Save

The Role of the Eagala Model in Promoting Psychological Wellbeing in Adolescents: A Mixed-Methods Approach

In: Society & Animals
Authors:
Nicoleen Coetzee Department of Psychology, University of Pretoria Pretoria South Africa

Search for other papers by Nicoleen Coetzee in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Sharon Boyce Department of Psychology, University of Pretoria Pretoria South Africa

Search for other papers by Sharon Boyce in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Andries Masenge Department of Statistics, University of Pretoria Pretoria South Africa

Search for other papers by Andries Masenge in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
View More View Less
Full Access

Abstract

The aim of the study was to determine if the Eagala model, an equine-assisted intervention, would enhance the psychological wellbeing of adolescents living in a township in South Africa. An embedded mixed-methods research design with convenience sampling was used. Participants were randomly assigned to an experimental or control group. The experimental group participated in the Eagala intervention. The Ryff Scale of Psychological Wellbeing (RSPWB) was used to collect quantitative data while letters to the horses were used as qualitative data. Independent t-tests indicated significant differences between the two groups on the overall score of the RSPWB and five of its dimensions. Using thematic analysis, qualitative themes that enhanced understanding of the quantitative findings were identified. The results confirmed the effectiveness of the Eagala intervention. A Kruskal-Wallis test, however, indicated that between four to eight sessions need to be attended in order for the intervention to be effective.

Abstract

The aim of the study was to determine if the Eagala model, an equine-assisted intervention, would enhance the psychological wellbeing of adolescents living in a township in South Africa. An embedded mixed-methods research design with convenience sampling was used. Participants were randomly assigned to an experimental or control group. The experimental group participated in the Eagala intervention. The Ryff Scale of Psychological Wellbeing (RSPWB) was used to collect quantitative data while letters to the horses were used as qualitative data. Independent t-tests indicated significant differences between the two groups on the overall score of the RSPWB and five of its dimensions. Using thematic analysis, qualitative themes that enhanced understanding of the quantitative findings were identified. The results confirmed the effectiveness of the Eagala intervention. A Kruskal-Wallis test, however, indicated that between four to eight sessions need to be attended in order for the intervention to be effective.

One of South Africa’s historical legacies is the existence of impoverished communities, often referred to as townships, on the outskirts of cities and towns. An example of such a township is Diepsloot. Diepsloot is situated on the northern outskirts of Johannesburg and is spread over 12 square kilometers with a population estimate of 350 000 (Makwela, 2015). Almost half of the Diepsloot population consists of adolescents (Stats SA, 2012). Adolescents are defined as young people between the ages of 10 and 19 years (World Health Organization, 2014).

According to the life span developmental approach, adolescence refers to the period when the majority of an individual’s biological, cognitive, psychological, and social characteristics are changing from what is typically considered childlike to what is considered adult-like (Lerner & Galambos, 1998; Overton, 2010). Blau et al. (2019) noted that in order to facilitate a successful transition into adulthood, adolescents need to have a sense of meaning and fulfilment in life. This implies that adolescents need to obtain eudaimonic wellbeing to manage the transition into adulthood successfully. Eudaimonic wellbeing, which is used interchangeably with psychological wellbeing, is achieved by functioning optimally (Ryff, 1989). Individuals who function optimally display six facets in their daily lives, namely autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, purpose in life, positive relations with others, and self-acceptance (Ryff, 1989).

Adolescents from Diepsloot, however, struggle to function optimally and hence experience a lack of psychological wellbeing due to the impoverished conditions they live in (Cluver et al., 2009; Luby et al., 2013; Ratele, 2007). In addition to poverty, Diepsloot has been associated with violent service delivery protests and xenophobic attacks (Mahajan, 2014). Service delivery protests are a series of public protests and civil disturbances as a means of expressing grievances regarding the ineffective delivery of various social services by government agencies in communities. These protests may involve violence and altercations with police units resulting in death, serious injuries, and potential arrest. In addition, drug abuse is rampant and violence against children, adolescents and women is commonplace (Mahajan, 2014). As a result, adolescents living in Diepsloot often express a sense of disillusionment and discontent with their situation (Cross, 2014).

Furthering the challenge to achieve psychological wellbeing is the lack of psychological services in Diepsloot. The situation has not changed since 2011, when Shamos (2011) reported that the mental health needs of the community are provided for by two general health clinics and counsellors situated at the South African Depression and Anxiety Group (SADAG) facility.

Since quality mental health services cannot be provided in a traditional manner, it seems that there is an urgent need for the presentation of alternative forms of therapeutic interventions that would promote psychological wellbeing among adolescents in Diepsloot. However, when considering new avenues for therapeutic interventions, one needs to keep in mind that adolescents, particularly those who grew up in the face of adversity, need to be approached with great care and sensitivity (Hill, 2007; Reddy et al., 2010).

Chandler (2005), who successfully assisted adolescents from impoverished backgrounds to improve their psychological wellbeing, maintains that Animal-Assisted Therapy (AAT) should be considered as a modality when working with youth from impoverished backgrounds. AAT involves a therapeutic intervention using animals which is delivered by health professionals who practice within their scope of professional expertise (Hoagwood et al., 2016; Kruger & Serpell, 2010). Research has shown that AAT creates an environment where individuals experience empathy, sensitivity, and comfort (Coleman, 1999; Dillman, 1999; Roseberry & Rovin, 1999; Salotto, 1999). The presence of the animal places emphasis on the nonjudgmental nature of the context in which AAT takes place and hence provides a safe, trusting and nonthreatening atmosphere in which psychological wellbeing can be promoted (Chandler, 2005; Coetzee et al., 2013; Coleman, 1999; Lentini & Knox, 2009). Commonly used animals during AAT are dogs, cats, rabbits, horses, and donkeys (Fine, 2015).

One of the Non-Governmental Organizations (NGOs) currently active in Diepsloot is the Shumbashaba Community Trust (SCT). SCT provides counselling and psychosocial support programs that focus on making a positive difference in individuals’ lives. Horses form an integral part of their program implementation which is based on the model proposed by the Equine Assisted Growth and Learning Association (Eagala, 2019). The Eagala model is a distinctive experiential psychological framework that incorporates horses into interventions to solve mental health challenges or restore and promote mental health and wellbeing through the horse-human relationship (Eagala, 2019). Participants are not required to have previous experience with horses as there is no riding; all interactions are deliberately ground-based activities.

Horses as herd and prey animals have a well-developed “fight or flight” instinct and are subsequently very sensitive to changes in the environment, including the intent, body language, and emotional states of participants (Lentini & Knox, 2009; Smith-Osborne & Selby, 2010). The responses and reactions of the horses provide instantaneous feedback to participants, thereby creating a unique therapeutic medium where the horses become agents of change, prompting processes of development, learning and growth that will ultimately enhance psychological wellbeing (Bachi & Parish-Plass, 2017; Selby & Smith-Osborne, 2013).

A mental health professional and an equine specialist comprise the team that facilitates the process of feedback between the horses and participants. This team also assists participants to explore cognitions, affect, and behaviors related to psychological wellbeing whilst interacting with the horses (Eagala, 2019; Fournier et al., 2018).

The Eagala intervention implemented by SCT involves 90-minute group sessions that take place weekly over an eight-week period. Participants are randomly assigned to three smaller subgroups. Each subgroup is allocated a facilitation team and herd of four to six horses. Horses are from various breeds, and they range in age from 10 to 25 years. The horses normally participate in therapeutic riding and Eagala model sessions three to four times per week. They are fed twice daily and groomed when stabled at night. When the horses are not working, they are free to graze as herds in large fields. An outline of the Eagala model intervention is provided in Table 1.

Preliminary studies on equine-assisted therapy, including Eagala Model interventions, indicated that adolescents exposed to it experience improvements in hope for the future, problem solving, and mastery, as well as increases in general levels of functioning and reductions in psychological distress (Burgon, 2011; Kemp et al., 2014; Schultz et al., 2007; Whittlesey-Jerome et al., 2016). It is, however, important to note that studies suggest that the number of sessions attended has an effect on the intervention benefits. Schultz et al. (2007) found a statistically significant correlation between the percentage improvement on the Children’s Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) Scale scores and number of sessions attended by 63 children, aged 4–16 years, who received a minimum of six and an average of 19 Eagala sessions. Within the South African context, Boshoff et al. (2015) found that an eight-session equine-assisted therapy program yielded significant improvements in subjective wellbeing of adolescent boys attending a custodial school. Nevertheless, systematic reviews of equine-assisted literature indicate inconsistencies in intervention duration, ranging from one to 119 sessions. Consequentially, the authors highlight the need to establish dosage outcomes for intervention effectiveness (Anestis et al., 2014; Kendall et al., 2015; Selby & Smith-Osborne, 2013).

Table 1
Table 1
Table 1

An overview of the Eagala model intervention

Citation: Society & Animals 2023; 10.1163/15685306-bja10092

Although the Eagala model is used by SCT in impoverished communities, little scientific research was thus far conducted to determine the extent to which the intervention promotes psychological wellbeing among the adolescents of such communities in South Africa. In keeping with Barnes’s (2012) guidelines set forth for formulating aims for mixed-methods studies, the present study’s aims were hence to 1) determine if adolescent residents of Diepsloot that partook in the Eagala model intervention differed significantly with regards to levels of psychological wellbeing from those who did not participate in the intervention; 2) establish how many sessions are required for changes in psychological wellbeing to be experienced; 3) understand what participants of the Eagala model intervention experienced and learned; and 4) establish if any changes in psychological wellbeing could be related to subjective experiences.

Methods

Participants

A sample of convenience was drawn from adolescents participating in the programmes offered by the SCT. The sample consisted of 40 females and 34 males, between the ages of 13 and 17 years. Participants were randomly assigned to either the control or experimental groups. The participants in the control group were placed on a waitlist to participate in the intervention once the research was completed. The statistics describing the sample are provided in Table 2.

Research Design

An embedded mixed-methods design was used during the study. The quantitative phase consisted of a quasi-experimental between-subjects design with quantitative data collected pre- and post-intervention. The qualitative data was collected right before the post-intervention quantitative data collection took place (Creswell, 2013).

Table 2
Table 2

Descriptive statistics of the sample

Citation: Society & Animals 2023; 10.1163/15685306-bja10092

The results of the qualitative data were combined with the quantitative findings to expand and deepen understanding of the quantitative results as well as to establish if any changes in psychological wellbeing could be related to participants’ subjective experiences of the intervention. This was achieved by linking the significant quantitative findings to the qualitative themes. The point of integration of these two data strands occurred in the discussion of the results (Creswell, 2013).

Quantitative Measures

Biographical Questionnaire

This questionnaire provided information on the biographical details of the participants indicating their gender, age, and current grade in school.

The Ryff Scale of Psychological Wellbeing (RSPWB)

Fernandes et al. (2010), as well as Gao and McLellan (2018), studied the reliability and construct validity of the RPSWB among Portuguese and Chinese adolescents. They concluded that the measure is suitable for use in multicultural contexts (Fernandes et al., 2010; Gao & McLellan, 2018). The RSPWB is based on Ryff’s (1989) interpretation of psychological wellbeing and as mentioned previously, comprises six facets – namely, autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. Each facet represents a dimension of psychological wellbeing. For the purposes of this study, the 84-item RPSWB, consisting of 14 items per dimension, was used. Participants had to rate themselves on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = moderately disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = slightly agree, 5 = moderately agree, 6 = strongly agree).

Studies conducted by Ryff (1989; 2014) and Van Dierendonck (2005) reported Cronbach’s alpha values for the six dimensions ranging from 0.77 to 0.93. The Cronbach alpha coefficient of the overall RSPWB calculated for the current study was 0.92, with values ranging from 0.51 to 0.74 for the six dimensions.

Qualitative Measure

Letters to the Horse

The qualitative measure consisted of a writing activity in the form of letters to the horses. Participants from the experimental group were asked to write letters to the horses in which they indicated what they experienced and learned whilst participating in the intervention. Letters ranged from half a page to two pages in length.

Procedure

The research took place at the SCT’s premises in Diepsloot which consisted of an outdoor classroom in their stable yard and horse paddocks. The quantitative data were collected pre- and post-intervention for both the experimental and control groups. As mentioned previously, the participants in the experimental group were randomly assigned to three smaller subgroups, each one with their own facilitation team. These subgroups were then allocated a paddock and a horse herd, consisting of four horses that remained the same throughout the intervention. The control group continued with their daily activities and did not participate in the intervention.

As was also mentioned before, the qualitative data were collected right before the quantitative data collection of the post-intervention. During the qualitative data collection phase, 37 letters to the horses were obtained from the participants of the experimental group.

Ethical Considerations

The research was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Humanities at the University of Pretoria. The SCT board of trustees gave written permission for the research project to form part of one their outreach projects. Informed consent was obtained from parents whilst the adolescents provided informed assent before the study started.

Participation was voluntary, and privacy as well as confidentiality of the participants was always respected. Confidentiality was ensured by allocating participant numbers to the participants of the study. The facilitation team also signed letters of confidentiality.

The welfare of the horses was monitored by the equine specialist and SCT’s stable manager. All horses were selected for suitability of temperaments and were habituated to this type of work. They were not restrained, they were unencumbered and free to interact or not, with the participants. Hence, they could remove themselves from human interaction at any time during the intervention.

Data Analysis

Quantitative Data Analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS Version 23®. An independent t-test was conducted before and after the intervention to determine if any significant differences occurred between the experimental and control groups on the scores of the RSPBW.

Due to their challenging living conditions, participants’ attendance fluctuated during the course of the intervention. The variance across the number of sessions attended was thus high. In order to control for this, three categories of attendance were identified: zero sessions attended (control condition) (n = 37); and two categories from the experimental condition, namely, 1–3 sessions attended (n = 10); and 4–8 sessions attended (n = 27). A Kruskal-Wallis test was then conducted to determine the number of sessions to ensure achievement of psychological wellbeing (Field, 2013).

Qualitative Data Analysis

Inductive thematic analysis was used to analyze the qualitative data. The thematic analysis was done in accordance with the six steps set out by Joffe (2012) as well as Braun and Clarke (2006). These steps include 1) familiarization with the data though reading and re-reading the letters, 2) generating initial codes, 3) searching for themes, 4) reviewing and refining the themes, 5) defining and naming themes, and 6) conducting a final analysis of the identified themes in relation to the research aim. To ensure the credibility and trustworthiness of the data analysis, two methods were applied, namely, transferability and triangulation. Transferability was achieved by analyzing the data in a systematic fashion, using the six steps of thematic analysis. This ensured that the process can be replicated (Creswell, 2013). Triangulation, on the other hand, occurred through linking the significant quantitative findings to the qualitative themes (Creswell, 2013).

Results

Quantitative Results

The independent t-test yielded no significant differences between the experimental and control groups prior to the intervention. Post-intervention results, on the other hand, indicated significant differences (p < 0.05) between the groups on 1) the overall RSPWB scores; 2) the purpose in life scores; 3) the positive relations scores; 4) the environmental mastery scores; 5) the personal growth scores; and 6) the self-acceptance scores. Upon closer inspection of the data, the averages of the experimental group in all the instances were found to be higher than the reported averages of the control groups (Table 3). Effect sizes ranged from medium to large.

Table 3
Table 3

Independent t-test results on post intervention and subscales scores of RSPWB

Citation: Society & Animals 2023; 10.1163/15685306-bja10092

As was mentioned previously, a Kruskal-Wallis test was conducted to determine the number of sessions that need to be attended to ensure the success of the intervention. The results are presented in Table 4.

Table 4
Table 4

Kruskal-Wallis post-intervention results

Citation: Society & Animals 2023; 10.1163/15685306-bja10092

Table 4 indicates that the Kruskal-Wallis yielded significant differences (p < 0.05) for the overall scores of the RSPWB, and the dimensions of purpose in life, positive relations, environmental mastery and personal growth. As is recommended by both Field (2013) and Pallant (2016), a follow-up pairwise comparison analysis was conducted to evaluate pairwise differences among the three groups. The results of the pairwise comparison analysis are summarized in Table 5.

Table 5
Table 5

Pairwise comparison analysis to determine effect of number of sessions on RSPWB scores

Citation: Society & Animals 2023; 10.1163/15685306-bja10092

Tables 4 and 5 indicate that attending four to eight sessions of the intervention makes a significant difference to the overall score of the RSPWB and to the dimensions of purpose in life, positive relations, environmental mastery, and personal growth. Effect sizes ranged from small to large.

Qualitative Results

Thematic analysis of the qualitative data yielded six themes, namely, positive relationships, environmental management, a hopeful and meaningful life, self-worth, personal growth, and horses as agents of change. These themes and their supporting data extracts are displayed in Table 6.

Table 6
Table 6

Identified themes and associating data extracts

Citation: Society & Animals 2023; 10.1163/15685306-bja10092

Discussion

Besides the significant differences observed between the experimental and control groups on the overall score of the RSPWB, significant differences were also discovered on the dimension related to purpose in life (see Table 3). Purpose in life can be described as the degree to which an individual has a general sense of purpose, meaning and goal directedness (Ryff, 1989, 1995, 2014). As a result of this depiction of purpose in life, the theme a hopeful and meaningful life was associated with this dimension. It appears that, after interacting with the horses, the adolescents from the experimental group found meaning in their lives. This is evident in participants’ verbal responses where they observed that the horses assisted them to find “meaning in my personal life,” “meaning in my life … to fulfill my purpose in life,” and “hope for life” (see Table 6). Having a purpose in life filled with meaning signals to these adolescents that there is a future awaiting them and that they can be hopeful about this future, regardless of the difficulties they encounter everyday (Frankl, 1988; Yeager & Bundick, 2009). Benesova et al. (2014) furthermore noted that adolescents experiencing hope and meaning often participate in activities that will increase satisfaction with their lives. They especially enjoy participating in team sports and will often engage in community activities (Benesova et al., 2014; Slezackova, 2017). Slezackova (2017) determined that hope and meaning not only play a role in adolescents’ psychological wellbeing, but also form an important part of adolescent development.

The significant differences observed between the experimental and control groups with regards to the dimension of positive relations are echoed in the theme positive relationships. When adolescents experience their relationships as positive, they not only realize that they are loved, but also discover their ability to love others. Burgon (2011) noted that positive relationships teach adolescents how to form affectionate, nurturing, and empathic relationships with others. This is confirmed by excerpts from the letters where participants specifically mentioned that the horses taught them about “love you and know you love us,” “empathy to others,” “help others and they help you if you are in need” (see Table 6). Garcia (2014) found that the formation of positive relationships in the presence of horses create profound experiences where adolescents become aware of the existence of unconditional love, thus increasing their psychological wellbeing.

Another factor associated with positive relationships is trust (Pratt & Dirks, 2007). Latella and Abrahams (2015) noted that individuals who interact with horses often mention how the horses enabled them to establish mutual relationships based on trust. In the present study, some of the letters specifically mention “they trust me and I trust them,” “trust yourself,” and “have trust.” When adolescents trust others, they become comfortable with the idea of talking about their experiences (Ryff, 1989, 1995, 2014), which often result in the lowering of depression levels (Kemp et al., 2014). Although lowered depression levels are commonly associated with heightened levels of wellbeing, Pratt and Dirks (2007) argued that it could also be indicative of the existence of resilience. They reasoned that when faced with adversity, adolescents with positive relationships are more resilient because of the trust and support that individuals in such relationships offer one another.

The third dimension of the RSPWB which yielded significant differences between the experimental and control groups relates to environmental mastery. This dimension appears to be similar to the theme of environmental management that came to the fore during the qualitative analysis. Environmental mastery/management refers to adolescents’ awareness of the impact they have on their immediate environments (Ryff, 1989, 1995, 2014). Adolescents who are able to master/manage their environments know that they have the freedom to make choices in accordance with their personal needs and values (Ryff, 1995; Sagone & De Caroli, 2014). They are also fully aware that all choices have consequences and that such consequences will impact their experience of life to a positive or negative extent (Ryff, 1989, 1995, 2014). The adolescents of the present study seemed to be aware of this as they noted that “I don’t have to make wrong choices in life and you have to make good choices in life,” “Bad choices have bad consequences,” and “I have to choose good choices and know the consequences will also be there” (see Table 6). Keyes (2007) and Wong (2012) proposed that adolescents who can take personal responsibility for their actions and subsequent consequences are more open to subjective experiences of optimal living and flourishing. In addition, such adolescents are able to grow and expand as individuals, furthering their psychological wellbeing and ability to function optimally (Sagone & De Caroli, 2014).

The next dimension displaying significant differences between the experimental and control group is that of personal growth. Interestingly, personal growth also emerged as a theme when the thematic analysis was conducted. It is important to note that personal growth and growth as a result of mastering one’s environment differ slightly from one another. Growth resulting from mastering the environment is related to adolescents’ ability to make decisions and live with the consequences of those decisions. Personal growth, on the other hand, involves being open to new experiences, realizing personal potential, and displaying the ability to change because of acquired self-knowledge (Ryff, 1989, 1995, 2014). It therefore appears that personal growth can be associated with the ability to adapt and change. Within the context of the present study, it is evident that the horses facilitated personal growth since the letters mentioned “you are most beautiful and precious because you have changed my life,” “my life has changed,” “I am very different because of you,” and “I have learned too many things here” (see Table 6). Ayub and Iqbal (2012) noted that adolescents displaying personal growth tend to experience fewer mental health issues and display high levels of psychological wellbeing. This confirms previous studies’ findings which noted that an increase in personal growth is associated with a reduction of stress levels and the development of effective coping skills (Fabry, 1988; Rathi & Rastogi, 2007; Wong, 1998).

The last dimension displaying significant differences between the experimental and control group is the dimension related to self-acceptance. A close inspection of the qualitative data analysis revealed that the theme identified as self-worth can be linked to this dimension. The establishment of self-acceptance indicates the adolescents of the experimental group became aware of their self-worth and learned that they are worthy of love and respect. The crucial role played by the equine assisted intervention is evident when participants remark in their letters to the horses that “I have learned I have to respect myself,” “learned respect,” and “thanks for showing me respect” (see Table 6). Adolescents who realize they are worthy of respect tend to display positive attitudes toward themselves and are more likely to accept their negative attributes as well (Ryff, 1989, 1995, 2014). An example of this is a participant noting “the white [horse] taught me about respect and being different.”

Self-acceptance is also related to having patience with oneself when making mistakes (Chardonnens, 2009). Some excerpts of the letters state that “I shouldn’t give up so easily, they taught me to be patient” and “the black horse taught me to be patience not to give up so easily.” Adolescents displaying self-acceptance will be less likely to engage in self-destructive behavior (Garcia et al., 2014), which increases the probability of positive functioning (Ryff & Keyes, 1995), and experiencing psychological wellbeing (Kemp et al., 2014).

Apart from the themes linked to the quantitative results, an additional theme emerged during the qualitative data analysis. This theme was labeled horses as agents of change. The theme indicates that the adolescents in the experimental group realized that they changed and felt better about themselves after participating in the intervention. They attributed these changes and newfound knowledge about themselves to the horses. The participants wrote in this regard statements such as “thank you the horses for teaching me new things I didn’t know,” “the black [horse] teached me not to be afraid easily,” “learned many things about life, respect, uniqueness, attitude towards others, relationships, empathy,” and “when I with the horses learned we are unique” (see Table 6).

The notion of horses as active agents of change resonates with Chardonnens’ (2009) belief that horses act as co-therapists during therapeutic situations. She furthermore suggested that horses are a source of strong affective communication, and that the unconditional love and acceptance they display resonate in those interacting with them (Chardonnens, 2009). According to Vincent and Farkas (2017), this experience of positive feelings or attachment allows for the creation of authentic positive relationships with others and oneself. It thus appears that horses’ patience and nonjudgmental characters facilitate self-discovery and self-acceptance. As a result, individuals interacting with horses will attain personal growth and realize they are living a value-centric life. This confirms the notion that the presence of horses significantly contributes to the successful outcome of AAT. It furthermore shows that individuals who are not familiar with horses can experience enhanced levels of psychological wellbeing as a result of interacting with them.

In light of the above discussions, it is posited that the Eagala intervention played a significant role in the establishment of psychological wellbeing among adolescents from the Diepsloot Township. It should, however, be kept in mind that the number of sessions attended during the equine assisted intervention will play a role in the success of the intervention. This is evident from the results presented in Tables 4 and 5 which indicate that attending four to eight sessions have the most significant impact on achieving psychological wellbeing. These results concur with the findings of Schultz et al. (2007), Smith-Osborne Selby (2010), and Kendall et al. (2015), who all noted that the number of sessions attended during EAP have a significant impact on achieving psychological wellbeing. Schultz et al. (2007), who specifically conducted research on the Eagala model intervention, found a statistically significant correlation in the percentage increase in the Children’s Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scores and the number of sessions attended. In the same vein, Trotter et al. (2008) noted an improvement in positive behavior among adolescents when they attended eight or more sessions of an Eagala intervention.

Limitations and Recommendations for Future Studies

Since only a limited number of participants could be accommodated during the Eagala intervention, no power calculation was done to determine the sample size. Instead, a sample of convenience was used. This, in addition to the small size, implies that the findings of the present study cannot be generalized to the adolescent population as a whole and needs to be interpreted with caution. Future studies therefore need to focus on obtaining larger samples.

Another limitation of the study is its cross-sectional nature. It is recommended that future research should incorporate longitudinal studies to determine if adolescents are able to maintain psychological wellbeing post intervention. Additionally, such studies should attempt to include more than one quantitative measure of psychological wellbeing to increase the reliability of the findings. More measures should furthermore be identified and standardized for use within the South African context.

Conclusion

Based on the results obtained from the quantitative and qualitative analyses it is concluded that the Eagala model intervention played a significant role in promoting psychological wellbeing in a group of adolescents living in the under-resourced township of Diepsloot. It should, however, be noted that at least four to eight sessions of the intervention need to be attended before a change in psychological wellbeing will become evident.

The results furthermore suggest that the Eagala model is an appropriate alternative form of intervention that could be used to promote psychological wellbeing in adolescents from under-resourced townships.

Funding

This work did not receive any grants.

Conflict of interest

All the authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  • Anestis, M. D., Anestis, J. C., Zawilinski, L. L., Hopkins, T. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2014). Equine‐related treatments for mental disorders lack empirical support: A systematic review of empirical investigations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70(12), 11151132. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22113

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ayub, N., & Iqbal, S. (2012). The relationship of personal growth initiative, psychological wellbeing, and psychological distress among adolescents. Journal of Teaching and Education, 1(6), 101107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bachi, K., & Parish-Plass, N. (2017). Animal-assisted psychotherapy: A unique relational therapy for children and adolescents. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(4), 38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104516672549

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barnes, B. R. (2012). Using mixed methods in South African psychological research. South African Journal of Psychology, 22(1), 463475. https://doi.org/10.1177/008124631204200402

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Benesova, K., Kupcova, M., Vildova, L., Farkasova, K., Slezackova, A., & Malatincova, T. (2014). Naděje pro příští generaci: Psychosociální souvislosti naděje u dětí ve věku 8 až 12 let. [Hope for the next generation: The psychosocial context of hope in children aged 8 to 12 years] [Paper presentation]. Symposium on Hope, Masaryk University, Brno, Czechia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blau, I., Goldberg, S., & Benolol, N. (2019). Purpose and life satisfaction during adolescence: The role of meaning in life, social support, and problematic digital use. Journal of Youth Studies, 22(7), 907925. https://doi.org/10.1080/13676261.2018.1551614

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boshoff, C., Grobler, H., & Nienaber, A. (2015). The evaluation of an equine-assisted therapy programme with a group of boys in a youth care facility. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 25(1), 8690. https://doi.org/10.1080/14330237.2015.1007611

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77101. https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Burgon, H. L. (2011). Queen of the world: Experiences of ‘at-risk’ young people participating in equine-assisted learning/therapy. Journal of Social Work Practice, 25(02), 165183. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650533.2011.561304

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chandler, C. K. (2005). Animal assisted therapy in counselling. Routledge.

  • Chardonnens, E. (2009). The use of animals as co-therapists on a farm: The child – horse bond in person-centered equine-assisted psychotherapy. Person-Centered & Experiential Psychotherapies, 8(4), 319332. https://doi.org/10.1080/14779757.2009.9688496

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cluver, L., Gardner, F., & Operario, D. (2009). Poverty and psychological health among aids-orphaned children in Cape Town, South Africa. AIDS Care, 21(6), 732741. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540120802511885

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coetzee, N., Beukes, J. T., & Lynch, I. (2013). Substance abuse inpatients’ experience of animal-assisted therapy. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 23(3), 477480. https://doi.org/10.1080/14330237.2013.10820654

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coleman, V. (1999). Animal rights, human wrongs: A blueprint for a better society. Blue Books.

  • Creswell, J. W. (2013). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches. Sage.

  • Cross, C. (2014). Qualitative assessment of the Diepsloot economy. In S. Mahajan (Ed.), Economics of South African townships: Special focus on Diepsloot. World Bank Studies (pp. 142178). World Bank.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dillman, D. (1999). Kids and critters: An intervention to violence. In F. Ascione & P. Arkow (Eds.), Child abuse, domestic violence & animal abuse: Linking the circles of compassion for prevention and intervention (pp. 424432). Purdue University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Equine Assisted Growth and Learning Association [Eagala]. (2019). Fundamentals of Eagala model: Becoming an Eagala certified professional (9th ed.). Equine Assisted Growth and Learning Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fabry, J. (1988). Guideposts to meaning, discovering what really matters. New Harbinger Publications.

  • Fernandes, H. M., Vasconcelos-Raposo, J., & Teixeira, C. M. (2010). Preliminary analysis of the psychometric properties of Ryff’s scales of psychological wellbeing in Portuguese adolescents. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13(2), 10321043. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600002675

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. Sage.

  • Fine, A. H. (Ed.). (2015). Handbook on animal-assisted therapy: Foundations and guidelines for animal-assisted interventions. Academic Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fournier, A., Letson, E., Berry, T., & Pasiuk, E. (2018). Human-animal interaction and metaphor in equine-assisted psychotherapy: Empirical support for the Eagala model. Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin, 6(1), 4763. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2016.1181372

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frankl, V. E. (1988). Man’s search for meaning. Penguin Group.

  • Gao, J., & Mclellan, R. (2018). Using Ryff’s scales of psychological wellbeing in adolescents in mainland China. Biomed Central Psychology, 6(17), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-018-0231-6

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Garcia, D., Al Nima, A., & Kjell, O. N. E. (2014). The affective profiles, psychological wellbeing, and harmony: Environmental mastery and self-acceptance predict the sense of a harmonious life. PeerJ, 2, e259. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.259

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Garcia, D. M. (2014). The paradox of otherness in horse-human connections: A Peircean interpretation [Poster presentation]. Semiotic Society of America 39th Annual Meeting, Seattle, WA.

  • Hill, N.R. (2007). Wilderness therapy as a treatment modality for at-risk youth: A primer for mental health counselors. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 29(4), 338349. https://doi.org/10.17744/mehc.29.4.c6121j162j143178

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hoagwood, K. E., Acri, M., Morrissey, M., & Peth-Pierce, R. (2016). Animal-assisted therapies for youth with or at risk for mental health problems: A systematic review. Applied Developmental Science, 21(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2015.1134267

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Joffe, H. (2012). Thematic analysis. In D. Harper & A. Thompson (Eds.), Qualitative research methods in mental health and psychotherapy: A guide for students and practitioners (pp. 209223). Wiley-Blackwell.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kemp, K., Signal, T., Botros, H., Taylor, N., & Prentice, K. (2014). Equine facilitated therapy with children and adolescents who have been sexually abused: A program evaluation study. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23(3), 558566. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9718-1

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kendall, E., Maujean, A., Pepping, C. A., Downes, M., Lakhani, A., Byrne, J., & Macfarlane, K. (2015). A systematic review of the efficacy of equine-assisted interventions on psychological outcomes. European Journal of Psychotherapy & Counselling, 17(1), 5779. https://doi.org/10.1080/13642537.2014.996169

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keyes, C. L. (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: A complementary strategy for improving national mental health. American Psychologist, 62(2), 95108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.95

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kruger, K. A., & Serpell, J. A. (2010). Animal-assisted interventions in mental health: Definitions and theoretical foundations. In A. Fine (Ed.), Handbook on animal-assisted therapy: Theoretical foundations and guidelines for practice (Vol. 3, pp. 3348). Academic Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lentini, J. A., & Knox, M. (2009). A qualitative and quantitative review of equine facilitated psychotherapy (EFP) with children and adolescents. The Open Complementary Medicine Journal, 1(1), 5157.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lerner, R. M., & Galambos, N. L. (1998). Adolescent development: Challenges and opportunities for research, programs and policies. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 413446. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.413

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luby, J., Belden, A., Botteron, K., Marrus, N., Harms, M., Babb, C., Nishino, T., & Barch, D. (2013). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development. The mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. Journal of American Medical Association Paediatrics, 167(12), 11351142. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mahajan, S. (2014). Economics of South African townships: Special focus on Diepsloot. https://doi.org/10.1596/978-1-4648-0301-7

  • Makwela, M. (2015). Diepsloot. Diepsloot Online. Retrieved from http://www.diepsloot.com/

  • Overton, W. F. (2010). Life-span development: Concepts and issues. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), Cognition, biology, and methods across the lifespan (Vol. 1, pp. 129). Wiley.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pallant, J. (2016). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS (6th ed.). Open University Press.

  • Pratt, M. G., & Dirks, K. T. (2007). Rebuilding trust and restoring positive relationships: A commitment-based view of trust. In J. E. Dutton & B. R. Ragins (Eds.), Lea’s organization and management series. Exploring positive relationships at work: Building a theoretical and research foundation (pp. 117136). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ratele, K. (2007). Poverty and inequality. In M. Moleko & A. Visser (Eds.), Community psychology in South Africa (2nd ed., pp. 267284). Van Schaik Publishers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rathi, N., & Rastogi, R. (2007). Meaning in life and psychological wellbeing in pre-adolescents and adolescents. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 33(1), 3138.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reddy, S. P., James, S., Sewpaul, R., Koopman, F., Funani, N. I., Sifunda, S., Josie, J., Masuka, P., Kambaran, N.S., & Omardien, R. G. (2010). Umthente uhlaba usamila: The South African youth risk behaviour survey 2008. South African Medical Research Council.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roseberry, K. B., & Rovin, L. M. (1999). Animal-assisted therapy for sexually abused adolescent females. In F. Ascione & P. Arkow (Eds.), Child abuse, domestic violence & animal abuse: Linking the circles of compassion for prevention and intervention (pp. 433442). Purdue University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological wellbeing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 10691081. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological wellbeing in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(4), 99104. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772395

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar