In two behavioral experiments, we explored effects of long-term musical training on the implicit processing of temporal structures (rhythm, non-rhythm and meter), manipulating deviance detection under different conditions. We used a task that did not require an explicit processing of the temporal aspect of stimuli, as this was irrelevant for the task. In Experiment 1, we investigated whether long-term musical training results in a superior processing of auditory rhythm, and thus boosts the detection of auditory deviants inserted within rhythmic compared to non-rhythmic auditory series. In Experiment 2, we focused on the influence of the metrical positions of a rhythmic series, and we compared musicians and non-musicians’ responses to deviant sounds inserted on strong versus weak metrical positions. We hypothesized that musicians would show enhanced rhythmic processing as compared to non-musicians. Furthermore, we hypothesized that musicians’ expectancy level would differ more across metrical positions compared to non-musicians. In both experiments, musicians were faster and more sensitive than non-musicians. Although both groups were overall faster and showed a higher sensitivity for the detection of deviants in rhythmic compared to non-rhythmic series (Experiment 1), only musicians were faster in the detection of deviants on strong positions compared to weak ones (Experiment 2). While rhythm modulates deviance processing also in non-musicians, specific effects of long-term musical training arise when a refined comparison of hierarchical metrical positions is considered. This suggests that long-term musical training enhances sensitivity to the metrical structure and improves temporal prediction mechanisms, even during implicit processing of meter.
Purchase
Buy instant access (PDF download and unlimited online access):
Institutional Login
Log in with Open Athens, Shibboleth, or your institutional credentials
Personal login
Log in with your brill.com account
Bendixen A. (2014). Predictability effects in auditory scene analysis: A review , Front. Neurosci. 8, 60. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00060.
Besson M. , Faïta F. , & Requin J. (1994). Brain waves associated with musical incongruities differ for musicians and non-musicians , Neurosci. Lett. 168, 101–105.
Bolton T. L. (1894). Rhythm , Am. J. Psychol. 6, 145–238.
Boot W. R. , Kramer A. F. , Simons D. J. , Fabiani M. , & Gratton G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control , Acta Psychol. 129, 387–398.
Boot W. R. , Blakely D. P. , & Simons D. J. (2011). Do action video games improve perception and cognition? Front. Psychol. 2, 226. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00226.
Bouwer F. L. , & Honing H. (2015). Temporal attending and prediction influence the perception of metrical rhythm: Evidence from reaction times and ERPs, Front. Psychol. 6, 1094. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01094.
Bouwer F. L. , Werner C. M. , Knetemann M. , & Honing H. (2016). Disentangling beat perception from sequential learning and examining the influence of attention and musical abilities on ERP responses to rhythm. Neuropsychologia 85, 80–90.
Brochard R. , Dufour A. , & Despres O. (2004). Effect of musical expertise on visuospatial abilities: Evidence from reaction times and mental imagery , Brain Cogn. 54, 103–109.
Cook P. , Rouse A. , Wilson M. , & Reichmuth C. (2013). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep the beat: Motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non vocal mimic , J. Comp. Psychol. 127(4), 412–427.
Elliott M. T. , Wing A. M. , & Welchman A. E. (2014). Moving in time: Bayesian causal inference explains movement coordination to auditory beats , Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 281(1786), 20140751. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0751.
Fitch W. (2013). Rhythmic cognition in humans and animals: distinguishing meter and pulse perception , Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7, 68. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00068.
Friston K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses , Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360(1456), 815–836.
Geiser E. , Ziegler E. , Jancke L. , & Meyer M. (2009). Early electrophysiological correlates of meter and rhythm processing in music perception, Cortex 45, 93–102.
Geiser E. , Sandmann P. , Jäncke L. , & Meyer M. (2010). Refinement of metre perception — Training increases hierarchical metre processing , Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 1979–1985.
Grahn J. A. , & Rowe J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: Premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception , J. Neurosci. 29, 7540–7548.
Herholz S. C. , & Zatorre R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: behavior, function, and structure, Neuron 76, 486–502.
Honing H. (2012). Without it no music: Beat induction as a fundamental musical trait , Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1252, 85–91.
Jones M. R. , & Boltz M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time , Psychol. Rev. 96, 459–491.
Jones M. R. , Moynihan H. , MacKenzie N. , & Puente J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays , Psychol. Sci. 13, 313–319.
Jongsma M. L. , Desain P. , Honing H. , & van Rijn C. M. (2003). Evoked potentials to test rhythm perception theories , Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 180–183.
Jongsma M. L. , Desain P. , & Honing H. (2004). Rhythmic context influences the auditory evoked potentials of musicians and nonmusicians , Biol. Psychol. 66, 129–152.
Klein C. , Liem F. , Hänggi J. , Elmer S. , & Jäncke L. (2016). The ‘silent’ imprint of musical training , Hum. brain Mapp. 37, 536–546.
Large E. W. (2000). On synchronizing movements to music , Hum. Mov. Sci. 19, 527–566.
Large E. W. , & Jones M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events , Psychol. Rev. 106, 119–159.
Large E. W. , & Snyder J. S. (2009). Pulse and meter as neural resonance , Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 46–57.
Lee H. , & Noppeney U. (2014). Music expertise shapes audiovisual temporal integration windows for speech, sinewave speech, and music, Front Psychol. 5, 868. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00868.
Lerdahl F. , & Jackendoff R. (1983). An overview of hierarchical structure in music , Music Percept. 1, 229–252.
London J. (2012). Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Luce R. D. (1986). Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization, Oxford Psychology Series, No. 8. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press on Demand.
Martin J. G. (1972). Rhythmic (hierarchical) versus serial structure in speech and other behavior, Psychol. Rev. 79, 487–509.
Münte T. F. , Altenmüller E. , & Jäncke L. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity , Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 473–478.
Ono K. , Altmann C. F. , Matsuhashi M. , Mima T. , & Fukuyama H. (2015). Neural correlates of perceptual grouping effects in the processing of sound omission by musicians and nonmusicians , Hear. Res. 319, 25–31.
Parncutt R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms , Music Percept. 11, 409–464.
Patel A. D. , Iversen J. R. , Bregman M. R. , & Schulz I. (2009). Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal , Curr. Biol. 19, 827–830.
Rohrmeier M. A. , & Koelsch S. (2012). Predictive information processing in music cognition. A critical review , Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 164–175.
Schwartze M. , Rothermich K. , Schmidt-Kassow M. , & Kotz S. A. (2011). Temporal regularity effects on pre-attentive and attentive processing of deviance , Biol. Psychol. 87, 146–151.
Stanislaw H. , & Todorov N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures, Behav. Res. Meth. Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149.
Thaut M. H. , Trimarchi P. D. , & Parsons L. M. (2014). Human brain basis of musical rhythm perception: Common and distinct neural substrates for meter, tempo, and pattern, Brain Sci. 4, 428–452.
Vuust P. , & Witek M. A. (2014). Rhythmic complexity and predictive coding: A novel approach to modeling rhythm and meter perception in music, Front. Psychol. 5, 1111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01111.
Vuust P. , Pallesen K. J. , Bailey C. , van Zuijen T. L. , Gjedde A. , Roepstorff A. , & Østergaard L. (2005). To musicians, the message is in the meter: Pre-attentive neuronal responses to incongruent rhythm are left-lateralized in musicians, Neuroimage 24, 560–564.
Vuust P. , Ostergaard L. , Pallesen K. J. , Bailey C. , & Roepstorff A. (2009). Predictive coding of music–brain responses to rhythmic incongruity, Cortex 45, 80–92.
Wallentin M. , Nielsen A. H. , Friis-Olivarius M. , Vuust C. , & Vuust P. (2010). The Musical Ear Test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 188–196.
Wallin N. L. , & Merker B. (2001). The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press.
Winkler I. , Háden G. P. , Ladinig O. , Sziller I. , & Honing H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music, Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 106, 2468–2471.
Woodrow H. (1909). A Quantitative Study of Rhythm: The Effect of Variations in Intensity, Rate and Duration, Archives of Psychology No. 14. New York, NY, USA: Science Press.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 243 | 39 | 0 |
Full Text Views | 191 | 1 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 20 | 4 | 0 |
In two behavioral experiments, we explored effects of long-term musical training on the implicit processing of temporal structures (rhythm, non-rhythm and meter), manipulating deviance detection under different conditions. We used a task that did not require an explicit processing of the temporal aspect of stimuli, as this was irrelevant for the task. In Experiment 1, we investigated whether long-term musical training results in a superior processing of auditory rhythm, and thus boosts the detection of auditory deviants inserted within rhythmic compared to non-rhythmic auditory series. In Experiment 2, we focused on the influence of the metrical positions of a rhythmic series, and we compared musicians and non-musicians’ responses to deviant sounds inserted on strong versus weak metrical positions. We hypothesized that musicians would show enhanced rhythmic processing as compared to non-musicians. Furthermore, we hypothesized that musicians’ expectancy level would differ more across metrical positions compared to non-musicians. In both experiments, musicians were faster and more sensitive than non-musicians. Although both groups were overall faster and showed a higher sensitivity for the detection of deviants in rhythmic compared to non-rhythmic series (Experiment 1), only musicians were faster in the detection of deviants on strong positions compared to weak ones (Experiment 2). While rhythm modulates deviance processing also in non-musicians, specific effects of long-term musical training arise when a refined comparison of hierarchical metrical positions is considered. This suggests that long-term musical training enhances sensitivity to the metrical structure and improves temporal prediction mechanisms, even during implicit processing of meter.
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 243 | 39 | 0 |
Full Text Views | 191 | 1 | 0 |
PDF Views & Downloads | 20 | 4 | 0 |