1942. Notes on Vedic noun inflexion. MedelingenderKoninklijkeNederlandseAkademievanWetenschappen,AfdelingLetterkunde 5(4): 161–256.
Lundquist, Jesse. 2015. On the accentuation of Vedic -ti- abstracts: Evidence for accentual change. Indo-Europeanlinguistics 3: 42–72.
Lundquist, Jesse. 2016. On the accentuation of compound s-stem adjectives in Greek and Vedic. In Goldstein, David M. & Jamison, Stephanie W. & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe27thAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 97–114. Bremen: Hempen.
Lundquist, Jesse. 2017. ArchaismsandinnovationsinthesongsofHomer. Dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.
Lundquist, Jesse. 2021a. Does tvátpitāraḥ = εὐπάτωρ? o-grades, amphikinetics, and compounds in Sanskrit, Greek, and Indo-European. Indo-Europeanlinguistics 9: 128–170.
Lundquist, Jesse. 2021b. Penelope’s αἰνοπαθῆ (σ 201): Very old or very young? JournalofGreeklinguistics 21: 193–223.
Lundquist, Jesse & Yates, Anthony D. 2018. The morphology of Proto-Indo-European. In Klein, Jared S. & Joseph, Brian D. & Fritz, Matthias (eds.), ThehandbookofcomparativeandhistoricalIndo-Europeanlinguistics, 2079–2195. Berlin / New York: de Gruyter.
Macdonell, Arthur A. 1910. Vedicgrammar. Strassburg: Trübner.
Matasović, Ranko. 2005. The centum elements in Balto-Slavic. In Meiser, Gerhard & Hackstein, Olav (eds.), SprachkontaktundSprachwandel:AktenderXI. FachtagungderindogermanischenGesellschaft,HalleanderSaale,17. bis23September2000, 363–374. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Mayrhofer, Manfred. 1986. IndogermanischeGrammatik. Band I/2: Lautlehre. Heidelberg: Winter.
McCone, Kim. 1995. Oir. senchae, senchaid, and preliminaries on agent noun formation in Celtic. Ériu 46: 1–10.
Meier-Brügger, Michael & Fritz, Matthias. 2021. IndogermanischeSprachwissenschaft10. Berlin / New York: de Gruyter.
Meissner, Torsten. 2005. S-stemnounsandadjectivesinGreekandProto-Indo-European. Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Melchert, H. Craig. 1983. A ‘new’ PIE *men suffix. DieSprache 29(1): 1–26.
Melchert, H. Craig. 2007/8. Neuter stems with the suffix *-(e)n- in Anatolian and Proto-Indo-European. DieSprache 47(2): 182–191.
Melchert, H. Craig. 2013. Ablaut patterns in the Hittite i-conjugation. In Jamison, Stephanie W. & Melchert, H. Craig & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe24thAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 137–150. Bremen: Hempen.
Mielcke, Christian G. 1800. Littauisch-deutschesunddeutsch-littauischesWörterbuch. Königsberg: Hartung.
Neri, Sergio. 2005. Riflessioni sullapofonia radicale di proto-germanico *namōn ‘nome’. HistorischeSprachforschung 118: 201–250.
Neri, Sergio. 2017. Elementidimorfologiaflessivanominaleindoeuropea. Perugia: Università degli Studi di Perugia.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 1986. HeadandhorninIndo-European. Berlin / New York: de Gruyter.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 1997. The “Saussure effect” in Latin and Italic. In Lubotsky, Alexander (ed.), Soundlawandanalogy.PapersinhonorofRobertS.P.Beekesontheoccasionofhis60thbirthday, 181–203. Leiden: Rodopi.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 1998. TwostudiesinGreekandHomericlinguistics. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 2010. PIE -Cmn- and Greek τρᾱνής ‘clear’. In Kim, Ronald & Oettinger, Norbert & Rieken, Elisabeth & Weiss, Michael (eds.), Exanatolialux. AnatolianandIndo-EuropeanstudiesinhonorofH. CraigMelchertontheoccasionofhissixty-fifthbirthday, 269–277. Ann Arbor / New York: Beech Stave Press.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 2014a. Feminine, abstract, collective, neuter plural: some remarks on each (expanded handout). In Neri, Sergio & Schuhmann, Roland (eds.), StudiesonthecollectiveandfeminineinIndo-Europeanfromadiachronicandtypologicalperspective, 273–306. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 2014b. Greek τέκμαρ ‘sign’ and τέκμωρ ‘sign’: why both? In Oettinger, Norbert & Steer, Thomas (eds.), DasNomenimIndogermanischen:Morphologie,SubstantivversusAdjektiv,Kollektivum. AktenderArbeitstagungderIndogermanischenGesellschaftvom14. bis16. September2011inErlangen, 215–260. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Nussbaum, Alan J. 2017. Agentive and other derivatives of “τόμος-type” nouns. In Le Feure, Claire & Petit, Daniel & Pinault, Georges-Jean (eds.), Adjectifsverbauxetparticipesdansleslanguesindo-européennes. ProceedingsoftheArbeitstagungoftheIndo-EuropeanSociety,Paris,24–26September2014, 232–266. Bremen: Hempen.
Oettinger, Norbert. 1982. Reste von e-Hochstufe im Formans hethitischer n-Stämme einschließlich des ‘umna’ Suffixes. In Neu, Erich (ed.), Investigationesphilologicaeetcomparativae. GedenkschriftfürHeinzKronasser, 162–177. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
Oettinger, Norbert. 2003. Zum Ablaut von n-Stämmen im Anatolischen und der Brechung ē > ya. In Tichy, Eva & Wodtko, Dagmar S. & Irslinger, Britta (eds.), IndogermanischesNomen:Derivation,FlexionundAblaut. AktenderArbeitstagungderIndogermanischenGesellschaft,Freiburg,19bis. 22September2001, 141–152. Bremen: Hempen.
Olander, Thomas. 2009. Balto-Slavicaccentualmobility. Berlin / New York: de Gruyter.
Olander, Thomas. 2015. Proto-Slavicinflectionalmorphology. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Olander, Thomas. 2018. The dark past of Balto-Slavic prosody: On Jay Jasanoff’s book TheprehistoryoftheBalto-Slavicaccent. Lingua 207: 98–106.
Pardini, Francesco P. 2011. Isostantivilituaniformaticolsuffisso *–men. Dissertation, Università degli Studi di Firenze.
Parmentier, Léon. 1889. Lessubstantifsetlesadjectivesin -ες- danslalangued’Homèreetd’hésiode. Ghent: Vanderhaeghen.
Pedersen, Holger. 1926. Lacinquièmedéclinaisonlatine. Copenhagen: A.F. Høst.
Pedersen, Holger. 1933. Étudeslituaniennes. Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaard.
Petit, Daniel. 2010. UntersuchungenzudenbaltischenSprachen. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Petit, Daniel. 2018. Accentual mobility in Lithuanian and its implications for Proto-Indo-European. In Goldstein, David M. & Jamison, Stephanie W. & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe28thAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 223–258. Bremen: Hempen.
Probert, Philomen. 2003. AnewshortguidetotheaccentuationofAncientGreek. London: Bristol Classical Press.
Probert, Philomen. 2006a. Accentuation in Ancient Greek deverbative ā-stems. In Kölligan, Daniel & Sen, Ranjan (eds.), OxfordUniversityworkingpapersinlinguistics,philology&phonetics 11, 122–142. Oxford: Oxford University.
Probert, Philomen. 2006b. AncientGreekaccentuation.Synchronicpatterns,frequencyeffects,andprehistory. Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Probert, Philomen. 2010. Ancient Greek accentuation in generative phonology and Optimality Theory. Languageandlinguisticscompass 4(1): 1–26.
Probert, Philomen. 2012. Origins of the Greek Law of Limitation. In Probert, Philomen & Willi, Andreas (eds.), LawsandrulesinIndo-European, 163–181. Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Pronk, Tijmen. 2009. The accentuation of the Slavic n-stems. In Olander, Thomas & Larsson, Jenny H. (eds.), Stressingthepast.PapersinBalticandSlavicaccentology, 101–114. Leiden / Boston: Brill.
Pronk, Tijmen. 2014. Proto-Indo-European mn-stems in Balto-Slavic. In Oettinger, Norbert & Steer, Thomas (eds.), DasNomenimIndogermanischen:Morphologie,SubstantivversusAdjektiv,Kollektivum. AktenderArbeitstagungderIndogermanischenGesellschaftvom14. bis16. September2011inErlangen, 318–326. Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Rasmussen, Jens E. 1989. StudienzurMorphophonemikderindogermanischenGrundsprache. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft.
Rasmussen, Jens E. 2001. Against the assumption of an IE *kwetwóres rule. In Huld, Martin E. & Jones-Bley, Karlene & Della Volpe, Angela (eds.), ProceedingsoftheTwelfthAnnualUCLAIndo-EuropeanConference, 15–28. Washington, D.C.: Institute for the Study of Man.
Rau, Jeremy. 2009. Indo-Europeannominalmorphology.ThedecadsandtheCalandSystem. Innsbruck: Institut für Sprachen und Literaturen der Universität Innsbruck.
Remmer, Ulla. 2002–2003. Das indogermanische Suffix *-mon- im Altirischen (Teil I). DieSprache 43(2): 171–211.
Risch, Ernst. 1974. WortbildungderhomerischenSprache2. Berlin: De Gruyter.
Rix, Helmut. 1976. HistorischeGrammatikdesGriechischen.Laut- undFormenlehre. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.
Rix, Helmut. 1985. sūdor und sīdus. In Ölberg, Hermann M. & Schmidt, Gernot & Bothien, Heinz (eds.), SprachwissenschaftlicheForschungen.FestschriftfürJohannKnoblochzum65. Geburtstagam5. Januar1984dargebrachtvonFreundenundKollegen, 339–350. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft.
Rix, Helmut. 1992. HistorischeGrammatikdesGriechischen.Laut- undFormenlehre2. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.
Roussou, Stephanie. 2018. Pseudo-Arcadius’ epitomeofHerodian’s de prosodia catholica. Oxford / New York: Oxford University Press.
Sandell, Ryan. 2015. Productivityinhistoricallinguistics.ComputationalperspectivesonwordformationinAncientGreekandSanskrit. Dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.
Sandell, Ryan. 2016. R̥gvedic śáktīvant-. Accentuation and statistical modeling of allomorph selection in Vedic -mant/vant-stems. In Goldstein, David M. & Jamison, Stephanie W. & Vine, Brent (eds.), Proceedingsofthe27thAnnualUCLAIndo-Europeanconference, 143–162. Bremen: Hempen.
Schaffner, Stefan. 2001. DasVernerscheGesetzundderinnerparadigmatischegrammatischeWechseldesUrindogermanischenimNominalbereich. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft.
Schaffner, Stefan. 2004. Mittelirisch fethid, ‘geht, macht seinen Weg’, althochdeutsch wadalōn, wallōn ‘umhergehen, wandern; umherwogen’, altenglisch waðuma ‘Woge, Welle’, waðōl ‘vollmond’, und Verwandtes. In Poschenrieder, Thorwald (ed.), DieIndogermanistikundihreAnrainer, 277–314. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft.
Schindler, Jochem. 1967. Zu hethitisch nekuz. ZeitschriftfürvergleichendeSprachforschung 81: 290–303.